Wednesday, December 7, 2011

Wednesday Search Challenge (12/7/11): Who was she and how much did she get??

Regular readers of SearchResearch know that we are sober, quiet, and interested in the deeper uses of online research.  One typically doesn't find much that's salacious here.  


But for today's Search Challenge we'll make an exception.  This search is about gossip and somewhat tawdry rumors that we want to run down.  The reason this is interesting to readers today is that it involves Mark Twain and a bit of 19th century scandal mongering. 


As you probably know, Mark Twain suffered from a bit of a financial setback in the middle of his life.  This led to his regular touring shows where he would lecture and generally wax wise about the state of the world.  
Today's search challenge is this: 


Background:  Mark Twain lost much of his money by investing heavily in the development of a very complex device.  The developer was a bit of a scoundrel who not only lost Twain's investment, but was caught up in a series of accusations and counter-accusations with a noted woman.  


Question:  Who was the developer; who was the woman, and ultimately, how much did the developer pay to her to resolve the accusations?  




Image from USPTO of the invention that led to financial setbacks for Twain.  

Friday, December 2, 2011

Did you know.. Weather in Google Maps?

I think I'm going to start a new header in my blog posts  "Did you know..."


Even though I work at Google, I'm still sometimes surprised by features that launch that somehow slipped by me.  


Here's one of them:  You can now turn on the "Weather Layer" in Google Maps.  Just go to the Satellite / Map icon in the upper right corner of the map, roll your mouse over it and a popup layer will appear.  Select "Weather." 
When you do, a bunch of weather icons (current weather) will appear as a layer on the map. Here's one for the current weather on the Big Island of Hawai'i.  



And here, the nice little demo video the Maps people made.  





Glad I finally noticed! 


Search on! 



Thursday, December 1, 2011

Answer: Antarctic Islands and India?

Here's the short answer: 


WHO:  Yves-Joseph de Kerguelen de Trémarec (in 1772) 


WHAT:  Kerguelen Islands, deep in the southern Indian Ocean


WHAT IS CONNECTION WITH INDIA:  Kerguelen's rocks are very similar to ones found in northeast India near the Rajmahal Traps  (those hills I mentioned).  The traps were formed by a geologic hotspot that initially formed in Gondwana.  As the plates shifted over time, the hotspot emitted a series of eruptions that left underwater ridges and plateaus over time.  As the surface shifted, this hotspot gave birth to both the Rajmahal Traps, underwater features like the Ninety East Ridge and the Kerguelen Islands.  


Now, the longer answer... How did I search this one out? 


When I first heard about this problem, all I knew was that the discoverer was 18th century French.  A few searches like [ French privateer island discovery ] I finally found that it was the Kerguelen Islands (after a few false starts--there were, after all, other islands discovered by the French--Réunion, Mayotte, etc.).  


When trying to find out what the connection was between Kerguelen and India I read an article about the Kerguelen Plateau.  That's the microcontinent ("once covered in conifers") that's now underwater 1 - 2 km underwater.  That got me to thinking about possible geological connections between the plateau and the striking ridges in the Indian ocean.  


From there it was a quick search for [ Kerguelen Plateau ] which led me to the Wikipedia article about the plateau.  A bit of reading about the Kerguelen hotspot, a quick finding that the Kerguelen hotspot has produced basaltic lava for about 130 million years and has also produced the Kerguelen IslandsHeard Island, the McDonald Islands, and the Ninetyeast Ridge.  The very last line of that article suggests a connection with India. 


Following a search for [ Kerguelen India hotspot ] leads pretty quickly to a nice article at PhysOrg.com detailing the connection between the two.  Here's my simplified diagram:  
As the plates move around above the hotspot, a line
of volcanoes and basaltic flood plains (what become
plateaus) forms in a line above. 

And as reader Hans pointed out,this is wonderfully documented in the article "Ar(40)/Ar(39) Geochronology of the Rajmahal Basalts, India, and Their Relationship to the Kerguelen Plateau" (J. Petrology, v43, n 4, p 1141-1153, 2002)





Bathymetric map of Indian Ocean (from  SRTM30_plus via Wikipedia).
Note the striking ridge (Ninety East Ridge) pointing nearly due
north from Antarctica to the Bay of Bengal. 



Postscript: I think I the challenge was harder for me than for you!  When I started, I didn't even know where the islands were!  Just figuring out that they were in the Indian Ocean took me 15 minutes.  So when I wrote this question, I gave several people a head start--they cleverly shortcut the whole search problem with a query of  Antarctic Islands India ] which gets you to the Keguelen Islands pretty easily.  Well, as I say, it's an open-internet search challenge. 


But in the next few weeks we'll make the problems a little more difficult--until we're ALL super searchers! 


Search on! 

Wednesday, November 30, 2011

Wednesday Search Challenge (Nov 30, 2011): Antarctic islands and India?


Here’s a tale that I’ve been trying to track down.  Put on your best discoverer’s hat and see if you can figure this one out as well.

I was told that not long after the Seven Years War, a Breton privateer discovered islands near Antarctica that he believed were the headlands of a new and previously undiscovered continent.  That didn’t pan out, but he did find islands  that are somehow related to an entire submerged small continent that was formerly covered in pine trees.  Strangely enough, that watery sub-continent is in turn somehow connected to the large hills near the city of Rajmahal in India.




WHO found WHAT islands, and WHAT IS their connection with India?  

You don't need any particularly tricky search techniques for this, but you do need to be willing to look around and find connections that you might not have thought about.  

Search on! 

Monday, November 28, 2011

Doodles, cool and archived


I saw a really surprising search the other day.  I was watching a school-age kid trying to find one of Google's clever interactive logos. You know, like the one from today celebrating Bob Noyce's 84th birthday.  


You've probably noticed--they're sometimes interactive, they're always celebratory in some way and they have a popup explaining what it's all about.  (I'm showing the popup on the right-hand side of the above image.)  


What you might not know is that if you click on the image on the Google Home Page, it will do an appropriate search for you on that topic.  


What was so interesting about watching this kid search for the Google Archive was that he didn't know they're called "Doodles," so he was fooling around searching with queries like [Google images ] (which won't get you very far because the results are so heavily skewed by Google Images, the search verical).  He tried things like [cool Google images ] before I finally told him to try adding the word "doodle," which of course got him what he wanted.  


Search lesson:  Sometimes you just need the right word to access the right body of information.  The deep trick is knowing which is the right word. 


In this case, the kid might have tried to search for [Google images ] on Images and he would have found all kinds of results, including links to the Google Official Blog, which then has links to the archived Doodles.  


To save YOU the trouble, here are a few links to Google's collection of great Doodles.  


The official Google Doodle archive


The marvelous Doodle ode to Stanislaw Lem 


The Les Paul interactive Doodle (play your own tunes!)


And last, but not least, the video of the famous Charlie Chaplin Doodle.   


Search on!



Changes... always changes to Google products


You probably saw the recent Google blog post about sunsetting different products.  http://googleblog.blogspot.com/2011/11/more-spring-cleaning-out-of-season.html


Here it is in short:   (Remember, this is my personal blog and doesn't represent Google official thoughts, policy or inclinations...) 


1.  Google Bookmark Lists (Dec 19, 2011) -- essentially nobody was using them (I mean, not even was using them, so you know it didn't have much uptake).  Fatal flaw--what user problem was it solving?  Nobody really knows, so it's going away in December.  


2.  Google Friend Connect (Mar 21, 2012) -- being superceded by Google+ features.  


3.  Google Gears (Dec 1, 2011) -- it was a valiant attempt to make Google products work offline as well as online.  But with the advent of HTML 5 (and the various offline features it offers), Gears is rapidly becoming redundant.


4. Search Timeline (Oct, 2011) -- this is unfortunate, as there really isn't anything else quite like it. Yes, you can use Google Insights for some of this function, but the ability to do Timelines over News Archives is just gone.  I'm hunting around for a good replacement for the ability to do this kind of search + charting.  


5. Google Wave (Mar 30, 2012) -- I can't say that I'm sorry about this one being turned down.  In my use of Wave, it was just a giant, unwieldy thing.  Nice idea... but it was too much all in one package.  I want a speedboat, but got a cruise ship.  


6. Google Knol (Oct 1, 2012) -- This too was a great idea--authored Wikipedia style articles.  But it never took off in the way that Wikipedia has, partly because the articles never got enough links to make them show up high in the results.  Sigh.  Unfortunately, Wikipedia is suffering its own set of difficulties.  (Have you tried to add a new article to Wikipedia recently?  See Danny Sullivan's recent rant about this.  He's totally right.  His critique shows the growing problems that Wikipedia is having, and worries me about its future.)  


Moral of this particular story:  As I've been saying for a while, things come, things go.  The good news here is that Google is getting better about letting everyone know about these changes.  You can see the culture change over time.  Used to be that changes just happened without any kind of comment.  People noticed, or they didn't.  


Another kind of change that went unremarked (but I'll tell you) was that there were recently a bunch of changes to the online Dictionary.  When you do the [define: ] operation (such as [define:peruse]) the quality has gone up, primarily in marking typical uses.  While keeping up with shifting definitions is a full-time job, the fact that Google is paying attention to the quality is a great thing.  


But it's one of those things that isn't, from Google's point of view, worth mentioning.  It's just one of the continuous changes that tries to make everything better.  Of course, not every change is 100% improvement for everyone, but the goal (and I think Google pretty much succeeds at this) is to make the majority of people happier and have access to higher quality information.  (For even more information, see Matt Cutts recent post about recent ranking changes or the YouTube video about ranking changes below.)  


If you see something that is clearly worse for you (or better yet, worse for a large number of people), let me know.  The least I can do is to pass your observations along. 


Search on! 






Friday, November 25, 2011

Memories of green lines on the land



There are lines on the landscape that you can’t see easily, lines that mark the memories of stream pathways and old property boundaries, leaving a persistent impression of the past on the surface of today.

We’ve all seen lines of trees before.  Depending on the place, straight tree lines are usually old property boundaries, with the trees marking off the edges of ephemeral property ownership that changes at decade speed, while the trees change at century speed, out of sync with the short / fast / furious lives of those that would use them to mark a bounding box around what they think they temporarily own.   In California there’s been a long-standing tradition of using eucalyptus trees to mark off edges, even though the trees remain long after the human claimants have been returned to the soil as nutrients.   

What’s even more remarkable is that these green lines can be seen through Google Maps.  The aerial images (“Satellite view,” even though many of the images at the scale we’re interested in are actually taken from planes) often reveal the past of a place through the green lines. 
Click on image to see at full size.   

In the above image you can see a long line of trees just below the lake on the left.  That's Felt Lake in the Stanford University foothills.  But those are 60 foot tall eucalyptus trees that used to mark the southern end of the old Stanford farm.  You can pretty clearly see what the boundary was.  That line of housing development crowds right up next to the property line, making a nearly vertical slash through the hills.  

But I’m really more interested in the ways memories of streams persist in the landscape.  Here’s an aerial image of some land in Palo Alto taken in 1948.  The sinuous line of trees running through the photo is Matadero Creek.  The straight line running almost vertically is a railroad track, and you can see that the line of trees following the creek stops at a major highway—in this case it’s El Camino Real, an old road that has been in that location for hundreds of years. 


 From 1948... I pieced these together from Google Earth images in History view mode.   

What captivates me about this is that the tree lines persist for a very long time, even through 50 years of intense development.  As you can see, the line of trees along the creekbed have survived, in a sense, for half a century.  The creek is in a very different setting now; once it enters the industrial park, the stream is completely driven by runoff from acres of asphalt, and no longer fed by trickles from grassy fields. 


This is a recent image.  I’ve highlighted in red the creek once it enters the Stanford industrial park  and flows into suburbia.  It’s really broken only in two places—first when it crosses Foothill Blvd (just before entering the industrial park), and then just a bit later when it was channelized to go beneath the Tibco building (the straight segment in the middle of the industrial park). 

Seeing these images makes me think of how much of a lasting impact we have on the world.  Developers channelize creekbeds, but the lines still persist.  It’s hard to undo that much geography, it’s just incumbent unless you move the entire landform. 

These lines of trees (oaks, mostly) explain something you see from time to time walking through suburbia.  Every so often, there’s a tree that is way out-of-size, something that just seems incongruous with its surround.  If it’s an oak, chances are good that it’s part of a much larger structure—a persisting line of trees that remembers a world before the developers came.

I find this both heartening and sad at the same time.  I feel lucky to have come along at a point in time when one can still see these marks on the land.  At the same time, it’s a bit like finding a palm tree deep in a scrub oak woodland—the marks of people on the land are inescapable, and often woefully out-of-place.  

We cover, we bury, we replant… and still the land manages to persist.  The big oak in the suburbs is an oddity, way out of keeping with the other trees in the area, until you realize that it’s been there for 200 years.  Ultimately, that makes me glad.  I hope the trees win in the end.  

And feeling lucky enough to live in a time when I can see both the trees from the ground and from the air through Google Maps and Google Earth. 

Searching for more trees and more green lines on the land... 

Thursday, November 24, 2011

Answer: What's that flower?

The quick answer is that it's Ithuriel's Spear, aka Wally Basket, aka Grass Nut.


Latin name: Triteleia laxa.

Regular reader Hans nicely provided a link to an illustration of Ithuriel and the spear he used to reveal that the frog he's threatening was actually the devil in disguise.  (Who knew?) 

To solve this challenge... I first looked up where I (were the flower) was.  If you enter the lat/long into Google Maps, 37.1540, -121.4200, you'll find that you're in the middle of Henry Coe State Park. 

Why does this matter?  Because when locating things like wildflowers, birds, animals, insects (etc.) the geo-location matters a great deal.  There are many thousands of blue wildflowers in the world, but your first big clue in figuring out which one it might be is location.  

Once I knew that this flower was in a State Park, I figured that this might be useful in doing my search.  So I did: 


I could have used a search term like "northern california," but I went with Henry Coe because I know that state parks often have volunteer organizations that publish things like collections-of-wildflower-photos.

Once I did that search, I saw that one of the top hits was "Album of Blue Wildflowers from Henry Coe State Park."  

Ah ha!  Since I had a couple of good pictures (see yesterday's challenge page), it was pretty easy to compare with each of the photos in that album.  And, sure enough, the 13th image down is a match.  

But matching flowers based on just an image can be tricky, so I copied the Latin name (Triteleia laxa) and did another search on just that.  


which led me to the Wikipedia page for Triteleia laxa (which wasn't all that helpful, but did give me another image to compare).  It ALSO led me to the Calflora site entry for Tritelia laxa, which IS a great, very authoritative site.  (Why is it authoritative?  Because it links to very well-known herbarium sites such as the Jepson Manual and the USDA plant manual.)  

That pretty much confirmed it for me, but to be triply sure, I ALSO checked the Jepson Manual entry for Triteleia laxa... which was consistent with my field observations about number-of-stames, descriptions of anthers, flower composition, etc.  

A quick search for [ Ithuriel's Spear Milton ] confirmed that the story of Ithuriel's spear and the devil-disguised-as-frog appear in Book IV of Paradise Lost by the 17th century English poet John Milton.  

Search moral:  There are a couple of takeaways.  
1.  When searching for animals, plants, and similar localized things, consider starting your search with a geo-reference.  In this case I used "Henry Coe State Park" -- I could have used something else (even California as a search term would have helped.)  
2.  When looking for a specific flower, you probably need to find images to verify your observation.  If you REALLY want to get into the details, you're going to need to use a wildflower identification key to verify that the flower you're looking at really is what you think it is.  Note that most keys are region specific--be sure the key you're using is for the location the flower is in!  

Search on! 

Wednesday, November 23, 2011

Wednesday Search Challenge: What's that flower? (Nov 23, 2011)



You've probably asked a question like this before:  What's that flower?  


Unless you're botanically inclined, or have a friend who's horticulturally minded, it's often kind of hard to figure these things out... especially for wildflowers that are unrelated to anything you'd see in a domesticated garden.


As you might guess, this happens to me all the time.  

This past March I was out for a leisurely hike at lat/long 37.1540, -121.4200 when I noticed a pretty blue flower.   Here are a couple of pictures so you can get an idea of what I saw.  
It was about 3 feet high and was growing in open grassland--nothing especially odd or strange about where it was growing.  Just along the trail, sticking it's head up 


When I got home I looked it up based on what I saw. 


To make your task slightly easier, I'll tell you that the flowers were blue, but that there were some variation in flower color between the plants.  They were blue, blue-purple, or white.  The flower tube was around 12–25 mm long, with the petals ranging from 8–20 mm in length.  The flowers are cone-shaped and are made up of 6 petals that are fused together at the bottom.  In the flower there are 6 stamens, 3-lobed stigma atop a single triple-chambered ovary.  In the flower,  filaments attached at 2 levels, and the anthers ranged from 2–5 mm.  As you can see, individual flowers were arranged in clusters of 4 - 8 flowers at the end of longish stalk, roughly 100 mm long.   The leaves are long and grass-like in appearance.  


When I figured out what it was, I was surprised to discover was that its common name is the name of a weapon that's used by a particular angel!  (Do angels normally carry weapons??)  


Question:  Can you figure out what the Latin and common names of this flower are?  


For extra credit on this one--be sure to write down HOW you figured this one out, and how long it took you.  I'm curious to see if you find this one hard or very simple.  Let me know! 

Search on! 


Tuesday, November 22, 2011

Intuition and counterintuition


Intuition:  What’s the deal with intuition these days?  It seems to be on everyone’s mind, a brainworm on the loose.  People are claiming that tablets like the iPad “tap into intuition,” that Steve Jobs had an “intuitive designer’s sense,” George Bush “trusted his gut feelings” about the presence of WMD in Iraq, Kim Kardashian decided that “intuition led me to divorce” after 72 days of wedded bliss,  and the Huffington Post writes “Science says to trust your gut.”  Intuition seems to be more valued than ever, although there seems to have been no recent upgrade in our collective intuitive skills.  Is intuition really just the flowering of some inner, secret power? 

By contrast, the current movie “Moneyball” is about the success of the Oakland A’s team of 2002, a team that was put together with guidance of  some clever statistical analysis by their general manager, a baseball quant-jock if there ever was one.   Even Roger Ebert says the film is about “the war between intuition and statistics.”  Is there really a war going on? 

In his book “Blink,” Malcolm Gladwell talks about intuition as “thin-slicing” experience based on training up your experience base after 10,000  hours of practice in a field.  If you have that much time-on-task, practicing and learning how to operate efficiently in a field, then you’re capable of rapidly assessing a situation based on few, rapidly scanned clues, and somehow coming up with a quick recognition of what’s going on. 

Here's the thing to know:  How much time have you spent doing search?  My best estimate is that I've done about 5,000 hours of search since I first started using Google in late 1998.  That is, I started practicing my search skills around 13 years (or 4748 days) ago.  

If I've done a bit over 1 hour of search / day since then (which seems reasonable), that means I've invested ~5,000 hours of practice.  While that's a lot of time, it's only half of the 10,000 hours that are usually needed for real expertise. 

System 1, System 2: Nobel Prize winner Daniel Kahneman points out that people have two different and parallel systems of thought when they confront  problems.  The cleverly named “System 1” is a fast recognizer of situations in context—it identifies and labels objects, picks up on relationships, and does so by recognition, rapidly, rather than by deliberation.  

Then there’s “System 2,” the slower, more deliberate, symbol-pushing and rational part of our minds. 

As an example, System 1 recognizes that the number sequence 2, 4, 8, 16, 32… is just doubling from one to the next.  It would be System 2 that lets you realize that this is also the powers of 2.  Of course, if you’re a computer scientist, the powers of 2 has become, over many repeated exposures, something that’s a System 1 effect.  For non-CS majors the problem 2 * 256 is a System 2 task.  For CS-majors, it’s a System 1 task—you recognize the pattern and say, “it’s 512…” without thinking much.  In this sense, intuition is what you’ve been trained to expect to perceive.  It is the power of repeated exposure and the accumulation of inarticulate recognition skills.  

About System 1 Kahneman writes in his book Thinking, Fast and Slow
“We are prone to think that the world is more regular and predictable than it really is, because our memory automatically and continuously maintains a story about what is going on, and because the rules of memory tend to make that story as coherent as possible and to suppress alternatives. Fast thinking is not prone to doubt.”

Nor is System 1 particularly good at noticing contradictions.  We swim in a sea of counterintuitions--things that seem to be intuitively correct, but are not.  We have difficulty seeing the sea we swim in simply because we swim in it all the time.  

Some examples:  
  • How is it that clouds made of water vapor yet can float mid-air?  (Water is awfully heavy.)  
  • The world is visibly and obviously flat—yet we now believe that it’s intuitively obvious that the world is round.  (Trust me, historically speaking, that wasn’t obvious at all!)  
  • When we’re on a merry-go-round, our intuition tells us that the force is outward—that centrifugal force is really trying to thrust us radially away from the center, not on a tangent along the direction of travel.  
  • Dense things are typically opaque, except for glass and water, which are “intuitively obvious” exceptions to the rule.  

Counterintuitive: Except that they’re not obvious, it’s just the pattern you’ve seen so often that System 1 doesn’t even pick up on the contradiction. 

Intuitive thinking is primarily what you’ve experienced and on patterns you pick up.  While it’s fast, it’s also errorful.  Dan Ariely has asked hundreds of Princeton undergraduates the following question:  “A bat and ball together cost  $1.10.  The bat costs $1 more than the ball.  How much is the ball?”  It’s a simple question, but around 50% of Princeton seniors get it wrong and say that the bat costs 0.10.  (That can’t make sense.  If the ball is 0.10, then “$1 more than the ball” means that the bat is $1.10 – adding the bat and ball prices together means the bat + ball are $1.20.) 

So, why do they get it wrong so often?  

Because they don’t check their work.  Why?  Because it’s a bit of a hassle, and the answer is so obvious and apparent (and the stakes are so low) that it’s not worth the effort.  This is characteristic of many intuitive answers—it’s so *obvious* that it’s not worth checking. 

Seems to me that there’s not really a contradiction between System1 (intuition) and System 2 (rational) modes of knowing.  Instead, one’s just faster than the other.  Both are useful, both are necessary.  But the great achievement of science has been to tell us that intuition always has to be checked.  In essence, science is the overturning of intuition by slow, painful, careful System 2 reasoning.  Clouds float because cloud-borne water droplets are really, really tiny and are kept aloft by random air movements.

A note from Dan’s System 2:  Cloud droplets average between 1 and 100 microns.  A typical droplet 20 microns in diameter is 4.2 picoliters in volume with a weight of 4.2 nanograms, falling at a terminal velocity of 0.02 mph.  Thus, an updraft at just over 0.02 mph will keep the cloud in the sky.  That’s not much updraft.  Or to look at it another way, if the cloud formed at 10,000 feet, at 0.02 mph it will take nearly 4 days to fall to the ground.

The trick is to know when to use one mode versus the other.  “Trust the force, Luke” Obi-Wan whispers into our ears.  But the same time, you can’t build a pan-galactic empire with massive infrastructures based on your intuitions about mechanical engineering, power grid design, or waste management systems. 

We know there are a number of rather rapid inferences that are System 1.  A while ago Bob Zajonc showed that determining preference is much faster than cognition… that is, it anticipates cognition.  You see this when making choices about which kind of thing you prefer over something else—be it mates, brands of peanut butter or random Chinese characters.  As he said, “Preferences need  no inferences.” It’s fast and gets there before you’ve figured out what you really should want.   That’s what System 2 reasoning is for. 

It strikes me that we have a lot of brainware dedicated to specific fast System 1 type tasks, and even those mechanisms get it wrong much of the time.  We’ve all seen various visual illusions that we KNOW can’t be true—a vertical table that looks much longer than a horizontal table; two patches of color that look very different in a scene, but can be easily shown to be the same color when placed side-by-side. 

The strange thing is that visual illusions persist, even when you know they’re wrong; even when you’ve measure the differences; even when you put the color patches side by side.  Even though you do the measurements yourself, you can’t convince your vision system to accept your measurement as truth.  It’s as though you haven’t learned anything in the last minute of experience.  

(In the example below, squares A and B are the same color.  Use an eyedropper tool to measure the colors and find they're the same. This illusion image is from Wikipedia,  created by Edward H. Adelson, Professor of Vision Science at MIT in 1995.)  



Even though we DO more vision than anything else we do with our brains,  we still have predictable, repeatable mistakes in vision.  How can we be better in domains where we don’t have so much hardwired brain? 

This is the realm of the cognitive illusion, and is even more problematic.  We know that if you set up a program with an opt-out vs. an opt-in choice, the differences in selection rate are huge.  In one European study, the rate of people selecting to be in a organ donation program was massively different: in Sweden the participation rate is 86%, while in Denmark the participation rate is only 4%.  The difference is that one is opt-in while the other is opt-out. 


Dan Ariely points out that when we’re filling out the form we 

“feel as though we’re making decisions… but the person who designs the form that is actually making the decision for you.  You like to think that the options don’t influence us, but that’s not true.”  

The illusion is that you have control over your destiny.  That is, you think your System 2 rational decision-making system will actually express your deeply-held, fundamental beliefs.  “I believe that organ donation is a social good, so I’ll participate.”  That’s the story you might tell yourself.  But statistically speaking, the choice is made for you when the form is designed as opt-out. 

We don’t recognize the illusions we all live with day-to-day, we’ve come to accept them as intuitive and obvious.  Normal is what you’ve grown up with, the non-intuitive, confusing, difficult  high-technology world is whatever was invented after you turned 8, and stopped seeing everything as pre-destined.  

Isn't that intuitive?