The Tangwang Language-An Interdisciplinary Case Study in Northwest China
…
12 pages
1 file
Sign up for access to the world's latest research
Abstract
AI
AI
The Tangwang language, first reported in the 1980s, is the focus of interdisciplinary research examining its linguistic features, including phonology, morphology, and syntax, within the context of historical and genetic influences in Northwest China. Through a combination of linguistic analysis and molecular anthropology, the study investigates language contact, admixture, and replacement dynamics, providing insights into the correlation between language and genetic history. The findings contribute to the understanding of language evolution in a complex socio-cultural landscape.
Related papers
Advances in the emergence of language, human cognition, and modern cultures, 2009
Can agent-based language evolution contribute to archeology? Luc Steels Index The contributions presented in this volume are intended to depict a panorama of the topics explored by various research teams in the framework of the OMLL programme, to summarize new relevant data and emerging theories, to provide an updated view of this interdisciplinary venture, and, when possible, to provide directions for future research. In their variety, the collected papers cover the original call for proposals and reflect the multiplicity of interests and research strategy used to tackle this complex issue. It is obvious that the papers included in this volume do not cover all aspects of research in the vast scientific area of the EUROCORES Programme "The Origin of Jean-Marie Hombert and Francesco d'Errico engraved or painted representations, personal ornaments, burial practices, musical traditions, anatomy and encephalization. The authors' argument contradicts the hypothesis of a symbolic revolution coinciding with the arrival of anatomically modern humans in Europe some 40,000 years ago and highlights inconsistencies in the anatomically-culturally modern equation and the potential contribution of anatomically "pre-modern" human populations to the emergence of these abilities. The so-called "Neolithic revolution" has had a major impact on population growth and human migrations. It involved domestication of plants and animals. In their article Tresset, Bollongino, Edwards, Hughes and Vigne examine the early diffusion of bovids in Europe as a trace of human migrations, contacts and exchanges. By investigating the localisation of wild ancestors and process of dissemination of different species they show that different processes took place. Sheep and goats had no wild ancestors in Europe, consequently domesticated sheep and goats can be considered as good tracers for the expansion of farming advance or at least of Neolithic influence. Cattle and pigs on the other hand had potential ancestors in Europe which means that independent local domestication or hybridization with animals imported from the Near East was possible. Genetic data indicate that domesticated sheep, goats and cattle were introduced in Europe with no interaction with local populations for goats and sheep or very little in the case of bovines. For pigs, the process was very different; a first introduction from the Near East was followed by a local domestication of the wild boar leading to the disappearance of the original Eastern lineages. Language and genes Genetic data from modern populations have been used to provide dates for the emergence of anatomically modern humans and to locate their continent of origin. They have also been used to infer information concerning more recent population movements. On the basis of linguistic data collected from modern languages it is possible to reconstruct earlier groupings of currently spoken languages into language families and thus infer population movements associated with these language groupings. In this volume, four geographical/linguistic areas are investigated: West-Central Africa, Northern Africa, Central Asia and the Himalayas. These studies illustrate the complexity of the relationship between genes and languages and clearly show that a better understanding of such historical scenarios will only be possible with the joint contribution of linguistic and genetic research. Van der Veen, Quintana-Murci and Comas focus on West-Central Africa and address issues related to the so-called "Bantu expansion" and more specifically to
International Journal of Eurasian Linguistics, 2019
Review of: Dan Xu & Hui Li (eds.), Languages and genes in Northwestern China and adjacent regions. Springer Nature.
2018
Demographic events often leave traces in languages and genes: this prompted Darwin’s prediction that the evolutionary tree of human populations would provide the best possible phylogeny of language relationships. We tested Darwin’s expectation through long-distance genome-language comparisons across Eurasia, relying on independently assessed quantitative tools on both sides. To do so, we had to resort to a linguistic method able to compare across different families, based on abstract syntactic characters, which proved more apt for long-term historical reconstruction than phonemic ones.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2007
Numerous studies indicate strong associations between languages and genes among human populations at the global scale, but all broader scale genetic and linguistic patterns must arise from processes originating at the community level. We examine linguistic and genetic variation in a contact zone on the eastern Indonesian island of Sumba, where Neolithic Austronesian farming communities settled and began interacting with aboriginal foraging societies Ϸ3,500 years ago. Phylogenetic reconstruction based on a 200-word Swadesh list sampled from 29 localities supports the hypothesis that Sumbanese languages derive from a single ancestral Austronesian language. However, the proportion of cognates (words with a common origin) traceable to Proto-Austronesian (PAn) varies among language subgroups distributed across the island. Interestingly, a positive correlation was found between the percentage of Y chromosome lineages that derive from Austronesian (as opposed to aboriginal) ancestors and the retention of PAn cognates. We also find a striking correlation between the percentage of PAn cognates and geographic distance from the site where many Sumbanese believe their ancestors arrived on the island. These language-gene-geography correlations, unprecedented at such a fine scale, imply that historical patterns of social interaction between expanding farmers and resident hunter-gatherers largely explain community-level language evolution on Sumba. We propose a model to explain linguistic and demographic coevolution at fine spatial and temporal scales.
Language Dynamics and Change, 2015
This paper examines aspects of how linguistics and human genetics can collaborate in the investigation of human prehistory. Matters that need more careful attention for linguistic-genetic correlations to have value are emphasized. Some ways to make collaboration between geneticists and linguists more productive are considered, while some misconceptions frequently encountered in work which correlates languages and genes are clarified. In particular, the questions posed by Comrie (2006) in his position paper on language and genes are addressed.
Diachronica, 2007
Reviewed by J. Marshall Unger (The Ohio State University) This collection of papers "arose out of a workshop on the phylogeny of East Asian languages, organized by Laurent Sagart and the much missed Stanley Starosta in Périgueux … 29-3 August 200" (xxi). It comprises a useful introduction by the editors, four contributions on archaeology, seven on linguistics, and six on genetics and physical anthropology. The focus is on languages usually classified as Sino-Tibetan, Hmong-Mien, Daic, Austro-Asiatic, and Austronesian and their speakers, past and present. The so-called Altaic languages, Korean, Japanese, Ainu, and isolates of China do not, unfortunately, figure in the linguistics chapters, but there is no shortage of material on high-level theories and macrophyla. The book will therefore be of interest not only to scholars specializing on East Asia and the Pacific but also to those concerned with theoretical aspects of the comparative method and the challenges of interdisciplinary inquiry in general.
Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages, 2003
Not to be reproduced in any form without written permission from the publisher.
Mémoires de la Société de Linguistique de Paris, 2017
When looking to language data as a source of information on human (pre)history, linguistic areas have long been the very poor relation of language families. Both within linguistics, and in conjunction with archaeology and genetics, far less attention has been paid to convergence areas than to diverging families. Yet human populations have inevitably interacted in complexes of both convergent and divergent processes. This holds in linguistics no less than in culture and genetics: witness Matisoff’s (1990: 113) “Sinosphere” vs. “Indosphere”, two contrasting areal convergence zones, but within the same diverging Tibeto-Burman family. This imbalance between families and areas distorts and diminishes what we can learn from comparative linguistics, both historical and typological. It also means that we have much to gain if we can rebalance, to look much more seriously at the real-world contexts through (pre)history in which linguistic areas arose. Archaeologists and geneticists, when faced with signals of convergence between human populations on the sociocultural and demographic levels, often still think only in terms of divergent families as the linguistic parallel — rather than the more natural fit with convergent areas, so little known outside linguistics. Linguistics, meanwhile, labours under its own misconceptions and outdated visions of other disciplines, in the balance between migratory and diffusionist interpretations of the human past. Explaining linguistic areas requires one to think in terms of demographic and socio-cultural processes radically different to those traditionally invoked to account for language family expansions. Or indeed, to rethink whether certain contexts and processes — trade, mobility, and so on — are good explanations for divergent families at all, when in fact they can be more plausible shapers of linguistic convergence areas instead. This contribution aims to set out some general first principles for a prehistory of language areas. These principles will be illustrated by cases drawn from a range of (pre)historic contexts from across the globe: Meso-America, the Andes and Amazonia, the Balkans, mainland south-east Asia, and the ‘Altaic’ zone of north-eastern Asia.
… of language: proceedings of the 6th …, 2006
In a study by Cavalli-Sforza et al. (1988), the spread of anatomically modern man was reconstructed on the basis of genetic and linguistic pieces of evidence: the main conclusion was that these two approaches reflect a common underlying history, the history of our past still frozen in the genes of modern populations. The expression `genetic history' was introduced (Piazza et al. 1988) to point out that if today we find many genes showing the same geographical patterns in terms of their frequencies, this may be due to the common history of our species. A deeper exploration of the whole problem can be found in Cavalli-Sforza et al. (1994). In the following, some specific cases of structural analogies between linguistic and genetic geographical patterns will be explored that supply further and more updated information. It is important to emphasize at the outset that evidence for coevolution of genes and languages in human populations does not suggest by itself that some genes of our species determine the way we speak; this coevolution may simply be due to a common mode of transmission and mutation of genetic and linguistic units of information and common constraints of demographic factors. 1. The Genetic Analysis of a Linguistic Isolate: The Basques The case of the Basques, a European population living in the area of the Pyrenees on the border of Spain and France who still speak a non-Indo-European language, is paradigmatic. What are the genetic relations between the Basques and their surrounding modern populations, all of whom are Indo-European speakers? Almost half a century ago it was suggested (Bosch-Gimpera 1943) that the Basques are the descendants of the populations who lived in Western Europe during the late Paleolithic period. Their withdrawal to the area of the Pyrenees, probably caused by different waves of invasion, left the Basques untouched by the Eastern European invasions of the Iron Age. In their study of the geographic distribution of Rh blood groups, Chalmers et al. (1948) pointed out that the Rh negative allele, which is found almost exclusively in Europe, has its highest frequency among the Basques. Chalmers et al. hypothesized that modern Basques may consist of a Palaeolithic population with an extremely high Rh negative frequency, who later mixed with people from the Mediterranean area. In more recent times genetic analyses have produced the following conclusions: (a) Mitochondrial and Y-chromosome DNA polymorphisms support the idea that the Basques are genetically different from the other modern European populations (Richards et al., 2000, 2002; Semino et al. 2000). (b) Mitochondrial and Y-chromosome DNA polymorphisms support the idea that the Basques are the descendants of a Palaeolithic population (Richards et al., 2000, 2002; Semino et al. 2000). The main haplogroups contributing to the European mitochondrial geography are H, pre-V, and U5. Haplogroup H is the most frequent haplogroup in both Europe and the Near East but occurs at frequencies of only 25% 30% in the Near East and the Caucasus, whereas the frequency is generally 50% in European populations and reaches a maximum of 60% in the Basque country. The age ranges of the mitochondrial founders of these lines are mostly palaeolithict: specifically the age ranges of the mitochondrial haplogroup V which is found at the highest frequency among the Basques and the Saami are preneolithic. In agreement with the suggestion proposed to explain the distribution of mtDNA haplogroup V (Torroni et al. 1998), the distributions of Y chromosome groups R* and R1a have been interpreted by Semino et al. (2000) to be the result of postglacial expansions from refugia within Europe. European mtDNA estimates the Neolithic component in the Basques to be the lowest for any region in Europe. Although the criteria used to identify Near Eastern founder types are somewhat heuristic and involve many assumptions, the relative number of types in different European populations should still be informative, and the Basque component, estimated at 7%, clearly lies outside the distribution for
References (33)
- 2 Vowels . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
- 3 Tones . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 3.3.1 Tone Splits and Mergers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
- 3.2 Tone in Tangwang . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66
- 3.3 Accent/Stress in Tangwang . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
- Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
- 4 Tangwang Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 4.1 Word Building . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
- 1.1 N + Suffix [ʦɿ] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
- 1.2 Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
- 2 Case Marking in the Tangwang Language . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
- 2.1 Nominative and Accusative. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
- 2.2 Accusative and Dative. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86
- 2.3 Ablative Case . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 4.2.4 Instrumental Case . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
- 2.5 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
- 3 Suffixes Borrowed from Mongolic Languages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
- 3.1 Reflexive Possessive Suffix [nə] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
- 3.2 Third Person Possessive [ȵi] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
- 3.3 The Suffix [thala] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96
- Tangwang Syntax . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 5.1 Word Order Typology and the Tangwang Language . . . . . . . . . . . 101
- 2 VO and OV in Tangwang . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 5.2.1 General Situation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 5.2.2 VO Order . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 5.2.3 OV Order . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104 5.2.4 VO and OV Are Both Possible . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 5.2.5 Adverbs and OV . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 5.2.6 Verb-Resultative Verb (VR) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
- 3 Influence of Standard Mandarin on Tangwang Syntax . . . . . . . . . . 110 5.3.1 Co-occurrence of ba and [xa] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
- Copula [ʂʅ] 'to be' . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112 5.3.3 The Verb [ʂuə] 'say' and Quoted Discourse . . . . . . . . . . . . 114
- Aspect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 5.4.1 [liɔ] and [xa] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115 5.4.2 [tʂɛ] and [tʂə] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 116 5.4.3 [kuə] . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 5.4.4 [li] and [liɛ]. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
- 5 Causative and Passive. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 5.5.1 [ki] as a Causative Marker . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 5.5.2 [ki] as a Passive Marker . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122
- 6 Concluding Remarks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
- About "Mixed Languages" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 6.1 Mixed Language Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 6.2 Lexical Versus Syntactic Borrowing. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126
- 2.1 Lexical Borrowing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127
- 2.2 Syntactic Borrowing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132
- 3 Quantification of Mixing Degree . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 6.3.1 Comparison of Two Cases of Borrowing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145
- 3.2 Further Tests and Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146
- 4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 149 Annex: Story in Tangwang. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153
- References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
- Author Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
- Subject Index. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181