Article history: Received 28 Sept. 2014
Introduction to Special Issue
Kurdish: GEOFFREY HAIG
A critical research overview ERGIN ÖPENGIN
Abstract
The Kurdish language is an integral component of any conceptualisation of “Kurdishness”, but
just what constitutes Kurdish remains highly disputed. In this introduction, we take up a num-
ber of key questions relating to Kurdish (e.g. whether it is one or more than one language,
which varieties should be considered under Kurdish, what are its origins, etc.), discussing them
in the light of contemporary linguistics. A critical assessment of the notions of “language” and
“dialect” is followed by a review of different approaches to classifying Kurdish, and exempli-
fied through the case-study of Zazaki. We suggest that a good deal of the confusion arises
through a failure to distinguish different kinds of linguistic evidence (in a narrow sense), from
the results of socially contracted and negotiated perceptions of identity, rooted in shared belief
systems and perceptions of a common history. We then present an overview of recent trends in
Kurdish linguistics and attempt to identify some of the most pressing research desiderata.
Keywords: Historical linguistics; areal linguistics; grammatical change; Kurdish.
Awirvedaneke rexneyî li lêkolînên li ser zimanê kurdî
Zimanê kurdî parçeyeke bingehî yê her çi têgihiştineke “Kurdînî”yê ye, lê belê hêj jî pirsek e ka kurdî bi xwe ji
çi teşkîl e. Di vê gotara seretayî de em berê xwe didine çendîn ji wan pirsên kilîlî yên derheq zimanê kurdî de
(wek, ka kurdî yek ziman e an ji yekê zêdetir ziman in, kîjan şêwezar divê di bin kurdî de bêne hesibandin, reh
û binetarên zimanê kurdî çi ne, hd.), û li jêr ronahiya zimannasiya hevçerx de li wan pirsan dikolin. Li dû 99
nirxandineke rexneyî ya têgehên “ziman” û “zarava”, hin boçûnên tesnîfkirina şêwezarên kurdî hatine raçavki-
rin, û bi pêdeçûneke dirêjtir li ser rewşa şêwezarê Zazakiyê hatine tetbîqkirin. Em diyar dikin ku para pirtir a
aloziyê ji wê yekê dertê ku delîlên zimannasî (bi menayeke berteng) bi duristî nayêne cudakirin ji sehên
huwiyet û xwanasandinê yên axêveran ku di nav jiyana civakî de durist dibin û rehên wan di pergaleke baweri-
yê û di tarîxeke hevbeş de ne. Em paşê meylên taze yên di zimannasiya kurdî de pêşkêş dikin û hewl didin hin
mijarên lêkolînê yên pêdivî destnîşan bikin.
لێکدانهوهیهکی ڕهخنهگرانه سهبارهت به لێکۆڵینهوهی زمانناسی:کوردی
هەرچەند مشتومڕێکی،زمانی کوردی وەک بەشێکی جیانەکراوە و پێویستی هەر شێوە لە بەچەمککردنی "کوردبوون" سەیر دەکرێت
ئێمە تاقمێک پرسیاری سەرەکی لەمەڕ، لەم پێشەکییەدا.زۆر لەسەر ئەو شتەی کە پاشماوەکانی ئەم زمانە پێک دێنێتەوە لە ئارادایە
دەبێ کامە جیاوازی و،زمانی کوردی بەدەستەوە دەدەین (وەک ئەوەی ئایا یەک یان زیاتر لە یەک زمانی کوردی بوونی هەیە
) و هەروەها لەژێر ڕووناکی.جۆربەجۆری لەژێر ناوی زمانی کوردیدا لێکبدرێتەوە؛ سەرچاوە و ڕەچەڵەکانی زمانی کوردی چین و هتد
" لەم رێگەدا هەڵسەنگاندنێکی ڕەخنەگرانە سەبارەت بە چەمکەکانی "زمان.زانستی زمانناسی هاوچەرخدا قسەوباسیان لەبارەوە دەکەین
و "زاراوە" دەگرینە بەر و بۆ ئەم مەبەستە پێداچوونەوەیەک سەبارەت بە ئەو بۆچوونە جیاوازانە دەستنیشان دەکەین کە پۆلینبەندی
ئێمە پیشانی دەدەین کە زۆربەی.زمانی کوردی دەکەن و هاوکات لە ڕێگەی بابەت ــ توێژینەوەی زازاکییەوە نموونەی بۆ دەهێنینەوە
هەرە زۆری ئەم پاشاگەردانییە بە هۆی کەمبوونی هەاڵوردەکردنی جۆرە جیاوازەکانی نموونە زمانییەکانەوە خۆی دەردەخات (بە
مانایەکی تەواو) و لە دەرەنجامی بەریەک کەوتنێکی کۆمەاڵیەتی و تێگەیشتنێکی دانووساندنانە لەسەر ناسنامە دەردەکەوێت کە ڕێشەی
لە درێژەدا هەڵسەنگاندنیکی گشتی دەهێنینەوە کە.لە سیستمگەلێکی باوەڕی هاوبەش و تێگەیشتن لە مێژووی گشتی و هاوبەشدا هەیە
تیایدا نوێترین ڕێبازەکانی زمانناسی کوردی پیشان دەدەین و هەوڵمان داوە تیایدا هەندێک لەو توێژینەوە چاپکراوە پێویستانەی کە
.لەمبارەوە کراون بناسێنین
Geoffrey Haig is Professor of Linguistics at the Department of General Linguistics, Universi-
ty of Bamberg, Germany. Email:
[email protected]
Ergin Öpengin is a Postdoctoral researcher and Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft fellow at
the Department of General Linguistics, University of Bamberg, Germany.
Email:
[email protected]
Kurdish Volume: 2, No: 2, pp. 99 – 122 October 2014
Studies ISSN: 2051-4883 & e-ISSN: 2051-4891 www.kurdishstudies.net
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Introduction
This volume of Kurdish Studies is dedicated to research on the Kurdish lan-
guage. While language issues have always been integral to academic and popu-
lar efforts at staking out Kurdish identities, much of the relevant linguistic
research either does not feed into the broader discourse, or is simplified or
distorted in various ways. We therefore welcome the initiative of the journal
editors in accepting a cross-section of current research in Kurdish linguistics,
reflecting fields as diverse as language contact and relevance theory in prag-
matics, as a modest attempt at embedding Kurdish linguistics more firmly into
the larger context of Kurdish studies.
Although the term “language” is a deceptively familiar item in most peo-
ple’s daily vocabulary, scientific approaches to “language” tend to highlight
different facets, yielding a multiplicity of varied conceptualisations: language
can be seen as a complex, self-organising semiotic system, as the repository of
cultural memory, an emblem of group identity, or a biologically endowed in-
stinct that triggers acquisition in early childhood, to name but a few. Our per-
spective, as linguists, is primarily in terms of language as a complex, self-
organising system, but we are sympathetic to an approach that sees the lin-
guistic system as embedded in a social matrix, of which it is both product, and
producer. In this introductory essay, we will take up both systemic linguistic
aspects as well as social ones in an attempt to develop a reasonably coherent
account of what constitutes “Kurdish”.
100 Attempting a definition of Kurdish/the Kurdish language(s) is an under-
taking beset with controversy. Our aim in the first part of this introductory
chapter is therefore to clarify certain conceptual matters concerning the no-
tions of “language” and “dialect”, before tackling some of the thornier issues
that have figured in discourse related to Kurdish, or “the Kurdish language”.
We then review some recent approaches to classifying Kurdish, before pre-
senting some proposals of our own. We suggest that a good deal of the con-
fusion arises through a failure to distinguish different kinds of linguistic evi-
dence (in a narrow sense), from the results of socially contracted and negoti-
ated perceptions of identity, rooted in shared belief systems and perceptions
of a common history. We do not argue for the precedence of any particular
kind of evidence in defining a language and its speech community; on the
contrary, a language is always at the nexus of a social construct with a set of
linguistic facts. What we emphasise, however, is the analytical importance of
distinguishing the findings of different fields. In the second part of the intro-
duction, we present an overview of recent trends in Kurdish linguistics,
though we make no claims to exhaustive coverage. Instead, we discuss what
we consider to have been the most salient trends, based on a selective cross-
section of the literature, and attempt to identify some of the most pressing
research desiderata. Our treatment focuses on more recent developments; we
refer to Haig and Matras (2002) for a summary of earlier research. Finally, in
the third section, we summarise the other contributions to this issue and how
they relate to the broader themes identified in this introduction.
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
On defining Kurdish
There are two principal inter-related issues in defining Kurdish: (i) which cri-
teria define its current scope, and which speech varieties should be included
under the label “Kurdish”; (ii) what is its historical descent, i.e. from which
proto-language(s) are the Kurdish varieties considered to have descended?
Though related, these issues are logically distinct. In practice, the first issue
relates mainly to the question whether Zazaki and Gorani (along with a few
other varieties like Laki and sections of what is generally called Luri) are part
of Kurdish, or independent languages. The second issue relates, on the one
hand, to whether a common ancestor can be postulated for all the varieties to
be considered under Kurdish, and, on the other hand, to the relationship of
this putative common ancestral Kurdish to a Middle or an Old Iranian lan-
guage. We begin with a discussion of the conceptual and methodological di-
mensions entailed in these two broad issues.
Dialect vs. language: conceptual issues
In spite of their apparent simplicity in daily usage, distinguishing between
“language” and “dialect” in a technical linguistic sense is very difficult, since
linguistic factors are inextricably entwined with sociological, political and eth-
nic ones. The most widespread diagnostic has been that of “mutual intelligi-
bility”, according to which varieties that are mutually intelligible are defined as
dialects of the same language, whereas varieties that are not mutually under-
standable are assigned to distinct languages. Intuitive as it may seem, it is far 101
from being a reliable diagnostic in several respects. In methodological terms,
it is the speakers of the varieties who understand each other or not, not the
varieties themselves. Thus speaker attitudes may weigh more than “objective”
measures of linguistic similarities/differences. It has also been pointed out
many times that this notion fails to account for chains of mutually intelligible
dialects (dialect continua), where speakers of contiguous varieties may under-
stand each other, but those at either end of the continua cannot. Where, then,
does one language stop and next begin? Furthermore, intelligibility is often a
dynamic process: on initial exposure to a different variety, one may under-
stand very little, but within a short space of time, intelligibility in one, or both,
directions, may increase dramatically. Again, this poses considerable methodo-
logical problems when attempting to assess mutual intelligibility.
In practice, there are numerous examples worldwide where the criterion is
simply ignored when languages are being defined. For instance, Swedish and
Norwegian are mostly mutually intelligible, both in spoken and written forms,
but they are standardly considered to be separate languages. On the reverse
side, mutually unintelligible varieties of Mandarin (Standard Chinese) and
Cantonese (the variety of Hong Kong, Macau, and Guangdong province),
together with five other major varieties, are considered to be dialects of Chi-
nese (cf. Wang, 1997: 55). In these and many other situations around the
world (e.g. Urdu-Hindi, Croatian-Bosnian-Serbian, Moldavian-Romanian),
what is a “language” and what is a “dialect” are determined on social and po-
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
litical grounds. More recently attempts have been made to relate measurable
linguistic differences (in the form of quantified phonetic differences, known
as Levenshtein distances) to degrees of mutual intelligibility, undertaken by
linguists working at the University of Groningen (see e.g. Gooskens, 2007).
The research has been conducted with speakers of Germanic languages such
as Danish, Dutch and Swedish, working in the contexts of written, standard-
ised languages in industrialised societies. It is difficult to see how this research
paradigm, promising though it may be, can be readily transferred to the Kurd-
ish context. In short, the oft-cited and supposedly “objective” criterion of
mutual intelligibility has to date been of little value in distinguishing languages
and dialects.
Recognising these shortcomings, a number of scholars have tried to go be-
yond mutual intelligibility towards more socially-informed definitions of lan-
guage and dialect. Crystal (1997: 248) has added “common/different cultural
history” to “mutual (un)intelligibility”. In a situation where these two criteria
do not match, it is considered not possible to decide on the status of the given
varieties. Trudgill’s (2000) distinction of “autonomous” vs. “heteronomous”
varieties addresses an important insight: an autonomous variety would be an
independent code, recognised as such for purposes of media and education
without necessary reference to an over-arching variety. A heteronomous va-
riety on the other hand is perceived as a variant of some autonomous code.
The intuition here is that when we use the term dialect, there is always the
102 sense of “dialect of X”, with X being some independently recognised linguis-
tic unit of a larger order. While this distinction is moderately useful in the con-
text of languages with state-sanctioned status, it is of restricted relevance for
the cluster of varieties that constitute Kurdish, and many other languages with
restricted official status.
It is thus a sobering fact that to this day, the science of linguistics has
nothing to offer in terms of an operative definition of “language” (cf. From-
kin et al., 2003: 446; Trudgill, 2000: 3–5). Within mainstream generative lin-
guistics, one response to this state of affairs has been to deny the relevance of
“a language” entirely, and to focus solely on the abstract linguistic abilities of
the individual speaker. Under this conception, linguistics “becomes part of
psychology, ultimately biology”, and the notion of “a language” (which im-
plies a community of speakers sharing that language) is simply sidestepped
(see Chomsky, 1986: 30-32 for justification of this line of argument). Sociolin-
guists, however, for whom the notion of speech community, or communities
of practice, is indispensable, (e.g. Fasold, 2005; Romaine, 2001), conclude that
language and dialect are fundamentally social, and not linguistic constructs: “a
language is a language if it has been so socially constructed” (Fasold, 2005:
698). The view that a language is in some sense a tangible, homogenous entity
with a more or less fixed form, most suggestively fostered in the case of large
national languages, is likewise untenable. As Linell (2005: 45) puts it:
[…] there is no single system of spoken language corresponding to
the idea of a unitary national language; instead there are overlap-
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
ping regional and social varieties as well as partially specific lan-
guages tied to communicative activities and genres. The notion of
a unitary national language is an artificial social reality attempted at
as the result of political actions, including linguists’ standardising
efforts.
Let us consider how some of these issues would relate to the Kurdish case.
It should by now be evident that there is no simple answer to the question of
whether Kurdish is “one language”, and if so, which varieties should belong
to it. The criterion of mutual intelligibility is fundamentally flawed, as we have
pointed out above, and would almost certainly yield contradictory results if
applied to Kurdish. In fact, in the absence of a generally accepted definition
of language, the question of whether Kurdish is a language is vacuous. We
can, however, meaningfully investigate what speakers of the varieties con-
cerned perceive about their own variety in relation to others. In this case,
there seems to be a relatively broad consensus among speakers of So-
rani/Central Kurdish, and speakers of Kurmanji/Northern Kurdish that their
respective varieties can be identified with a larger-order entity Kurdish/Kurdî.
Similar perceptions may hold for speakers of Southern Kurdish (Fattah,
2000), and for some varieties of Gorani (see below). We can then proceed to
explore the histories, sources and variations in these self-perceptions: where
do they originate, how have they shifted, how they correlate (or not) with oth-
er principles of social organisation (tribal, religious, means of subsistence, po- 103
litical). A perception of “Kurdish” is in fact historically well-attested in the
Sharafname of Sharaf Khan Bidlisi, who some 420 years ago was developing a
definition of Kurdish together with a detailed classification of its dialects on
ethnic/cultural grounds. Reference to Kurds, and to Kurdistan, feature regu-
larly in the sources of the Ottoman era; it is evident that a notion of group
identity that preceded the modern era was well established, but the precise
extension of that group identity is difficult to establish in retrospect.
In practice, there is relatively little controversy with regard to So-
rani/Central Kurdish, and Kurmanji/Northern Kurdish. The litmus test for
approaches to Kurdish are Zazaki, and Gorani. In the case of Zazaki, the dis-
cussion has become regrettably politically charged, and the linguistic argu-
ments are regularly instrumentalised by different political factions, rendering
rational debate increasingly difficult. The case is, however, extremely instruc-
tive, and therefore worth dwelling on in some detail here (below we take up
the arguments from historical linguistics on Zazaki). Reliable information on
the historical self-perceptions of Zaza speakers is hard to come by. Evliya
Çelebi, the celebrated Ottoman traveller, in his account of his journey from
Bitlis to Van, refers to the “Zaza” among a list of Kurdish tribes grazing the
alpine pastures of the Suphan Mountain north of Lake Van (Bulut 1997: 221).
In this part of his travelogue, Çelebi regularly refers loosely to “Kurdistan”
and the “Kurdish people” (qavm-i ekrad), but has otherwise little to say of the
Zazas. According to Özoğlu (2004: 34-35), in general Çelebi, like the historian
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Sharaf Khan mentioned above, treats the Zazas on a par with other tribal (and
dialectal) groups among the Kurds, concluding that “since Evliya gathered his
information among local sources from the region, one can conclude that the
Zaza speakers were considered Kurds by Evliya’s sources”. In more recent
times, when Kurds mobilised as a political entity as in the Sheikh Said uprising
of 1925, Zaza and Kurmanji speakers were collectively implicated (and the
leader of the revolt himself was a Zaza speaker). Finally, it is worth recalling
that one of the most widespread traditional self-designations for the Zazas
and their language is Kirmanj/Kirmanjkî We can reasonably assume that this
is the same word as Kurmanj/Kurmanjî; the difference in the quality of the
first vowel is minimal (the short centralised vowels are frequently interchange-
able in a number of words), and the suffixes -kî and -î are the regular equiva-
lents of each other in Zazaki and Kurmanji respectively. If this is the case, we
can assume a common self-designation for both groups, possibly in the sense
of a generic term for people associated with particular kinds of livelihoods,
rather than terms targeting ethnic or linguistic identities (see Asatrian, 2009:
28-30 for a discussion of the term “Kurmanj”). Thus although we are far
from anything approaching a reliable ethno-linguistic characterisation of pre-
modern identity perception among the Zazas, and although there are un-
doubtedly considerable local wrinkles that more general statement would fail
to capture, there is certainly good evidence for an inclusive perception of
“Kurds” which generally subsumed the Zazas.
104 At the turn of the 20th century, western philologists began to analyse the
structure of Zazaki. The most influential scholar in this respect was Oskar
Mann. Mann pointed out a number of phonological and morphological dif-
ferences between Zazaki and Kurmanji, which led him to the claim that the
two should be considered quite distinct languages. On his view, Zaza did not
belong to Kurdish (cf. discussion in Mann and Hadank, 1932: 19-23), and
below). Mann’s views were entirely based on linguistic/philological facts; they
actually entail no consequences in terms of speakers’ perceived identities, and
initially, the discussion on the position of Zazaki was largely confined to Ira-
nian philology. The speakers themselves were unaware of these evaluations of
their languages up until the 1980’s, when Kurdish intellectuals in the diaspora
came to be informed about the discussions. Subsequently, a small number of
Zazaki-speaking exile intellectuals, applying a positivistic notion to identifica-
tions and classifications, began to adopt the idea. As a result, some Zazaki-
speaking intellectuals who had previously referred to Zazaki as part of Kurd-
ish, and the Zazas as Kurds, rephrased their discourse in favour of an exclu-
sivist Zazaki language and identity distinct from Kurdish1. Given the many
1 It is worth noting that this shift (from Kurdish to an exclusivist Zaza(ki) identity) happened in
a relatively short period of time, as illustrated in the case of Zülfü Selcan, the author of the
book Grammatik der Zaza-Sprache (1998). The author was an activist in pro-Kurdish progressist
circles at the turn of 1980s. In an article that he published in the Hêvî magazine, publication of
the Paris Kurdish Institute, he freely uses the terms Kurd and Kurdish language to refer to
Zaza people and Zazaki, considering the latter under “Kurdish”. This is apparent also in the
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
political stakeholders in the contested arena of Kurdish identities, it is hardly
surprising that this debate has long since left the purely academic domain, and
the originally purely philological arguments have been entirely misconstrued
and instrumentalised by different parties. Our point here is that the arguments
from philology, which we take up below, were never intended as statements
regarding perceived identities, and are in fact largely orthogonal to that debate.
Reconstruction of language history
The analysis of the more or less “hard” linguistic facts – the lexicon, the
phonology, the morphology – can yield a basis for classification of languages,
involving the reconstruction of language history (commonly known as “com-
parative method”). The comparative method works on the comparison of
phonologies in a set of languages, and seeks to identify systematic corre-
spondences as evidence of shared history. In addition, features of morphology
and syntax may also be compared which again can yield clues regarding inno-
vations shared among the varieties investigated. The underlying assumption is
that certain kinds of change are regular, and the systematic comparison allows
one to identify changes shared in some varieties, but absent in others. From
this, the analyst reconstructs the most plausible set of historical movements –
splits among the investigated language group – that would have yielded the
available picture. Thus we can arrive at a relative measure for degree of relat-
edness among the varieties concerned: the more closely related two varieties
are, the shorter the time span that has elapsed between now and the point in 105
time at which the two varieties split from their common source. However, the
success of this method depends in part on the time depth of attestation of the
languages concerned. Note that this method works solely with observable
linguistic data, with little or no relation to the social conditions of the speech
communities themselves. Thus the results of the comparative method cannot
simply be interpreted as evidence for defining “language” or “dialect” (and
indeed, nothing in the comparative method is contingent on these concepts).
Of course we would expect a degree of correlation between socially deter-
mined identity perception, and degree of linguistic relatedness: a group shar-
ing a close-knit social structure and a common identity is unlikely to speak
varieties that are only distantly related, and conversely, we would not expect
groups speaking only very distantly related varieties to share a common group
identity; but these are only rough tendencies, and there are numerous exam-
title of the article, as “Folklorê Kurdî ebe zaravayê dimilkî”, which can be translated as “Kurd-
ish folklore in its Dimilkî dialect”. Some years after, the author adopted the view of Zazaki as a
separate language and the Zazas a distinct people from the Kurds. Recently, the author shared
his 1983 article on his online academic profile page, however, in the new version of his article
he replaced all of the occurrences of the terms “Dimilkî dialect” and “Kurdish” with “Zazaki
language” and “Zaza people”, deleting quite a few other references to Kurdish including those
in the title of the work. For the original version of the article see Zilfî (1983) and for the modi-
fied recent version of the same article see:
http://www.tunceli.edu.tr/akademik/fakulteler/edebiyat/bolum/doded/zaded/EdebiyateZaza
y1.pdf (Accessed: November 25, 2012).
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
ples worldwide where degree of social identity of the speakers does not match
linguistic relatedness of their speech. For example, many Black African Amer-
icans legitimately claim a group identity distinct from, for example, the de-
scendants of the British invaders of North America. Linguistically, however, it
is a simple fact that today, members of both groups speak closely–related va-
rieties of a Germanic language. The issue of language origins is logically dis-
tinct from the issue of perceived group identity, and should not be confused
with it, though in many cases the two do go hand in hand.
Although the classification provided by historical linguistics purports to be
“objective”, we should note that it is far from secure, and the available evi-
dence leaves considerable scope for interpretation. Let us briefly consider
some evidence from phonology, as it relates to the position of Zazaki vis-à-vis
Kurdish. In Zazaki, words such as werd- “eat”, or weš “good, fine” exhibit an
initial [w-], whereas Kurmanji and Sorani have the velar fricative [x-] (with
additional labialisation in some varieties). In this feature, Zazaki patterns with
Hawrami/Gorani (see below). However, although this piece of evidence
points towards a different path of development in Zazaki as opposed to So-
rani and Kurmanji, Gippert (1996) concludes that on the whole, the evidence
from phonology does not suffice to yield a conclusive picture regarding the
position and origins of Zazaki.
Potentially more revealing is the evidence from morphology. Perhaps the
most salient difference between Zazaki and the rest of Kurdish is the for-
106 mation of the present indicative. In Zazaki, it is formed with an infixal aug-
ment, containing the nasal [-n], which attaches to the present stem: we-n-o (eat-
PRES.AUGM-3S.MASC) “he eats”, (cf. Paul, 1998: 74-76 for details). In
Kurmanji and Sorani, on the other hand, no stem-final augment is possible in
the present tense. Instead, present stems are preceded by certain prefixes, for
example di- in most of Kurmanji, as in di-xw-e (IND-eat:PRS-3SG) “s/he eats”,
de- or a- in Sorani, and so on. It is generally assumed that the stem-augment
of Zazaki goes back to an old participial form, but there is no reflex of this
participle in either Sorani or Kurmanji. However, close parallels are found in
West Iranian languages of the Caspian region, for example Mazanderani
(Haig, in print), or Semnani (Gippert, 1996). These facts, taken at face value,
speak for a Caspian origin of Zazaki. However, the interpretation remains
controversial; Gippert (1996), and following him Jügel (this volume) suggest
that the present-stem formation based on a participle may be a “recent” loan
influence, citing North East Neo-Aramaic and East Armenian as possible
sources. In the case of North-East Neo-Aramaic, this does not seem particu-
larly plausible, as it is by no means clear where, or when, Zazaki would have
been exposed to heavy influence from any variety of this language. East Ar-
menian is a much more likely contender. But why should all varieties of Za-
zaki, regardless of their geographic setting, have undergone a contact-induced
development of this type, whereas none of the surrounding varieties of
Kurmanji did, although they were exposed to Armenian and Neo-Aramaic
influence? Finally, the simple fact remains that in order to develop a participi-
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
al-based present stem formation at all, the original participial-forming mor-
phology must have been retained in Zazaki. It is the retention of this mor-
phology in Zazaki that sets it off from the rest of Kurdish, and this can hardly
be explained through recent contact influence. Rather, it would seem more
probable that precisely this point distinguished the ancestor of Zazaki from
the ancestors of the other varieties considered to constitute Kurdish, and the
development of the participial-based present tense (and the extension of the
nasal augment to the past imperfective) are early innovations, hence found in
all varieties of Zazaki. Contact influence is certainly possible, but its source is
more likely to be found in the formative stages of Zazaki, possibly prior to its
spread to its current location.
It is often suggested that Zazaki is in fact more closely related to Gorani
than to either Kurmanji or Sorani. The most prominent advocate of this view
was Oskar Mann, who went as far as to claim “near identity” of Zazaki and
Gorani (Mann and Hadank, 1932: 25). Hadank himself, however, who was
entrusted with posthumously preparing Mann’s work for publication, had al-
ready pointed out that his predecessor’s assessment was exaggerated (Mann
and Hadank, 1932: 25-26). Above we noted the presence of an initial [w-] in
words such as “eat” and “read/study” in Zazaki, which is also shared in Go-
rani. However, it is not an exclusive factor uniting Zazaki and Gorani; it is
also found, for example, in Balochi (Korn, 2005: 122). It is therefore fairly
thin evidence on which to base a Zazaki/Gorani group, as is often assumed
(see below). Indeed, although we still await a detailed systematic comparison 107
of Gorani and Zazaki (a surprising desideratum), we are currently unaware of
a truly convincing historical demonstration of the viability of this sub-
grouping. It is true that each display features common to other languages out-
side of Kurdish, and not shared by Sorani and Kurmanji. Zazaki, for example,
shows obvious parallels to Iranian language of the Caspian region. But from
this it does not follow that they can be meaningfully grouped together within
a putative historical group of “Kurdish”. It simply follows that historically,
both need to be set off from Sorani and Kurmanji.
The concept of “Southern Kurdish” also raises certain difficulties. One is-
sue concerns where the borders of Southern Kurdish in relation to geograph-
ically contiguous West Iranian languages, such as Luri, should be drawn.
Anonby (2003), for example, suggests that Luri is part of a language continu-
um spanning northwest Iranian Kurdish, and southwest Iranian Persian. Such
a statement is difficult to reconcile with the traditional view of a northwest vs.
southwest Iranian split, and would essentially dissolve Southern Kurdish as a
viable genetic group.2 The question of “Southern Kurdish” has been most
extensively treated in Fattah (2000), who defends the coherence of the group.
2 Anonby (2003) combines observations and interviews regarding levels of mutual intelligibility,
with lexicostatistics. However, although both methods are of considerable interest in their own
right, neither method will reliably yield genetic sub-groupings, and nor will a combination of
the two. Anonby (2004/2005) applies more reliable methods to a classification of one variety of
Laki.
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Fattah recognises a group which he refers to as kurde du sud, which covers
most of what is traditionally included under “Southern Kurdish”, but he sub-
sumes this group under a larger group of dialectes kurdes méridionaux. Within the
latter, Fattah includes Laki, but considers it distinct from kurde du sud. The
most salient linguistic feature distinguishing Laki from kurde du sud is the pres-
ence of ergativity in past tenses in Laki (Fattah, 2000: 61). The position of
Luri within this scheme is discussed at some length (Fattah, 2000: 40-55), with
the general conclusion that it lies outside of the dialectes kurdes méridionaux, alt-
hough within Luri itself there are internal divisions that remain problematic
for any straightforward classification in terms of Kurdish / non-Kurdish.
Within all classifications known to us, Kurmanji and Sorani are uncontro-
versially defined as belonging to “Kurdish”. The question of their relationship
to each other has, however, seldom been explicitly discussed (see Jügel (this
volume) for references). In Haig and Öpengin (forthcoming), it is noted that
although there are intermediate varieties exhibiting typical properties of both
(e.g. Surči, discussed in MacKenzie, 1961), in general the boundary between
Sorani and Kurmanji is relatively clearly delineated. There is a long list of dis-
tinctive morphological features that distinguish them (cf. Haig and Öpengin,
forthcoming), and for any given variety, it is generally not difficult to assign it
to either Sorani or Kurmanji. This fact is rather surprising; it is certainly not
what one would expect if Sorani and Kurmanji had originated from a single
source, and then gradually spread into their current localities. Such a scenario
108 would have yielded a dialect continuum, with each variety gradually shading
into the next. Instead, we find two large and relatively distinct speech zones,
sharing a fairly narrow ribbon of overlap in which there are varieties exhibit-
ing the typical features of both. What this suggests is that Sorani and Kurman-
ji evolved in geographically distinct regions, and later came into contact. This
is not to deny the obvious relationship between the two, but it suggests that
we require a more sophisticated account of the genesis of these two varieties
than is currently available.
In sum, historical linguistics can help unravel the relationships between re-
lated languages (or indeed demonstrate their relatedness in the first place). But
we would urge caution in interpreting the findings, which are seldom as clear-
cut as the family trees that are traditionally used to represent them (see Jügel,
this volume). But again, the problems here are not restricted to Kurdish, but
are endemic to historical linguistics, regardless of the language family con-
cerned. For example, in Oceanic linguistics the term “linkage” is regularly
used to refer to geographically contiguous groups of related languages which
exhibit certain similarities, but cannot be reliably traced to a common ances-
tor, hence would not be representable on a traditional family tree (Lynch et
al., 2011). We have briefly investigated some of the problems associated with
Zazaki and Gorani in this respect, noting that from the perspective of histori-
cal linguistics, there is no doubt that these two are less closely related to So-
rani and Kurmanji. However, we also note that this does not imply that Za-
zaki and Gorani ever formed a historical unit. But perhaps the most important
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
point is that the comparative method yields a hypothesis regarding the ancient
history of the languages; its results cannot be simply translated into claims
regarding social identity constructions of the speakers.
Existent approaches to classification of Kurdish
Having discussed two major approaches to identifying language, we will
briefly review some existing classifications of Kurdish. It is noteworthy that
most classifications are not explicitly justified, but draw on a mix of geograph-
ic, socio-historical, ethnic and linguistic criteria. This is particularly true of
Hassanpour (1992) and Izady (1992). The classification of Fattah (2000) is
based on a more detailed discussion of linguistic and extra-linguistic criteria,
with a focus on Southern Kurdish. Consider first the classifications of Has-
sanpour and Izady:
Hassanpour’s (1992: 20) classification Izady’s (1992) classification of
of Kurdish varieties Kurdish varieties
I. Kurmanci Kurmanji group Pahlawani group
II. Sorani I. North Kurmanji I. Dimili (or Zaza)
III. Hawrami II. South Kurmanji II. Gurani (including
IV. Kirmashani (or Sorani) Laki and Hawrami)
Even these broad classifications differ in a number of respects, some
merely terminological but others more substantial. For example, in Has- 109
sanpour’s classification the term Hawrami, which is a highly specific regional
variety in Iranian and Iraqi Kurdistan (see MacKenzie, 1961, Mahmoudveysi
et al., 2011: 2-4), is intended as a general term encompassing both Gorani and
Zazaki. Given the linguistic (see above) and geographical distance between the
two varieties, it is at best odd to subsume one of the varieties under the other.
Izady’s classification involves an initial division into two major groups,
“Kurmanji” vs. “Pahlawani”. The latter name might be intended to reflect the
claim that both Zaza and Gurani are often considered rather archaic in the
sense that they have preserved certain features found in Parthian. But it is not
clear if this is intended, nor is it clear why Laki should be included in this pu-
tative grouping. It will be seen that in Izady’s classification, there is no obvi-
ous equivalent to Hassanpour’s “Kirmashani”. Of course both classifications
were intended as rough guides, and make no claim to exacting scientific rig-
our. But even this perfunctory comparison suffices to reveal a number of con-
tradictions and unresolved issues.
A classification based on extensive fieldwork and with more detailed justi-
fication is proposed by Fattah (2000):
Kurdish group Kurdo-Caspian group
I. Northern Kurdish or Kurmanji I. Zazaki
II. Central Kurdish or Sorani II. Hawrami (Gorani)
III. Southern Kurdish
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
In particular, Fattah discusses in considerable detail the nature of “South-
ern Kurdish” in relation to Laki, and also to Gurani. His suggestion of a Kur-
do-Caspian group is developed at some length (2000: 62-70), and it is worth
dwelling briefly on it here. Fattah concedes to the majority view of Iranian
philologists, who concur on assigning Zazaki and Gorani to a peripheral sta-
tus vis-à-vis the rest of Kurdish, but argues that this does not necessarily im-
ply their exclusion from Kurdish as a socio-linguistic unit (Fattah, 2000: 65):
En admettant l’hypothese de l’origine caspienne des Gurân, qui
remonterait à des époques très anciennes, et de leur installation, en-
suite, d’une part dans la grande région de Kirmânshah-Hamadân, et
d’autre part, pour une partie d’entre eux tels que les Zâzâ, vers l’ouest
dans le Kurdistan de Turquie, celle-ci n’est pas forcément en contra-
diction avec leur appartenance et leur implication dans les processus
de formation du peuple kurde, ou du moins leur fusion trés ancienne
avec eux, et leur participation dans la constitution de l’identité poli-
tique, culturelle et sociologique du peuple kurde. [footnote omitted]
This is a view we would generally comply with, though as linguists, we
have some reservations regarding the postulation of a distinct sub-group for
Zaza and Gurani, for the reasons outlined in the preceding section. Our own,
more cautious approach would be the following, which avoids sub-grouping
Zazaki and Gorani (at least until positive evidence in favour of such a move is
110 forthcoming) and identifies the five groups, whose approximate localities are
shown in Figure 1:
1. Northern Kurdish (Kurmanji): It is often divided into Badini (spoken
principally in Duhok and Hakkari provinces) and Kurmanji (in the
rest of Northern Kurdish speech zone) varieties; both include a num-
ber of other regional dialects (see Öpengin and Haig, this volume).
2. Central Kurdish (Sorani): Its main regional dialects are Mukri (Maha-
bad), Hewlêrî (Erbil), Silêmanî (Suleimaniya), Germiyanî (Kirkuk) and
Sineyî (Sanandaj).
3. Southern Kurdish: It includes the varieties such as Kelhuri, Feyli,
Kirmashani, as well as some dialects of what is called Laki, in Ilam
and Kermanshah provinces of Iraq and the town of Khaneqin in Iraq
(see above on Fattah’s classification).
4. Gorani: It covers what is known as Hawrami or Hawramani, with
the well-known dialects of e.g. Paveh and Halabja, and includes the
old transdialectal literary koine, the language of religious rites among
some Yaresan groups. In this sense, “Gorani” would include several
varieties spoken in present-day Iraq, e.g. Bajalani. (cf. Fattah, 2000:
62-70, and Mahmoudveysi et al., 2012 for discussion of “Gorani”).
5. Zazaki: Its three main dialects are Northern Zazaki (Tunceli-Erzincan
provinces), Central Zazaki (Bingöl-Diyarbakir provinces) and South
Zazaki (Diyarbakir province and Siverek town).
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
Figure 1. Map of language varieties spoken by the Kurds (from Öpengin,
2013)
In sum, there is no consensus in the literature when it comes to defining,
and classifying “Kurdish”. Again, this is not a particularly surprising, nor by 111
any means unusual state of affairs. A closer look at most of the supposed clear
cases of “languages” on a global scale yields a similar picture. The exercise is
nevertheless valuable as it serves to highlight precisely those areas where con-
flicting approaches yield methodological and conceptual challenges. In our
view, the crucial point is to avoid conflating the results from distinct method-
ologies, and to explicitly recognise the limitations of any kind of static taxon-
omy.
We certainly acknowledge, however, that such classifications are not mere-
ly academic exercises, but have quite concrete repercussions. Consider for
example the online discussion on whether Zazaki should have an independent
entry in Wikipedia, as opposed to a sub-entry under Kurdish.3 The arguments
posted provide interesting insights into how ideological arguments impact on
supposedly democratic forums. What should have emerged from this section,
among other things, is that it is perfectly possible to accept both the conclu-
sions of the historical linguists (Zazaki is historically not closely related to
Kurmanji), and the conclusions of many native speakers (Zazaki speakers are
Kurds, and their language belongs to a larger-order entity “Kurdish”). There is
not necessarily any contradiction here.
3 http://meta.wikimedia.org/wiki/Requests_for_new_languages/Wikipedia_Zazaki.
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Current trends in Kurdish linguistics
Taken in the broadest sense, linguistic research on Kurdish runs back several
centuries, starting with the grammar of Ali Taramakhi (see Leezenberg, 2014)
towards the end of 17th or 18th century and the grammar of Maurizio Garzoni
in 1787. However, we are concerned here with work that has been undertaken
in an academic context within the disciplines of Iranian philology, or general
linguistics, and we consider only work undertaken since 2000. We also exclude
the numerous studies conducted in the realm of standardisation, and the large
number of pedagogical works on Kurdish that have appeared in the last dec-
ade. Within European and North American academia, Kurdish linguistics re-
mains institutionally poorly represented; at best, Kurdish linguistics is an ancil-
lary sub-discipline within another department, as in the department of Gen-
eral Linguistics at the University of Bamberg, or at the Department of Empir-
ical Linguistics at the University of Frankfurt, or it is undertaken within the
framework of comparative Iranian philology at the departments of Iranian
Studies in Hamburg and Göttingen. The Kurdish Institute in Paris continues a
long tradition of invaluable descriptive work on Kurdish, but their main re-
search output is concentrated in political science and sociology. Somewhat
paradoxically, Turkey now offers considerable scope for Kurdish linguistics.
In particular, the Mardin Artuklu University has recently undertaken moves to
establish a section on linguistics as part of its existing Kurdish programme. In
Iran work on various varieties of Kurdish has been undertaken within Iranian
112 studies, though much of it remains relatively inaccessible to scholars outside
the country. In Iraqi Kurdistan, there are Kurdish departments at the many
recently-grounded universities, offering exciting new prospects for interna-
tional cooperation, though we are not in a position to sketch these develop-
ments here.
Despite some positive developments, research on Kurdish linguistics con-
tinues to be hampered by a lack of institutional support in domains such as
post-graduate or PhD programmes, and a lack of secure teaching positions.
Current research on the Kurdish language is thus largely carried out in a
piecemeal fashion by individual researchers from a disjoint set of disciplines,
without an overarching institutional framework. There is also a lack of dedi-
cated journals or regular conferences that treat the topic. However, following
the International Workshop on Variation and Change in Kurdish (August
2013, Bamberg), the organisers have decided to establish the event on a regu-
lar annual basis (in 2014 the conference is held at the Mardin Artuklu Univer-
sity).
Theoretical approaches to Kurdish
Within the framework of what Culicover and Jackendoff (2005) refer to as
Mainstream Generative Grammar, a number of studies have appeared on
Kurdish syntax, starting with Fattah (1997), and work in the generative tradi-
tion is continued for Sorani in a number of papers by Karimi on the ezafe
(2007) and agreement (2010), while Karimi-Doostan (2005) discusses complex
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
predicates. For Kurmanji, ongoing work by Gündoğdu (2011, forthcoming)
continues the tradition of generative-inspired research on syntax. A different
framework (Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, HPSG) is utilised by
Samvelian (2006, 2007) in her work on clitics in Sorani, while an alternative
approach drawing on prosodic phonology is developed by Öpengin (2013).
More theory-neutral approaches are adopted by Franco et al. (2013) on ezafe,
Haig (2002) on complex predicates, and Öpengin (2012b) on adpositions and
argument structure in Sorani. Pragmatics and relevance theory has been ex-
plored with reference to the Badini dialect of Kurdish extensively by Unger
(2012, and this volume).
General descriptive and historical studies
Apart from the numerous pedagogical works that have been produced in
the past decade (not discussed here), two interesting grammars (Sorani and
Kurmanji respectively) have been published on the internet by Thackston (not
dated). However, undue reliance on the model of Persian has led to some dis-
tortions, particularly in the Kurmanji grammar, which needs to be treated with
caution. A short grammatical sketch, intended primarily for linguists, is Aygen
(2007). Regrettably, it contains numerous factual and analytical errors, and
appears to be based on data gathered from a single native speaker, with some
supplementary explanations from Bedir Khan and Lescot (1970); we mention
it solely for the sake of completeness, but definitely cannot recommend it. A
lengthy overview of “Kurdish” by McCarus (2009) is useful, but treats almost 113
exclusively Sorani. Fattah (2000) is a monumental study of “Southern Kurd-
ish”, though somewhat difficult to navigate through. Relevant to the concept
of “Southern Kurdish” are the articles by Anonby (2003 and 2004/2005),
which discuss the place of Luri and Laki. An overview of Kurmanji in Turkey
is Haig and Öpengin (forthcoming), and an initial classification of Kurmanji
dialects is now available in Öpengin and Haig (this volume). Haig and
Öpengin (in print) provide an updated synthesis of structural and socio-
cultural aspects of gender in Kurdish. One area where Kurdish linguistics has
benefited from recent developments in general linguistics is research on en-
dangered and under-studied languages. Within the framework of a
Volkswagen-Foundation funded project, two dialects of Gorani from West
Iran were documented (see Mahmoudveysi et al., 2012, and Mahmoudveysi
and Bailey, 2013). In a similar spirit, Öpengin (2013, forthcoming) provides
the most comprehensive treatment of a dialect of Central Kurdish available to
date in any European language, based on extensive original texts and com-
bined with a theoretically informed discussion of person marking in this varie-
ty. Published research on the first-language acquisition of ergativity in
Kurmanji is now available in Mahalingappa (2013), while Mohamad (2014)
investigates Kurdish-German code-switching among pre-school children in
Austria.
The history of Kurdish syntax, with particular reference to alignment, is
discussed in Haig (2004a, 2008 and forthcoming b) within the context of West
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Iranian. An overview of grammatical changes is provided in Jügel (this vol-
ume). For Zazaki, since the comprehensive grammars of Paul (1998) and
Selcan (1998), very little substantial linguistic research has appeared. An over-
view of linguistic and socio-cultural aspects of the Zaza is provided in Keskin
(2010), and Paul (2009). Our overall impression is that within linguistics, Za-
zaki remains sorely underrepresented, both in terms of descriptive and more
theoretically oriented research.
Kurdish in contact with other languages
Kurdish is nested in a complex multi-lingual context, and the effects of
language contact continue to be a key topic in Kurdish linguistics. Following
the pioneering work by Dorleijn (1996) on ergativity, a number of topics have
been investigated. The vowel system is studied in Özsoy and Türkyilmaz
(2006) and evidentiality in Bulut (2000). A perspective on Kurdish as part of a
putative Anatolian linguistic area is Haig (2001, 2006, 2007, in print, forth-
coming a), Matras (2002, 2007, 2010). While the impact of Turkish, Persian
and Arabic on Kurdish has been emphasised in a number of publications,
there is also growing interest in Kurdish influence on Neo-Aramaic (e.g.
Khan, 2007, and Noorlander, this volume), and on vernacular varieties of Ar-
abic (Talay, 2006/2007). These studies are important as they provide valuable
linguistic evidence regarding the role of Kurdish as a lingua franca across large
areas prior to the spread and increasing dominance of the national languages.
114
Sociolinguistics
The highly complex social and political dimensions in which the Kurdish
language is evolving have been treated in a number of recent works. Of par-
ticular importance is the special issue of the International Journal of the Sociology of
Language, in 2012, dedicated to various questions of Kurdish in the countries it
is spoken. A number of publications by Salih Akin have created an important
body of literature in French, of special interest are Akin (2000, 2002, 2004).
Recently, some fieldwork-based sociolinguistic research has investigated the
linguistic vitality of Kurdish in Turkey (Öpengin, 2012a, Çağlayan, 2014), the
language attitudes of the speakers (Coşkun et al., 2013), but there has been
surprisingly little work within variationist sociolinguistics. A large number of
articles, on the other hand, have documented the language policy in Kurdi-
stan, to name only a few Zeydanlıoğlu (2012), Sheyholislami (2012), Has-
sanpour (2012), Haig (2004b and 2012). Hassanpour (2001) has dealt with the
gendered language in Sorani Kurdish, while Asadpour et al. (2012) discusses
the address terms in the same variety.
As is evident from this short overview, Kurdish linguistics has continued
to develop despite the paucity of institutional support. There is an increasing
level of theoretical sophistication in much of the more recent work, carried by
a growing number of highly-trained younger scholars, which bodes well for
the future of the field. Nevertheless, certain areas remain somewhat under-
represented, a couple of which we briefly outline here: within sociolinguistics,
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
the dominant paradigm remains a more abstract socio-political one, drawing
on identity, nationalism, and ethnicity theories rather than data-driven varia-
tionist approaches to sociolinguistics and multilingualism. With regard to the
history of Kurdish, given the continued popular interest in the topic, we are
surprised to note how little research is actually dedicated to disentangling the
linguistic facts; as mentioned, one topic which obviously demands attention is
the nature of the historical relationship between Zazaki and Gorani. Current-
ly, most researchers are content to repeat opinions originally formulated 100
years ago, which are sorely in need of verification. Given the advances in his-
torical linguistics over the last decades, we would welcome an application of
more recent methodologies to these questions. Finally, there is still a need for
well-grounded descriptive work on the numerous regional varieties within
Kurdish, which would provide the raw material for a more comprehensive
assessment of central issues regarding the nature of Kurdish as a linguistic and
socio-cultural entity, and its historical evolution.
Synopsis of the contributions
In his contribution, Thomas Jügel takes up the question of the relationship of
Kurdish to the rest of the West Iranian languages and attempts to develop a
relative chronology of the changes that led to Kurdish becoming a distinct
group. As Jügel notes, reconstructing the history of Kurdish is hampered by
the lack of reliable attestation beyond a couple of centuries. Thus any at-
tempts to trace the history of Kurdish need to rely in part on inferences 115
gleaned from the histories of better-attested, closely related languages, in this
case, Parthian and Middle Persian. The other confounding factor in recon-
structing the history of Kurdish is the effect of language contact. The state of
any particular variety, at any given point in time, is the result of both its inher-
itance – those features which are simply the continuation of the earlier stages
of that variety – and the influence of the languages with which it has been in
contact. Kurdish is a particularly challenging case due to the multi-lingual en-
vironment in which it is traditionally spoken, and also to the mobility of its
speakers over many centuries. Contact effects cannot be easily represented in
traditional family trees, so Jügel sets up a modified model of historical rela-
tionships which attempts to synthesise both “vertical” inheritance of linguistic
features and lateral effects of language contact. Jügel identifies a number of
grammatical features that serve to distinguish among different varieties of
Kurdish, including the presence versus absence of pronominal clitics, the
presence versus absence of oblique case marking on nouns, or gender on
nouns. On this basis, he proposes a relative chronology of grammatical
changes that led to the current distribution, basing his findings on what is
known of parallel developments in the better-attested Middle Iranian lan-
guages Parthian and Middle Persian. This is an important contribution as it
demonstrates both the potentials, and the limitations, of historical reconstruc-
tion in the case of Kurdish.
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Regional variation in Kurmanji is the topic of Öpengin and Haig’s contri-
bution. While an awareness of regional variation and the ability to negotiate
around it are part of informed native speakers’ competence, to date there is
virtually no serious research dedicated to the topic. The authors propose a
broad distinction into five regions, and then proceed to apply a combination
of established methodologies, targeting the lexicon, the phonology and the
morpho-syntax, with the intention of identifying the main linguistic features
that serve to differentiate the various regions. Given the size of the region and
lack of previous research, they emphasise the tentative nature of the proposed
classification, but note that their findings are generally consonant with layper-
sons’ perceptions, and also reflect the rough geographic distribution of the
varieties. The authors identify “Southeastern Kurmanji”, the variety of Hakka-
ri province, and including the Badini variety of Iraqi Kurdistan, as the most
distinctive, in that it possesses the largest number of features not shared by
any other variety. In some respects, these properties can be related to South-
eastern Kurmanji’s proximity to Sorani, but this cannot be the whole story, as
some of the features concerned contrast sharply with Sorani. The authors also
identify a Northwestern Kurmanji, e.g. of Elbistan, which displays a number
of divergent features, many of which have scarcely been documented, to say
nothing of being analysed. Along with presenting an initial framework for
future research on regional variation in Kurmanji, the authors aim for a rec-
onciliation between dialectal/philological research and more general work on
116 Kurmanji, which has primarily focussed on issues of standardisation. Rather
than seeing regional variation as an obstacle to standardisation, it can also be
seen as the repository of rich linguistic resources, and a legitimate source for
enriching the available register repertoire in Kurmanji.
In Christoph Unger’s contribution, a novel approach to the interpretation
of the so-called “future tense” in Badini Kurmanji is adopted, based on Sper-
ber and Wilson’s Relevance Theory (1995). Along with an analysis of the
Badini Kurdish facts, the paper also proposes an explanation for the differ-
ences between Badini Kurdish and the more widely-known Botan-based
“standard” variety of Kurdish with regard to tense and modality marking. Un-
ger notes that the so-called future marker of Badini, dê, is not only used to
indicate future time reference, but also for a number of modal nuances. The
question is, can these apparently divergent semantic functions be related to
each other in a coherent manner, or should we simply accept a disjoint list of
temporal and modal meanings. Unger argues for a unified semantics of dê,
framed in terms of procedural semantics: an underspecified set of conditions
that guides the listener’s interpretation of a particular linguistic item, on the
assumption that the listener will apply general default principles of relevance
and efficiency in her interpretation. In the case of dê, a procedural semantic
account involves the claim that the state of affairs modified by dê lies outside
the range of shared verifiable experience, but the speaker nevertheless com-
mits herself to their factuality. This would include future states of affairs
which the speaker can reasonably assume to come about, but other kinds of
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
events that are not verifiable but whose plausibility the speaker commits to.
The particle dê in Badini contrasts with another modal particle, da (not present
in other dialects of Kurmanji), and this opposition also serves to delineate the
function of dê in Badini. The Badini system contrasts with most other dialects
of Kurmanji, which allow the dê particle (or its variants wê, ê etc.) to combine
with different kinds of verb forms, including past subjunctives. Unger relates
these formal differences in combinability of dê with different verb forms to
differences in the procedural semantics associated with the particle in Badini,
and with its counterpart in the rest of Kurmanji. Thus what appears to be a
minor, and often overlooked, distinction in the grammars of the different va-
rieties can be plausibly related to distinct underlying semantics of the particles
concerned.
The pervasive effects of the national languages Turkish, Persian and Ara-
bic on Kurdish, both in the lexicon and the grammar, have been regularly
noted in the literature, and are a recurrent target of disapproval in some cir-
cles. What is much less well-known is that Kurdish itself has had a deep im-
pact on another language of the region, Neo-Aramaic. Kurdish influence on
Neo-Aramaic is the topic of Paul Noorlander’s richly illustrated contribution,
which draws on Matras’ functional-communicative theoretical framework for
analysing language contact. Kurdish influence on Neo-Aramaic is most evi-
dent in the lexicon, but it runs much deeper than merely the borrowing of a
large number of Kurdish words. It is generally agreed among Neo-Aramaicists
that the emergence of ergativity in the past/perfective verb system of Neo- 117
Aramaic largely follows a Kurdish model; the structural parallels are so strik-
ing, and the development is so unusual within Semitic that Kurdish influence,
although not actually provable, can hardly be discounted. This is perhaps the
most remarkable indication of the long-standing and intense contact between
the two language communities. A number of syntactic parallels include pat-
terns of clause combining, negation, and ezafe constructions, while in the
realm of morphology we find the adoption of the Central Kurdish definite-
ness suffix in some Neo-Aramaic dialects, or the Kurdish comparative suffix
for adjectives also being adopted in varieties of Neo-Aramaic. While the his-
tory of Kurdish has tended to be cast against the backdrop of the emergence
and increasing domination of the national languages in the last century, Noor-
lander’s article is a timely reminder of the centuries of co-existence between
Kurds and members of other stateless minorities in the region prior to the era
of nation states, a history which left an indelible mark on both languages.
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
References
Akin, S. (2000). Les Kurdes et le Kurdistan dans le discours scientifique turc.
The Journal of Kurdish Studies, 3, 51–60.
Akin, S. (2002). Discours rapporté et hétérogénéité discursive en kurde. Faits
de Langue, 19, 71–84.
Akin, S. (2004). La dénomination des personnes et la construction identitaire :
le cas des prénoms kurdes en Turquie. Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée,
80, 27-38.
Anonby, E. (2003). Update on Luri: How Many Languages? Journal of the Royal
Asiatic Society, 13(2), 171–97.
Anonby, E. (2004/5). Kurdish or Luri? Laki’s Disputed Identity in the Luri-
stan Province of Iran. Kurdische Studien, 4/5, 7–22.
Asadpour, H., Louragi-Pour, J. and Qalati, M. S. (2012). Sorani Kurdish ad-
dress forms: Case of Northwest Iran. International Journal of Linguistics,
4(4), 421–449.
Asatrian, G. (2009). Prolegomena to the Study of the Kurds. Iran and Caucasus,
13, 1–58.
Aygen, G. (2007). Kurmanji Kurdish Grammar. Munchen: Lincom Europa.
Bedir Khan, E. and Lescot, R. (1970). Grammaire Kurde. Paris: Adrien Mai-
sonneuve.
Bulut, C. (1997). Evliya Çelebis Reise von Bitlis nach Van: ein Auszug aus dem Seya-
hatname. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
118 Bulut, C. (2000). Indirectivity in Kurmanji. In L. Johanson and B. Utas (eds.),
Evidentials. Turkic, Iranian and Neighbouring Languages (147–84). Berlin:
Mouton, 2000.
Çağlayan, H. (2014). Aynı Evde Ayrı Diller: Kuşaklararası Dil Değişimi/Eğilimler,
Sınırlar, Olanaklar Diyarbakır Örneği. Diyarbakır: Diyarbakır Sosyal Siyasal
Arastırmalar Enstitüsü.
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New
York: Praeger.
Coşkun, V., Derince, Ş. M. and Uçarlar, N. (2011). Scar of Tongue: Consequences
of the Ban on the Use of Mother Tongue in Education and Experiences of Kurdish
Students in Turkey. Diyarbakır: Diyarbakır Sosyal Siyasal Arastırmalar En-
stitüsü.
Crystal, D. 1997. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language (2nd edition). Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Culicover, P. and Jackendoff, R. (2005). Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Dorleijn, M. (1996). The Decay of Ergativity in Kurdish. Tilburg: Tilburg Universi-
ty Press.
Fasold, R. W. (2005). Making Languages. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K.
Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium
on Bilingualism (697–702). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Fattah, I. K. (2000). Les Dialectes Kurdes Méridionaux: étude linguistique et dialec-
tologique. Louvain: Peeters.
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
Fattah, M. (1997). A Generative Grammar of Kurdish. (Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation). University of Amsterdam, Holland.
Franco, L., Manzini, R. and Savoia, L. (2013). Some call it article, some ezafe:
“Linkers” as “agreement”. Retrieved from http://www.academia.edu
/4393956/Some _call_it_article_some_ezafe_ Linkers_as_agreement
(Accessed: 22 January, 2014).
Fromkin, V., Rodman, R. and Hyams, N. (2003). An Introduction to Language
(7th ed.). Boston: Thomson/Heinle.
Gippert, J. (1996). Die historische Entwicklung der Zaza-Sprache [The histor-
ical development of the Zaza language]. Ware. Pêseroka Zon u Kulturê Ma:
Dımıli-Kırmanc-Zaza, 10, 148-154.
Gooskens, C. 2007. The contribution of linguistic factors to the intelligibility
of closely related languages. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Develop-
ment, 28(6), 445-467.
Gündoğdu, S. (2011). The phrase structure of two Dialects of Kurmanji Kurdish:
Standard Dialect and Muş Dialect. (Unpublished MA Thesis). Boğaziçi Uni-
versity, Turkey.
Gündoğdu, S. (forthcoming). Case and Ergativity in Kurmanji. Dilbilim
Araştırmaları.
Hadank, K. and Mann, O. (1932). Kurdisch-Persische Forschungen, Abteilung III, B
and II: Mundarten der Gûrân, besonders das Kändûläî, Auramânî und
Bâdschaälânî, bearbeitet von Karl Hadank. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Haig, G. (2001) Linguistic Diffusion in Present-day East Anatolia: From Top 119
to Bottom. In A. Aikhenvald and R. M. W. Dixon (eds.), Areal diffusion
and Genetic Inheritance. Problems in Comparative Linguistics (195-224). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Haig, G. (2002). Complex predicates in Kurdish: Argument sharing, incorpo-
ration, or what? Sprachtypologie Und Universalienforschung / Language Typology
and Universals, 55 (1), 15–48.
Haig, G. (2004a). Alignment in Kurdish. A Diachronic Perspective. Habilita-
tionsschrift. Christian-Albrechts-Universität zu Kiel, Germany.
Haig, G. (2004b). The invisibilisation of Kurdish: The Other Side of Language
Planning in Turkey. In S. Conermann and G. Haig (eds.) Die Kurden. Stu-
dien zu ihrer Sprache, Geschichte und Kultur (121-150). Hamburg: EB Verlag.
Haig, G. (2006). Turkish Influence on Kurmanji: Evidence from the Tunceli
Dialect. In L. Johanson and C. Bulut (eds.), Turkic-Iranian contact areas.
Historical and linguistic aspects. (283–299). Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Haig, G. (2007). Grammatical Borrowing in Kurdish (Northern Group). In Y.
Matras and J. Sakel (eds.), Grammatical Borrowing in Cross-linguistic Perspective
(165–184). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Haig, G. (2008) Alignment Change in Iranian Languages. A Construction Grammar
Approach. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Haig, G. (2011). Linker, Relativizer, Nominalizer, Tense-particle: On the
Ezafe in West Iranian. In F. Yap, K. Grunow-Hårsta, and J. Wrona
(eds.), Nominalization in Asian Languages: Diachronic and Typological Perspec-
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
tives Volume 1: Sino-Tibetan and Iranian Languages (363-390). Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Haig, G. (2012). Quelques remarques sur la politique linguistique au Kurdistan
d’Irak et ses répercussions l’unité culturelle des Kurdes. Etudes Kurdes, XI,
123–127.
Haig, G. (In print). East Anatolia as a Linguistic Area? Empirical and Concep-
tual Issues. In L. Behzadi, P. Franke, G. Haig, C. Herzog, B. Hoffmann,
L. Korn & S. Talabardon (eds.), Bamberger Orientstudien. Bamberg: Bam-
berg University Press.
Haig, G. (forthcoming a). East Anatolia. Language Contact in a Transition
Zone. In R. Hickey (ed.), Cambridge Handbook of Areal Linguistics. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Haig, G. (forthcoming b.) Deconstructing Iranian Ergativity. In J. Coon and
L. Travis (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Ergativity. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Haig, G. and Matras, Y. (2002). Kurdish linguistics: a brief overview. Sprachty-
pologie Und Universalienforschung / Language Typology and Universals, 55(1), 3–
14.
Haig, G. and Öpengin, E. (in print). Gender in Kurdish. Structural and Socio-
cultural Aspects. In M. Hellinger and H. Motschenbacher (eds.), Gender
Across Languages, Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Haig, G. and Öpengin, E. (forthcoming). Kurmanji Kurdish in Turkey: Struc-
120 ture, Varieties and Status. In C. Bulut (ed.), The Minority Languages in Tur-
key. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Hassanpour, A. (1992). Nationalism and language in Kurdistan, 1918-1985. San
Francisco: Mellen Research University Press.
Hassanpour, A. (2001). The (Re)production of Patriarchy in Kurdish Lan-
guage. In S. Mojab (ed.), The Women of a Non-state Country: The Kurds (227–
63). Costa Mesa, CA: Mazda.
Hassanpour, A. (2012). The indivisibility of the nation and its linguistic divi-
sions. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 217, 49–73.
Izady, M. (1992). The Kurds: A Concise Handbook. London: Taylor & Francis.
Karimi, Y. (2007). Kurdish Ezafe constructions: Implications for DP struc-
ture. Lingua, 117, 2159-2177.
Karimi, Y. (2010). Unaccusative Transitives and The Person-Case Constraint
Effects in Kurdish. Lingua, 120, 693-716.
Karimi-Doostan, G. (2005). Light verbs and structural case. Lingua, 115, 1737-
1756.
Keskin, M. (2010). Zazaca üzerine notlar. In Ş. Arslan (ed.), Herkesin Bildiği Sır:
Dersim - Tarih, Toplum, Ekonomi, Dil ve Kültür (221-242). Istanbul: İletişim.
Khan, G. (2007). Grammatical Borrowing in North-Eastern Neo-Aramaic. In
Y. Matras and J. Sakel (eds.), Grammatical Borrowing in Cross-Linguistic Per-
spective (197-214). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Korn, A. (2005). Towards a Historical Grammar of Balochi. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Leezenberg, M. (2014). Elî Teremaxî and the Vernacularization of Medrese
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London
HAIG & ÖPENGIN
Learning in Kurdistan. Iranian Studies, 47(5), 713–733.
Linell, P. (2005). The Written Language Bias in Linguistics. London: Routledge.
Lynch, J., Ross, M. and Crowley, T. (2011). The Oceanic Languages. London:
Routledge
MacKenzie, D. N. (1961). Kurdish Dialect Studies Vol. I. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Mahalingappa, L. (2013). The Acquisition of Split-ergative Case Marking in
Kurmanji Kurdish. In B. Edith L. and S. Stoll (eds.), The Acquisition of Er-
gativity (239-270). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Mahmoudveysi, P. and Bailey, D. (2013). The Gorani Language of Zarda, a Village
of West Iran. Texts, Grammar and Lexicon. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Mahmoudveysi, P., Bailey, D., Paul, L., and Haig, G. (2012). The Gorani Lan-
guage of Gawraju, a Village of West Iran. Grammar, Texts and Lexicon. Wies-
baden: Reichert.
Matras, Y. (2002). Kurmanjî complementation: Semantic-typological aspects
in an areal perspective. Sprachtypologie Und Universalienforschung / Language
Typology and Universals, 55(1), 49–63.
Matras, Y. (2007). The Borrowability of Structural Categories. In Y. Matras
and J. Sakel (eds.), Grammatical Borrowing in Cross-linguistic Perspective (31–
73). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Matras, Y. (2010). Contact, Convergence, and Typology. In R. Hickey (ed.),
The Handbook of Contact Linguistics (66–85). Malden/Oxford: Blackwell.
Matras, Y. (2011). Explaining Convergence and the Formation of Linguistic 121
Areas. In H. Osama, C. König and H. Nakagawa (eds.), Geographical Ty-
pology and Linguistic Areas (143-160). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
McCarus, E. N. (2009). Kurdish. In G. Windfuhr (ed.), The Iranian Languages
(587–633). London: Routledge.
Mohamed, B. (2014). Code-switching: A Case Study of Kurdish-German Pre-school
Bilingual Children. Frankfurt: Lang.
Öpengin, E. (2012a). Sociolinguistic situation of Kurdish in Turkey: Sociopo-
litical factors and language use patterns. International Journal of the Sociology
of Language, 217, 151–180.
Öpengin, E. (2012b). Adpositions and Argument Indexing in the Mukri Va-
riety of Central Kurdish: Focus on Ditransitive Constructions. Orientalia
Suecana, 61, 187–198.
Öpengin, E. (2013). Clitic/affix interactions: a corpus-based study of the person mark-
ing in the Mukri variety of Central Kurdish. (Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion). Université Sorbonne Nouvelle and Universität Bamberg.
Öpengin, E. (forthcoming). The Mukri Variety of Central Kurdish: Grammar,
Texts, Lexicon. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Özoğlu, H. (2004). Kurdish Notables and the Ottoman State. Albany: State Univer-
sity of New York Press.
Özsoy, A. S. and Türkyilmaz, Y. (2006). Front Rounded Vowels in the
Sinemili Dialect of Kurmanji - a Case of Language Contact? In L. Johan-
son and C. Bulut (eds.), Turkic-Iranian Contact Areas Historical and Linguistic
© Kurdish Studies
KURDISH: A CRITICAL RESEARCH OVERVIEW
Aspects (300–309). Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz
Paul, L. (1998). Zazaki. Grammatik und Versuch einer Dialektologie. Wiesbaden:
Reichert.
Paul, L. (2009). Zazaki. In G. Windfuhr (ed.), The Iranian Languages (545–586).
London: Routledge.
Romaine, S. (2000). Language in Society: An Introduction to Sociolinguistics (2nd ed.).
Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.
Samvelian, P. (2006). A Lexicalist Account of Sorani Kurdish Prepositions. In
S. Müller (ed.), Proceedings of the HPSG07 Conference. Stanford: CSLI Publi-
cations.
Samvelian, P. (2007). What Sorani Kurdish Absolute Prepositions Tell Us
about Cliticization. In Texas Linguistic Society IX: The Morphosyntax of Un-
derrepresented Languages (263–283). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Selcan, Z. (1998). Grammatik der Zaza-Sprache. Nord Dialekt (Dersim-Dialekt),
Berlin: Wissenschaft & Technik.
Sheyholislami, J., Hassanpour, A. and Skuttnab-Kangas, T. (eds.) (2012). Spe-
cial Issue. The Kurdish Linguistic Landscape: Vitality, Linguicide and
Resistance. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 217.
Sheyholislami, J. (2012). Kurdish in Iran: A case of restricted and controlled
tolerance. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 217, 19–47.
Sperber, D. and Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
Talay, S. (2006/2007.) The Influence of Turkish, Kurdish and Other Neigh-
122 bouring Languages on Anatolian Arabic. In G. Grigoro (ed.), Peripheral
Arabic Dialects (179-188). Bucharest: Centre for Arab Studies.
Thackston, W. M. (n.d.). Sorani Kurdish: A reference grammar with selected
readings. Retrieved from: http://www.fas.harvard.edu/\~iranian/ So-
rani/sorani_1_grammar. pdf (Accessed: 21 November, 2013)
Thackston, W. M. (n.d.). Kurmanji Kurdish: A reference grammar with select-
ed readings. Retrieved from: http://www.fas.harvard.edu/
~iranian/Kurmanji/ (Accessed: 21 November, 2013)
Trudgill, P. (2000). Sociolinguistics: An Introduction to Language and Society (4th
ed.). London; New York: Penguin.
Unger, C. (2012). Procedural semantics, metarepresentation, and some parti-
cles in Behdini Kurdish. Lingua, 122(14), 1613-1635.
Wang, W. (1997). Languages or Dialects? CUHK Journal of Humanities, 1, 54–
62.
Zeydanlıoğlu, W. (2012). Turkey’s Kurdish language policy. International Journal
of the Sociology of Language, 217, 99–125.
Zilfî. (1983). Folklorê kurdî ebe zaravê dimilkî. Hêvî, 1, 67-123.
www.kurdishstudies.net Transnational Press London