Well-Being, Poverty and Justice From A Child's Perspective
Well-Being, Poverty and Justice From A Child's Perspective
Sabine Andresen
Susann Fegter
Klaus Hurrelmann
Ulrich Schneekloth Editors
Well-being,
Poverty and
Justice from a
Child’s Perspective
3rd World Vision Children Study
Children’s Well-Being: Indicators and Research
Volume 17
Series Editor:
ASHER BEN-ARIEH
Paul Baerwald School of Social Work & Social Welfare, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Editorial Board:
Translated by Jonathan Harrow
Originally published in German language: 3. World Vision Kinderstudie. Wie gerecht ist
unsere Welt? by Sabine Andresen et al. (Eds.) Copyright © 2013 Beltz Verlag. Translated into
the English language: Well-being, Poverty and Justice from a Child’s Perspective: 3rd World
Vision Children Study by Sabine Andresen et al. (Eds.) Copyright © 2017 Springer
International Publishing Switzerland. Springer International Publishing AG is part of
Springer Science+Business Media All Rights Reserved.
Once again, the researchers carrying out the Third World Vision Child Study have
entered uncharted territory and opened up previously untapped areas for childhood
studies. This particularly includes the focus on “justice”—a topic that research in
this field is beginning to address only recently. The first two World Vision Child
Studies (2007 and 2010) already showed how closely children associate well-being
with justice. Even younger children reveal a marked sense of justice. However, they
also link justice closely to equal opportunity and equality of treatment—as the pres-
ent study also shows (see Chap. 2).
The Third World Vision Child Study focuses once again on the subjective well-
being of children and thereby the world they experience. Well-being is more than
just safeguarding children’s rights and being guided by the best interests of the child
as understood in legal terms. It is a multidimensional concept containing both objec-
tive and subjective criteria. By asking 6- to 11-year-old children to give their own
subjective appraisals of their well-being, the child study confirms once again that
children are able to give competent and authentic accounts of their own life situa-
tion; they are the specialists on their own lives. In addition, the study grants children
in Germany a voice that deserves to be heard—not only by parents and teachers but
also by academics and politicians. Children need child-oriented surroundings and a
child-oriented society.
As the results of the quantitative and qualitative surveys in this study show, child
well-being has much to do with participation in society and how far children are
able to participate and to develop their capabilities. The Second World Vision Child
Study (2010) already placed great value on the concept of self-efficacy, showing
how this gains in strength the more children are able to codetermine the environ-
ment in which they live. However, the precondition of codetermination is for par-
ents (and other persons involved in rearing and educating children) to take children
seriously, to listen to them attentively and honestly, and to devote enough time to
them. Here, the study shows once again that when parents have to go to work, this
does not necessarily impede the quality of the time and care they devote to their
children.
v
vi Foreword from World Vision Deutschland e.V.
Well-being also involves a fine balance between care and freedom. The children
tell us that too many regulations and prohibitions can constrain their well-being.
However, the granting of freedom to children can only develop its full potential
when they also have the chance to use the existing possibilities to shape their own
lives. Here is where we so clearly see that we are living in a four-fifths society in
Germany—one in which most children are growing up comparatively contentedly,
but one-fifth are living in precarious circumstances and have only very limited pos-
sibilities of participating in society.
Poverty and the risks of poverty, the Third World Vision Child Study reveals once
again, remain an urgent social problem in Germany. However, child poverty is a
multifaceted phenomenon: It is characterized by the constrained financial possibili-
ties of the parents, by their ability or inability to get by with the money available to
them, and, above all, by limited participation in social and cultural activities.
The risks that can lead to child poverty include unemployment, single parent-
hood, a migration background, fear of failure, and parents who lack an educational
background and educational aspirations—or a combination of these factors.
However, how do children perceive their own poverty? How do they deal with it?
How do they cope with poverty and the other constraints to their well-being? When
asked, most children place themselves in the middle between poor and rich and
indicate that they are fully aware of their responsibility for their own lives. However,
whereas some children seem to have an almost fatalistic outlook and simply accept
their fate of having limited opportunities, others display an amazing resistance to
everyday strains and obstacles. The secret of this resilience has yet to be worked out
in full.
The World Vision Child Study starts off by reviewing the situation of children in
Germany. However, it also aims to go beyond just a review: It should grant the chil-
dren a voice, give parents and teachers new findings to work with, and point to fields
in which policymakers can actively strive to create a society that is friendlier to
children. To underline clearly what policymakers need to address, the authors of the
study have once again devoted a special chapter to this topic under the heading
“Policy for Children” (see Chap. 9). Because poverty and disadvantage affect all
areas of life, the challenge for policymakers is to counter the growing social divi-
sions in our society. Poverty is always relative, because exclusion and limited par-
ticipation are generally perceived to be more painful than mere financial
constraints.
When a children’s charity such as World Vision, which has predominantly taken
up the fight against global poverty, once again addresses the subjective well-being
of children in Germany, this is in no way a sign of inconsistency. First of all, in our
developmental work throughout the world, the principle of child well-being has
become increasingly the decisive criterion for measuring the efficacy of our pro-
grams, and, second, the poverty that we have committed ourselves to fight does not
respect national borders. The gap between poor and rich no longer runs along the
divide between north and south but right through the middle of all societies, and that
includes Germany.
Foreword from World Vision Deutschland e.V. vii
When asked about their well-being, the great majority of children in Germany give
positive or even very positive answers. This reflects the security and care they expe-
rience within their own families and the active ways in which their parents attend to
their needs. One central aspect in the eyes of the 6- to 11-year-old children in our
study is to have their opinions taken seriously and not to be ignored. Other essential
aspects for their well-being are their circle of friends, being able to organize their
leisure time by themselves, and having a variety of leisure-time pursuits. These are
the areas in which children absorb important (informal) learning incentives that
extend and substantially supplement the formal education processes they experi-
ence at school.
One finding that also stands out in this new 2013 World Vision Child Study is
that the social origins of children continue to be a recurring theme in their life situ-
ations and the accompanying chances for them to participate in society. The results
of the latest 2013 Child Study indicate that it is above all those children coming
from the lowest social class1 who are being broadly left behind. These children are
unable to participate adequately in the wide range of services and opportunities
available in Germany. Instead, social risks accumulate in their daily lives, and, as a
consequence, they are denied their chances of development, capabilities, and
thereby life perspectives.
In this study, social class is used in a similar way to socioeconomic status. Our use of this term
1
ix
x Summary
One of the focuses in this Third World Vision Child Study is on the topic of justice.
Children orient themselves strongly toward their need for equality and equality of
treatment when they express their sense of justice. This includes not only personal
fairness when dealing with others but also being sure that there are equal chances
for everybody to join in. This is why they think that goods and opportunities of
access should be shared out among themselves. Depending on the situation they are
in, children initially favor the principle of equal distribution (equality) in their daily
lives, although, generally speaking, only when this is simultaneously accompanied
by respect for the principle of reciprocity. If this is not the case, for example, when
a few children do not stick to the rules or do not make any contribution themselves,
then they also evaluate this as unjust and intolerable. The sense of justice developed
by children in this age range has a clearly recognizable altruistic dimension and
serves not only their own interests but also the well-being of others. It is interesting
to see how the views of children and their desire for justice link up with the debate
on social justice. Even in the “grown-up” world, the final concern is “that we share
with each other and don’t leave anybody just standing there” (girl aged 7 years).
More than 90% of the children give positive or even very positive reports on their
personal life satisfaction. Indeed, satisfaction even seems to be showing a slight
increase. In contrast, 28% of the children from the lowest social class give ratings
ranging from negative to neutral and trail notably behind this positive appraisal.
About 90% of children rate the social interaction within their own family and circle
of friends as being just, and almost 80% consider social interaction at school to be
just as well. Almost 50% consider Germany as a whole to be just, but only 16%
think that the world is just. Slightly more than one-quarter of the children were
unable to give any answer to these last two questions.
Summary xi
Family and Background
As a trend, children continue to complain most frequently about a lack of care and
attention (“Both parents do not have enough time or one parent does not have
enough time; the other parent, sometimes yes and sometimes no”) when they have
either a working single parent (2013, 32%; 2010, 31%; 2007, 35%) or a parent who
is unemployed or not working for other reasons (2013, 29%; 2010, 30%; 2007,
29%). However, the proportion of children who lack care and attention when both
parents are employed either part- or full-time has remained constant at 8%.
Parents’ estimates do not differ from those of their children. Reconciling family
and work is most difficult for working single parents (49% just about okay or less
well) and also for parents who are unemployed or not employed for other reasons
(46% just about okay or less well). Relatively speaking, compatibility is considered
to work best in families in which both parents go to work either full- or part-time
(27% just about okay or less well).
Migration Background
A total of 34% of the children (aged 6–11 years) have a migration background. The
majority of these children also have German citizenship and were born in Germany.
A migration background is found in 59% of children from the lower class and also
a slightly disproportionally high proportion of 41% of children in the lower middle
class. Hence, children with a migration background continue to come dispropor-
tionately frequently from families of origin with little education. The lower the edu-
cation in the class of origin, then the likelier it is that the children from migrant
families will not speak German at home. Our results showed that 33% of the chil-
dren with a migration background speak predominantly their parents’ mother
tongue at home (2010, 33%; 2007, 35%), and here, it is 43% of the children from
the lower classes of origin compared to 26% from the upper classes of origin.
School
Since the first survey in 2007, the proportion of children attending an all-day school
or class has grown markedly. Among the 8- to 11-year-old children, it has almost
doubled from 13% in 2007 to 23% in 2013. However, the analyses show that even
today, whether a family has a realistic chance of deciding freely in favor of a place
in an all-day school depends decisively on where that family lives. In Eastern
Germany, almost twice as many children report attending an all-day school in 2013
compared to children in Western Germany (39% vs. 21%).
According to their own reports, the number of lower-class children taking advan-
tage of all-day school provisions is above average at 31%, whereas only one in
every five upper-class children spends the whole day at school. In all, we can deter-
mine an increasing number of all-day students in all social origin groups in the
World Vision Child Studies since 2007. A total of 61% of all-day students are
2
All children attend elementary school together until the end of fourth grade. On the secondary
level (mostly from fifth grade onward), the school system follows three tracks: Hauptschule,
Realschule, and Gymnasium. The Gymnasium leads to the highest school-leaving certificate, the
Abitur. Some of the German states (or Länder) have merged the Hauptschule and Realschule
together. There are also integrated comprehensive schools.
Summary xv
satisfied with this type of school. However, this is the case for only 41% of lower-
class children.
Child participation at school differs in its degree and frequency. A total of 40% of
all 6- to 11-year-old children do not feel that they have any say in the seven areas we
asked them about and feel that they are unable to often have their say in any of these
areas. Boys have this impression far more frequently than girls (44% vs. 35%) and,
at 57%, 6- to 7-year-old children much more than 10- to 12-year-olds. We already
asked about some areas of child participation in the first child study in 2007, and
there have been hardly any changes since then.
A very large majority of the children surveyed judge their school to be “very fair”
(22%) or “quite fair” (56%). Negative reports of “rather unfair” (1%) and “very
unfair” (3%) are very rare. However, the sense of justice relates closely to the degree
of participation possibilities in the school.
The children’s sense of justice also reveals a close relation to general satisfaction
with school. Students who find their school to be rather or very unfair are less satis-
fied with their school in general and vice versa.
A total of 79% of the children talk about their school positively. Compared to
2010, this satisfaction score has increased by 9% and is particularly strong in ele-
mentary school children.
Leisure Time
From 40% to 50% of 6- to 11-year-old computer users play games on their com-
puter or game console several times a week; about 15% report playing every day,
and 30% “not very often” spend their free time playing with computers and game
consoles. The trend over the last 6 years indicates that the frequency of use within
different age groups has remained broadly stable.
xvi Summary
As reported already in the last study, girls belong more than three times as often
(38%) to the group of all-rounders than boys (12%). Vice versa, boys spend at least
three times more of their leisure time (39%) as media consumers than girls (13%).
Whereas the latter tend more toward the creative-musical field and have a stronger
interest in theater, ballet, and dance as well as reading, their male peers continue to
engage particularly frequently in passive media-related activities such as watching
television or playing computer games.
Since the First World Vision Child Study, the frequency of television consumption
has declined somewhat across all 8- to 11-year-old children. Whereas in 2007, 56%
reported watching television very often, this applied to only 48% in 2010 and 49%
in 2013. Nonetheless, a more marked decline can be seen above all in the all-
rounders from 24% in 2007 to 12% in 2013. In the group of media consumers,
television is one of the most frequent leisure-time pursuits for 80–90% of these
children. And whereas the reported amount of viewing has declined in all-rounders,
it has increased in media consumers.
The use of computer and video games proves to be largely constant across all
three child studies (25–30%). However, it differs across various leisure types.
Among the all-rounders, the proportion of frequent players has sunk from an already
low starting level of 10% in 2007 to 2% in 2013, whereas about 60% of each group
of media consumers report playing computer games very frequently.
The proportion of children who read books frequently has declined slightly as a
whole. This finding applies to all three leisure types. Currently, 31% of children
read very often (2010, 37%; 2007, 34%). However, in the group of media consum-
ers, only 8% read very often in 2013 compared to 61% in the group of
all-rounders.
According to parents’ reports, the proportion of children who belong to at least one
club or organized group as well as other provisions (e.g., music school or cultural
center) is very high in the upper and upper middle class where it ranges from
approximately 80% to over 90%. Middle-class memberships are also constant.
Approximately three-quarters of the children from this social class and roughly
Summary xvii
two-thirds of children from the lower middle class possess at least one membership.
In contrast, the proportion of children from the lower class who are members of a
club or organized group has remained below 50% in all three child studies and is
also subject to strong fluctuations.
The trend indicates a conspicuous decline in memberships of clubs, groups, or
musical and cultural provisions among 6- to 7-year-olds. This applies particularly to
children from the lower classes. In general, children from the lowest social class still
continue to be able to participate to a markedly lesser extent in institutionally orga-
nized leisure-time activities, and, at this stage, no change in this trend can be
ascertained.
The trend toward increasingly more children in this age range having a mobile
phone already reported in 2010 has continued in 2013. In 2007, only 17% of 8- to
9-year-olds and 56% of 10- to 11-year-olds had a mobile telephone; in 2010, this
rose to 27% and 66%, respectively, and, in the current study, to 31% of 8- to 9-year-
olds and 72% of 10- to 11-year-olds. However, even when the proportion of those
with a mobile telephone is increasing, boys have not caught up with girls. At 56%,
8- to 11-year-old girls continue to more frequently have their own mobile telephone
xviii Summary
than boys at 49% (across all age groups together, 42% of the girls and 37% of the
boys have a mobile telephone). Another finding that has remained constant since
2007 is that children in single-parent households more frequently have their own
mobile telephone (62%) than children in other families (50%).
Friends
Compared to the two previous child studies, there has been a marked decline in the
frequency of personal meetings with friends during leisure time. In both 2010 and
2007, 68% of the children surveyed reported meeting their friends very often,
whereas in 2013, this is only 53%. However, this has no effect on the size of the
circle of friends. In all three child studies, approximately 40% of 8- to 11-year-olds
report having a large circle of friends containing 10 or more peers. Roughly one-
half of the children have between four and nine friends, whereas slightly more than
10% report having only a small circle of friends or none at all.
Across all three child studies, we can see that media consumers have fewer friends
than normal leisure-time users or all-rounders. Only about one-third of this group
report having 10 or more friends compared to roughly 35–40% in the other two
groups. In line with this, all three child studies show that among 8- to 11-year-old
Summary xix
media consumers, the proportion that have only a small circle of friends containing
three or less children is higher (17–21%) than that among normal leisure-time users
(12–14%) or all-rounders (7–11%).
Children aged 6–11 years most frequently meet their friends at school (78% just
about every day during the week) or outside (17% just about every day and 50%
several times a week). Online, 3% of all children meet their friends just about every
day (among the 10- to 11-year-olds, 6%), and a further 6% meet their friends online
several times a week (among the 10- to 11-year-olds, 14%). In comparison, 2% of
all children meet their friends just about every day in a club and a further 41% sev-
eral times a week there.
Among the 8- to 11-year-olds in the survey, the use of chat lines has lost its popular-
ity since 2007. Probably the reason for this decline is not that children are less
interested in using the Internet for social contacts but the increasing popularity of
other social networks such as Facebook. These are already used by a total of 15% of
the oldest respondents (10- to 11-year-olds).
The majority of children see themselves as being well regarded in their circle of
friends, and this positive impression has increased since 2007. In the first child
study, 43% of the 8- to 11-year-olds reported that their opinion was quite well
regarded in their circle of friends. In 2010, this rose to 45% of 8- to 11-year-olds,
and in 2013, it is now 53%.
xx Summary
Children have different opportunities to shape their daily lives for themselves. The
most possibilities of codetermination are given to them in the family and have to do
with their leisure time and their circle of friends. Here—depending on age—almost
90% of children themselves decide which friends they meet and what they do in
their leisure time. On the other hand, two out of every three children are told when
to do their homework by their parents, and every second child cannot decide for her
or his self how many other children she or he may bring back home.
Children from the lower class have the least possibilities of self-determination in
daily life. Only three out of every four children, and thereby far fewer than the aver-
age, can themselves decide which friends they want to meet, and also only two out
of every three children report that they themselves can decide what to do in their
leisure time. Another restriction to the possibilities of self-determination is deficits
in care. Having parents with little time for their children is accompanied, from the
child’s perspective, by stricter rules and regulations in daily life. Independent of
this, children with a migration background—regardless of gender—report being
subject to comparatively stricter regulation. The same applies, though to a markedly
lesser extent, to children from families with lots of children.
Some of these limitations relate to a lack of household resources. On the other
hand, (class-specific) differences in child-rearing styles also manifest here.
As a trend, children’s reports indicate an increase in how far their personal opinions
are taken seriously in the family and also in the circle of friends. Currently, 60%
report that their mothers take their personal opinions rather more seriously (2010,
57%; 2007, 56%). Among fathers, 49% value the opinions of their children rather
more seriously (2010, 48%; 2007, 47). About one-quarter of the children replied
with “sometimes one, sometimes the other” and about 10% (9% for the mother and
13% for the father) with “generally less”; the rest gave no reports on this. Regarding
the circle of friends, a rise to 51% of the children reported that their opinions were
taken more seriously (2010, 42%; 2007, 43%).
In contrast, only 29% of the children still reported that their class teachers tended
to take the children’s opinion more seriously. In 2010, this was 32% and, in 2007,
26%. Findings are similar for children attending a care institution such as a daycare
center. According to the children’s reports, 33% take the children’s opinion more
seriously (2010, 36%; 2007, 24%).
Once again, effects of class of origin emerge here. A total of 16% of lower-class
children report that their mothers do not take them seriously, and even 31% give
explicitly negative reports on their fathers. It is also notable that 19% of the children
with a migration background compared to 10% of the native German children eval-
uate being taken seriously by their fathers negatively, whereas there are no signifi-
cant differences regarding reports on the mothers. Here as well, this underlines the
significance of the parental child-rearing style.
The Third World Vision Child Study surveyed a nationally representative sample of
2535 children aged 6–11 years. The children were assessed personally through oral
surveys in their homes. These surveys took an average of 35 min. At the same time,
one parent was also asked to provide supplementary information on the origins and
social situation of the family. The qualitative part of the study was based on 12
interviews with children in this age range lasting between 1.5 and 3 h. Detailed
accounts of the children interviewed in the qualitative part are presented as indi-
vidual portraits.
Contents
Appendices�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 179
Index������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 227
xxiii
Chapter 1
How Children See Well-Being, Poverty,
and Justice: The Focus of the Third World
Vision Child Study
1.1 T
he World Vision Child Studies and International
Childhood Research
The World Vision Child Studies have just one basic philosophy: to give a voice to
children in Germany. These studies view children as the experts on the world they
live in: on their feelings, opinions, and experiences. Of course, it goes without say-
ing that mothers and fathers, brothers and sisters, grandparents, childcare workers,
sports coaches, and teachers are all experts as well, and they can tell us a lot of what
they know about children and childhood. However, it is now widely accepted that
the adult perspective on childhood and adult knowledge about children should not
be taken to be all-powerful and that it is important to view the children themselves
as informants and interpreters of childhood and being a child.
The First World Vision Child Study in 2007 presented and substantiated this
theoretical and methodological approach in some detail (Andresen and Hurrelmann
2007). One of the findings emphasized in 2007 has had a notable impact both in
Germany and abroad: the notable gaps the study revealed in what we know about
middle childhood, that is, children between the ages of 6 and 11 years. The World
S. Andresen (*)
Department of Education, Institute of Social Pedagogy and Adult Education,
Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
S. Fegter
Department of Humanities, Institute of Educational Sciences,
Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
K. Hurrelmann
Hertie School of Governance, Berlin, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
Vision Child Studies have contributed to closing these gaps. Together with other
studies in Germany, above all, the Kinder-Panel and the AID:A Studie (both from
the German Youth Institute), the KIGGS childhood health study (Robert Koch
Institute), and the Robert Bosch Stiftung’s (2012) study on child well-being, we
now have a range of well-designed surveys addressing this age group.
What is the state of international childhood studies in 2013 and how does the
Third World Vision Child Study relate to this research? These are the questions we
want to address in this introductory chapter. We shall start by positioning our
approach within national and international research on child well-being. Although
we have been looking at child well-being continuously ever since our First Study,
there have been further developments in this field since the Second Study in 2010.
These are presented in Sect. 1.2 and continue the theoretical discussion to be found
in the Second Study (Andresen et al. 2010).
A further important topic is the situation of children living in poverty. The First
World Vision Child Study in 2007 highlighted the effects of child poverty in
Germany. Our findings attracted a lot of public attention and led many people to ask
how the children themselves experience it. When we started to plan the Second
Study, such reactions encouraged us to focus more strongly on how children living
in Germany actually perceive poverty themselves. In the Third Study presented
here, we also deal intensively with the social differences that children experience in
Germany and take a closer look at the effects of poverty from the children’s perspec-
tive. This is another field in which there have been advances in the discussion in
research, politics, and education since 2010. In Sect. 1.3, we shall discuss recent
poverty research in detail.
It is particularly children from families with a migration background along with
those whose parents have low educational qualifications who are subject to poverty.
Therefore, in Sect. 1.4, we shall consider what has now become an established sta-
tistical concept in Germany: a migration background. This concept, which offers an
improved way of distinguishing the diverse experiences of migration to be found in
the population of Germany, is also important when surveying children. Many a child
who is assigned a migration background has been born in Germany and often has
German nationality. Nonetheless, it is assumed that, for example, the experiences of
parents or grandparents as immigrants can have specific effects on their child born
in Germany. We examine these relationships while also considering the interna-
tional discussion on these topics. This should make poverty and migration visible as
possible experiences of children and as a part of their family history.
The Third World Vision Child Study also broadens the range of the two previous
studies and examines the children’s sense of justice. What children require for a
good and thereby a just life is certainly something they frequently think about them-
selves. In our previous studies, we noticed that whenever we asked children about
their values or to tell us what a good life means for children regardless of where they
are growing up, their answers always addressed the core of the debates to be found
in justice theory. Children want to have their say here, and they have something to
say. In the Third Study, we explicitly address the topic of justice and report on the
children’s sense of justice and how they judge injustice and inequality. This opens
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 3
1.2 W
ell-Being as a Concept in Childhood Studies and its
Application in the Third World Vision Child Study
Like the past two studies in 2007 and 2010, the present study addresses the concept
of well-being. Although it also integrates new discussions and findings in this field,
our basic understanding remains unchanged: Once again, we are interested in find-
ing out about not only the subjective appraisals of our respondents but also their
specific social framing conditions. Having now carried out three studies in a row, we
are also in a position to compare the well-being and satisfaction of children in
Germany across time. One finding we can report straight away: In 2013, just as in
the two prior studies, overall well-being continues to be mostly very high.
Nonetheless, those children who are exposed to poverty frequently belong to the
group whose well-being is limited.
The concept of well-being does not just link up to academic issues but also to
political and educational practice. In general, one can define well-being by saying
that it stands for a comprehensive understanding of the qualities of a child’s life situ-
ation, welfare, and participation in society. Hence, well-being goes far beyond the
“best interests of the child.” This latter concept focuses predominantly on protecting
children, whereas well-being also includes children’s rights and thereby their par-
ticipation in society along with their capabilities. This shifts attention to children as
subjects with their own rights. As such, children do, of course, depend on the care,
protection, and education provided by adults, but they also have their own autono-
mous status. A further trend revealed by the present study illustrates this clearly: In
2013, as in the previous studies, one major aspect that continues to contribute to the
well-being of children is a measured balance between care and freedom in everyday
life (Andresen et al. 2012).
In general, research on child well-being is embedded in a tradition of striving to
make societies more child-friendly.
assigned to each dimension serve to specify and measure well-being. For example,
one possible indicator for education is the proportion of 15-year-olds attending the
highest level of secondary school in a nation. Hence, this refers to the objective
framing conditions of childhood in a nation and it considers who profits in what way
from, for example, the given educational provisions.
Only recently, two well-being researchers in the United States, William O’Hare
and Florencia Gutierrez (2012), collected and examined all available studies so far
that have applied an index of well-being. Their overview reveals the growing inter-
est in constructing dimensions and indicators of child well-being. Nonetheless, the
authors also showed that the concept is used in a broad range of ways. Indeed, just
about every research team has put together its own different set of dimensions and
indicators.
One reason for this heterogeneity is the political interest in obtaining practical
facts and statements. Important actors here are not only the major international
organizations such as the United Nations and its subsidiary organization UNICEF,
the World Health Organization (WHO), and the Organization for Economic
Cooperation and Development (OECD) but also national foundations. They fre-
quently work with comprehensive instruments to measure well-being. For example,
since 2007, UNICEF has been carrying out regular studies of the well-being of
children in 29 wealthy industrialized nations. In addition, UNICEF’s annual report
on the State of the World’s Children also addresses aspects of well-being in all
regions of the world. In 2013, for instance, it focused on children with disabilities
(UNICEF 2013). Very recently, UNICEF presented new data on well-being in
wealthy societies in its Report Card 11. These findings were based on an analysis of
overall satisfaction in 11-, 13-, and 15-year-old children in studies carried out by the
“Health Behaviour in School-aged Children” (HBSC) team.
The approach taken in the UNICEF study on the well-being of children in
wealthy countries has had many imitators. These have formulated dimensions of
well-being and developed indicators to measure it. Nowadays, many researchers are
also focusing on the dimension of subjective well-being, because results have shown
the need to pay attention to not only objective social framing conditions but also the
way they are perceived subjectively—especially when studying children.
One major advance has been the intensive work on determining the role of the
important dimension of subjective well-being and how it should be measured. This
has also drawn on established psychological research into, for example, the “quality
of life.” There are now many studies addressing the assessment of subjective well-
being. The UNICEF studies also work with this dimension, and they have analyzed
it in depth in their latest Report Card 11. This uses an index to assess subjective
well-being as broadly as possible. The index covers overall life satisfaction, close
relationships with parents and peers, general well-being at school, and subjective
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 5
health reports. In the near future, there will certainly be a need for further studies,
including international comparisons, in order to further clarify the state of subjec-
tive well-being in children (Bradshaw et al. 2013).
In this context, we should also mention the qualitative studies on the subjective
ideas of children, because we also drew on these in the Second World Vision Child
Study. One example of this is the research carried out in Australia by Tobia Fattore
and his colleagues (Fattore et al. 2012). This research team asked children to report
which areas of life they considered to be most important for well-being. They iden-
tified three areas: self, agency, and security. Self-refers essentially to the children’s
self-esteem, that is, their appraisal of themselves as good and valuable personalities.
Agency assesses how far children feel that they have control over their own lives
and the self-efficacy of their actions. Finally, security describes their feeling of
being secure and in good hands in their relationships with their parents and other
adults while nonetheless having sufficient scope when it comes to doing what they
themselves want to do.
1.2.3 D
eveloping and Testing Comprehensive Composite
Indices
In the United States, a Child Development Index (CDI) has been derived from
the Human Development Index (HDI) used by the United Nations. The annual
results published by a foundation receive a great deal of attention. They permit a
comparison between the different levels of well-being in the various federal states.
As a comprehensive index, the CDI can also be used for longitudinal observations
of, for example, how the well-being of children in families with different social
origins has changed over the last 20 years (Hernandez and Marotz 2012).
1.2.4 T
he Concept of Well-Being in the 2013 World Vision
Child Study
Whereas the first study in 2007 already defined well-being in terms of three dimen-
sions, namely, satisfaction with the freedom granted by parents, satisfaction with
the number of friends along with the quality of friendships, and general well-being
at school, we applied a more complex framework in 2010. We developed a broader
concept based on the idea of the “good life” and what is necessary to lead such a
good life in the sense developed by the American social philosopher Martha
Nussbaum in her Capability Approach (Andresen et al. 2010; Fegter and Richter
2013). The Capability Approach addresses the possibilities of self-realization and
the action scopes of individuals in the greatest range of different social contexts. We
have drawn on this approach for the World Vision Child Studies because it addresses
the abilities to act and possibilities of acting in a self-determined way, and because
we also know that this is a major concern for 6- to 11-year-old children. The ques-
tionnaire reveals our use of the Capability Approach in items asking children about
their self-efficacy—as a central foundation of capability. At the end of the individual
interviews, we also asked the children to name five things that every child needs in
order to have a good life—regardless of where that child is growing up. As the 2010
Study showed, this theoretical framework for well-being proved to be extremely
informative. For example, we were able to confirm a close relationship between
high self-efficacy and experiencing that one’s opinion is taken into account when
decisions are made in the parental home, at school, and among friends.
When conceptualizing child well-being in 2013, we have not only built on our
two earlier studies but also integrated the national and international discussion
sketched above. Because satisfaction can be a rather vague and everyday term in
German, we asked the children to tell us how satisfied they were with every single
different life domain in turn. The following dimensions form the concept of well-
being in our study:
• Care from one parent/both parents measured in terms of the amount of time they
devote to their children
• Freedoms in daily life measured in terms of how satisfied children are with the
freedoms their parents grant them
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 7
1.3 T
he Challenge of Child Poverty: Applying International
Discussions to the World Vision Child Study
What is well-being like for children living in poverty? Research on child well-being
is focusing increasingly on this issue. It needs to clarify how strongly poverty and
social disadvantage impact on well-being, what can be done to counter this, and
how child poverty needs to be defined and measured. As we shall show here, there
have been major new international studies in this field since 2010 along with sys-
tematic analyses of how child poverty needs to be measured and evaluated.
Nonetheless, we still know very little about what the children themselves experi-
ence. Up to now, studies on how children in poverty themselves see their world are
very rare in both national and international childhood research. However, such stud-
ies are essential if we are to understand which strategies children use to counter
precarious life conditions, how they themselves perceive their situation, and what
phenomena they have to deal with in their daily lives.
The last World Vision Study in 2010 addressed perceptions and experiences of
poverty in individual child portraits. However, none of the children we interviewed
in 2010 were themselves living in poverty; and the same applies to the children in
the Third Study. In the 2010 interviews, we gave children photographs to look at.
These depicted typical scenarios for relatively poor, relatively affluent, and very
affluent living conditions. On the basis of the children’s responses to these photo-
graphs in 2010, we were able to show that most children were quite capable of clas-
sifying “being poor.” Some referred to families they knew or children at their school
whom they perceived to be disadvantaged. We noticed that when discussing this
topic, children preferred to position themselves and their own families as being
8 S. Andresen et al.
located in the middle between poor and rich, and they generally associated wealth
with the need to be socially responsible.
For children, responsibility seems to be an important topic in the context of pov-
erty. It also plays an important role in the few studies carried out with children who
actually are poor. For example, poor children know exactly what the things they
desire cost, but they frequently do not ask their parents for them. They know the
prices and they know how much money per month their family has at its disposal.
Poor children also adopt responsibility for their parents when, for example, they
look after younger siblings and thereby try to ease the burdens on their mothers (and
fathers). Or they adopt responsibility for the emotional well-being of their parents
by worrying about them (Andresen et al. 2013; Meiland et al. 2013a, b). There is a
great need to find out about the daily lives of children living in poverty because
adults such as childcare workers or teachers need to be aware of the precarious liv-
ing conditions of these children in each and every location in which they interact
with them.
Both the major international comparative studies and the studies of child poverty in
Germany have contributed to a better understanding of the topic. We draw on all
these studies in our own work and we shall examine them in more detail here. One
intensive discussion centers on the fundamental question of how to measure poverty
in adults, youths, and children and how to interpret statistical data. One established
measure of poverty—also used in childhood studies—is oriented toward median
income. In the European Union (EU), anybody earning less than 60% of the median
disposable income in a given nation is considered to be at risk of poverty. Having
less than 50% of median income at one’s disposal is viewed as severe poverty; less
than 40%, as very severe poverty.
Report Card 10, published by UNICEF (2012) under the title Measuring child
poverty, has had a major impact on research. It assesses child poverty with a depri-
vation index and contrasts this index with findings based on the relative poverty
concept using the median disposable household income. The approach is in line
with the aforementioned efforts to establish a composite index of well-being. This
assumes, on the one hand, that child poverty can be assessed in relation to child
development and children’s needs independently from the comparative wealth of a
society, but, on the other hand, that it still has to be related to a nation’s median
income.
What goes into this deprivation index? It assesses whether a child has appropri-
ate and at least partially new clothing; all-weather shoes; regular daily meals includ-
ing fresh fruit, fresh vegetables, and meat (or a vegetarian equivalent); access to
books; regular leisure activities in the sense of nonformal education; and outdoor
leisure equipment such as a bicycle or roller skates. However, the index also includes
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 9
the opportunity to celebrate special occasions such as birthdays and the opportunity
to sometimes invite friends home. Because these are aspects that also characterize
children’s lives in Germany, we also include some of them in our study (see
Chap. 3).
If two or more of the above indicators that are considered to be relevant for an
average child’s life, for what we could call “normal childhood,” are lacking, then a
child’s situation is considered to be deprived. Based on these findings, the interna-
tional UNICEF research team has classified the European nations into different
groups with deprivation rates ranging from low to high.
One important finding from this international comparison, which is also con-
firmed in the World Vision Child Studies, is the particular risk of poverty facing
children in single-parent families and in families with unemployed parents or par-
ents with low education. This is also in line with the present findings. In Germany,
children in large families with more than two siblings also face a significantly higher
risk of poverty.
Germany currently reveals new trends in exposure to the risk of poverty. Most avail-
able statistics are based on the concept of relative income poverty, and, as pointed
out above, children whose families have less than 60% of median disposable house-
hold income at their disposal are taken to be at risk of poverty. In exact figures for
2011, this threshold stood at 880 Euro per month for a single-person household,
1848 Euro for a two-person household with two children under the age of 14 years
and 1144 Euro for a single parent with one child (Tophoven et al. 2015). Twenty
percent of children under the age of 15 live in households with less than 60% of
median disposable household income and 24.2% are considered to be at risk of
poverty and/or currently receiving welfare payments (SGB-II Bezug) (Tophoven
et al. 2015).
If we then go on to look at poverty trends over the last 20 years, poverty risk rates
reveal major fluctuations. For a long time, children and adolescents faced a higher
risk of poverty than the general population in Germany. For children up to the age
of 10 years, the rate has now dropped to the same level as the general population.
This contrasts strongly with the group of 11- to 20-year-olds who continue to face a
disproportionally high risk (Deutscher Bundestag 2013). Our latest figures in the
Third World Vision Child Study confirm this trend. However, when we look at
children up to the age of 10 years with a migration background, we can see that
they continue to reveal a comparatively high poverty risk of 15.1% (Deutscher
Bundestag 2013).
10 S. Andresen et al.
1.3.3 C
hild Poverty from a Longitudinal Perspective:
The AWO-ISS Studies
As well as asking how child poverty rates change over the years, it is particularly
important to know how being exposed to poverty in the early years influences a
child’s later life. Thanks to national and international monitoring, we can observe
trends in poverty over a longer period of time—as in the latest Youth Report of the
German Federal Government (Deutscher Bundestag 2013). It is particularly impor-
tant to know which groups experience poverty at an early age, for how long, and
how permanently. One highly relevant piece of research for this is the AWO-ISS
study that presented its latest findings from the fourth wave of measurement in
2012. The AWO-ISS is a longitudinal study of child and youth poverty that takes a
multidimensional, child-related, and resource-oriented approach. It has now been
tracking 900 children over a period of 15 years by comparing the living conditions
of children from low-income families with children from more affluent families in
a range of different areas of life. It has identified three groups of children who are
living in poverty:
• Poor children who experience no social, cultural, health-related, or material con-
straints despite growing up in a low-income family (“poor, but with
well-being”)
• Poor children who experience constraints in a few areas and therefore have to be
considered disadvantaged (“poor, disadvantaged”)
• Poor children who experience massive deprivations or disadvantages in various
areas (“poor, multiply deprived”)
The longitudinal perspective also reveals the large proportion of children who
were exposed to poverty as young children and have continued to have repeated
experiences of poverty as they grow up (AWO-ISS 2012). In adolescence, these
children have an increased risk of multiple deprivations and disadvantages in all
areas of life as well as low well-being. In contrast, poverty experienced in early
childhood that is then permanently overcome seems to have no negative
consequences.
The trend across all three of our studies reveals, on the one hand, a slight drop in
the poverty rate, but, on the other hand, a decline in membership of clubs and asso-
ciations among children living in poverty since 2010 (see Chap. 6).
One important issue, on which we can make only marginal statements in our study,
is coping with experiences of poverty. Back in 2000, Antje Richter already worked
out two different forms of coping in a study of child poverty in rural elementary
school children: problem-avoidant and problem-solving coping. These two forms
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 11
revealed marked gender-specific differences: Whereas both girls and boys were
equally capable of realizing they had a problem, it was girls who more frequently
applied the strategy of seeking social support to overcome it. Moreover, interna-
tional resilience studies have shown that boys react more strongly to family poverty
in early childhood, preschool age, and the first years of school by exhibiting exter-
nalizing problem behavior and impaired academic ability (Ittel and Scheithauer
2007; Luthar 1999). In contrast, the new AWO ISS study (2012) has found no
gender-specific effects in 10-year-olds living in poverty. Although nonpoor girls
tend to have fewer problems than nonpoor boys, this effect disappears under poverty
conditions. In other words, girls living in poverty are just as disadvantaged as boys
living in poverty. The current discussion on resilience as an ability to resist stress
and negative external influences reveals one very interesting finding in this context:
Resilient children tend to exhibit less gender-typical behavior and therefore possess
a broader repertoire of behaviors with which to counter everyday stress (see Zander
2013).
1.3.5 C
hild Poverty as Experienced Constraint: The Approach
Taken by the World Vision Child Studies
Poverty is a major topic in all three World Vision Child Studies. Our concept of
poverty is multidimensional. In other words, we do not view poverty as being just a
question of financial want, but take various areas of a child’s life into account such
as the family or leisure. This links up with the question whether research needs a
specific child poverty concept and how such a concept may differ from that of youth
or adult poverty. The question has emerged from concerns about whether children
are particularly helpless when it comes to the causes of their poverty and whether
the negative consequences of poverty have a stronger and more long-lasting effect
on children than on adults; in other words, whether children represent a particularly
vulnerable group.
Our study takes the perspective of the actors seriously; it tries to examine the
everyday experiences of children and to relate these to social conditions. In addi-
tion, we understand child poverty as a limitation of chances to participate in society
and to engage in self-realization.
The concept of child poverty in the World Vision Child Studies is based on the
following dimensions:
• Limited financial options in the family measured by the children’s subjective
assessments of whether the family is short of money or has enough for all neces-
sities such as warm clothing or school books.
• Limited participation in social and cultural life measured by the children’s sub-
jective assessments of various experiences such as not being able to go away on
vacation, go to the movies, or learn to play a musical instrument.
12 S. Andresen et al.
• In addition, parents’ subjective assessments of how well they manage the money
at their disposal. These were entered into what we called a class index that
divided all children into five groups with different social origins. The parents’
level of education, type of accommodation (owned or rented property), and
reports on the number of books in the household are also major components of
this index.
Both our own research and other studies have emphasized the extent to which
poverty is associated with shame and shaming. Children often feel stigmatized
together with their parents. Although the German government has launched nonfor-
mal education and participation grants for poor children, not all local authorities and
institutions have developed good ways of implementing them. This sociopolitical
measure is known as the Bildungs- und Teilhabepaket [education and participation
packet] is based on the assumption that parents will spend funds allocated to chil-
dren in other ways so that they will fail to reach the children concerned. Therefore,
parents may, or may have to, apply to receive school materials, private lessons,
excursions, or 10 Euro for a club membership for their children. However, the great
bureaucratic effort involved in this means that much of the funding allocated to this
measure also fails to reach the children. Moreover, children are ashamed to be iden-
tified as “aid recipients” for free meals at schools or daycare centers. The research
itself can also contribute to generating shame: Language, for example, can have a
powerful effect. In this context, although we apply the term “lower class” in our
World Vision Child Studies, we wish to emphasize that we use it exclusively to label
low socioeconomic status. We distance ourselves emphatically from stigmatizing
uses of the term such as “lower class culture.” The primary purpose of such dis-
courses is to stigmatize the behavior of people living in precarious circumstances
and to ignore the conditions in which they live. The children are very much aware
of these public discussions and the attributions of blame that frequently accompany
them. This is something that any study of well-being in children should acknowl-
edge and reflect on critically.
Unconsidered use of the term “with a migration background” can also be stigmatiz-
ing. This is despite the fact that recent findings such as the latest child migration
report of the German Youth Institute from 2013 show that there are only a few cases
in which migration alone predicts, for example, enrolment in daycare facilities for
children, school-leaving qualifications, or learning potentials. Nonetheless, how to
appropriately integrate children with different experiences of migration remains one
of the challenges facing childhood studies (Hunner-Kreisel and Stephan 2013). One
starting point and concern in the 2013 World Vision Child Study is to determine in
which cases migration can or cannot serve as an explanatory factor.
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 13
Which findings can we draw on for our Third World Vision Child Study or which
are relevant for childhood studies? Migration is frequently discussed in relation to
education and inequality. Despite a general shift toward higher participation in edu-
cation, marked differences still persist: For example, 37% of migrants complete
only basic secondary school (Hauptschulabschluss) compared to 20% of
nonmigrants.
Further differences in where people live and the risk of poverty are also certainly
relevant for the daily lives of children. First, the majority of children attributed with
a migration background live in large cities (Statistisches Bundesamt 2011b, p. 14).
Second, children with a migration background more frequently live in socially pre-
carious situations than children without a migration background. This is due to a
concentration of structural risk factors such as income poverty, unemployment, and
parents with a low level of education (Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung
2012). Children from families of Turkish origin are particularly at risk or exposed
to multiple risks here.
14 S. Andresen et al.
A final aspect that is also relevant for childhood studies is that the proportion of
people with a migration background is higher in younger age groups than in the
general population. Indeed, the younger the children, the higher the proportion with
a migration background. However, the children themselves mostly have no personal
history of migration but were born in Germany. Accordingly, children in German
cities meet other children with a wide range of different backgrounds when they
attend a Kindergarten or school, join a youth club, or go to the playground.
1.4.3 U
sing the Migration Concept in the World Vision
Child Study
The data from the Federal Statistical Office reveal the heterogeneity of migration
contexts and experiences. As in the previous studies, we also looked into the con-
cept of migration background when planning the Third World Vision Child Study.
For the sample recruited for the quantitative part of the study, we decided to orient
our research toward the concept of migration background on the basis of the micro-
census. For the qualitative interviews, we decided to trace the children’s individual
histories; and, this time, to purposefully select 7 children with a migration back-
ground within the sample of 12 children interviewed. Only the German version of
this book contains a chapter with the results of this qualitative part of the study. This
should cast light on the histories of the children, their families, and their daily lives,
and allow us to work out not only their resources but also their possible disadvan-
tages with the help of the children’s narratives. Combining these portraits with the
analysis in the qualitative part provides us with an even more detailed approach.
The concept of well-being and the analysis of poverty go hand in hand with ideas
about the “good life,” a “good childhood,” and just conditions. Children themselves
do not just relate well-being to justice but also the differences between poor and
rich. Based on what we learned in our previous studies, critical discussions on fam-
ily policy, and the available options for children to participate or make complaints,
we decided to focus particularly on justice in the Third Study. Although justice has
not been an explicit topic in recent childhood studies, we consider that it is time for
this to change. Many of the current challenges facing society relate to intergenera-
tional justice and social justice, and there is a need to clarify how the concept of
childhood on the one side and the experiences, feelings, and perceptions of children
on the other side relate to classic or contemporary ideas on justice.
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 15
In the World Vision Child Study, we are particularly interested in the sense of
justice among 6- to 11-year-olds. We have developed our questionnaire and our
interview guidelines with this in mind. We started off with everyday observations
showing that children in this age group discuss fairness but also injustice in a range
of different contexts. In interviews with children, we found that, for example, prom-
ises that are given, kept, or broken play an important role; and that conflicts with
other children can also be traced back to ideas on justice. Moreover, sanctions or
punishments for breaking the rules also relate to justice. In the parental home and at
school, children additionally get to know different reward systems, and it is cer-
tainly worth examining whether they perceive these to be just or unjust.
1.5.1 P
reparing the Topic for the Third World Vision
Child Study
Proceeding from these everyday observations and with reference to the fundamental
scientific interest and objectives of the World Vision Child Studies, we see an urgent
need for childhood studies to pay more attention to justice. However, up to now,
there has been a lack of research in this field apart from studies in developmental
psychology and the classic studies on moral development such as those carried out
by Jean Piaget. In our approach, we started by examining justice theory and explor-
ing its potential for issues in childhood studies. We then examined relevant empiri-
cal research and considered how we could apply the knowledge accumulated there
to our study. In particular, after examining empirical research starting initially with
developmental psychology and then moving on to recent experiments in economics,
we decided not to look for evidence of different concepts of justice but to ask the
children directly about their own sense of justice. This also links up with philo-
sophical discussions on the significance of the sense of injustice compared to more
abstract concepts of justice that require a great deal of cognitive reflection and a
distancing of oneself from one’s own interests.
This roughly sketches the focus of our study. Our aim is to understand what chil-
dren between the ages of 6 and 11 years consider to be just or unjust. Earlier inter-
views have already revealed that justice or fairness is of great concern to children
and that they discuss it in completely different ways—sometimes, more abstractly
and philosophically; other times, very concretely and in everyday terms. We have
tried to operationalize “justice” as closely as possible to the world of this age group.
We have done this by also focusing on selected positions in justice theory. What do
6-year-olds and what do 10-year-olds perceive as being just or unjust in the family,
in interaction with peers, and at school? What do they think about specific situations
and relationships? How do they judge the standards set by society and how do these
influence their daily lives? Finally, we are interested in what indications can be
derived from the findings for the concrete implementation of children’s rights in all
areas of their lives.
16 S. Andresen et al.
The issue of just behavior and a just organization of society is as old as philosophy
itself. Systematic ideas on just behavior and relations entered Western thought par-
ticularly in classical philosophy through the work of Socrates, Plato, and Aristotle.
One major aspect here is understanding justice as a virtue and therefore as a person-
ality trait in the sense of personal integrity. The Platonic dialogues conceive justice
as the highest virtue following wisdom, courage, and moderation. What is important
here is the human striving toward a just order.
In Aristotelian philosophy, justice serves not only as a general moral concept but
also for the appraisal of specific situations in interpersonal relations. Therefore, put
very simply, Aristotle points out that justice regulates essential aspects of human
coexistence and he tells us how it does this. Basically, these topics have lost none of
their relevance today; and not least for children, because they continue to be con-
cerned with the distribution of goods or positions, how to behave, and how to solve
conflicts. Accordingly, justice theories are concerned just as much with distribution
and compensation or exchange as with procedures for setting up and regulating a
just human coexistence.
One way of introducing such procedures is, for example, for partners to enter
contracts based on reasonable and transparent rules that have to be honored as far as
possible. However, being in any way able to make a contract already calls for a lot
of preconditions, and these are mostly not possible for a child to fulfill. Adults then
act on the child’s behalf. However, the questions whether a good is distributed justly
or unjustly, whether an exchange is disadvantageous for one of the parties, and
whether reward and punishment follow a just logic arise in the everyday experi-
ences of children. It is these that correspond to their sense of justice—as our find-
ings reported in Chaps. 2 and 7 clearly show.
The justice theory proposed by the American scholar John Rawls has had a major
impact on the recent history of philosophy. In his book A Theory of Justice (1971),
he explains how justice in the sense of fairness has to be fundamental for social
institutions. Nonetheless, it should not restrict the freedom of individuals. In an
obituary for Rawls who died in 2002, the German philosopher Winfried Hinsch
expressed this fundamental idea as follows: “Justice as fairness is the central theme
in Rawls’ theory. A society is just when the institutions on which it is based are
guided by basic principles that its citizens would personally impose on them-
selves—given fair conditions of freedom and equality” (Hinsch 2002, translated).
Rawls put his ideas into concrete terms by formulating a model of the “original
position”. This is a hypothetical or even experimental situation in which the
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 17
members of a society come together behind a “veil of ignorance” with the goal of
jointly determining the highest principles of justice for their society. Because of the
veil, none of them know who they and the others are in real life and what they can
achieve in the future. In this way, the veil prevents the decisions that citizens make
in this original position from being influenced by individual interests and socially
given balances of power. Rawls was convinced that this was the way to guarantee
and ensure fairness.
In order to pursue justice, according to Rawls, it is necessary for all persons to
possess basic goods such as fundamental rights, social privileges, resources, and the
social preconditions for self-respect. Simply leafing through any daily newspaper
shows us how crucial it is to guarantee such basic goods and how important this is
particularly for children—examples in Germany being the current call for a guaran-
teed child allowance independent of parents, or the implementation of basic rights
such as being able to point to injustice through an established complaints system in
sport clubs or schools.
Rawls was also interested in the social inequality that always goes hand in hand
with justice theories—a topic with a strong impact on children and therefore also on
childhood studies. Here he makes an interesting proposal: He suggests that social
inequality can be justified only after two conditions have been met: First, fair access
to positions and goods has to be ensured, that is, equality of opportunity; and sec-
ond, the greatest advantages have to be given to those least favored in a society.
Hence, he proposes a universal justice oriented toward strong principles.
In his book The Idea of Justice, the economist Amartya Sen (2010), however,
asks whether a more pragmatic theory of justice oriented toward situational ideas
might be more appropriate than one oriented toward universalistic principles, espe-
cially when making decisions on and dealing with social conflicts. He explains what
he means in an allegory over “three children and a flute:”
You have to decide which of three children—Anne, Bob, and Carla—should get a flute
about which they are quarreling. Anne claims the flute saying that she is the only one of the
three who knows how to play it (the others do not deny this), and that it would be quite
unjust to deny the flute to the only one who can actually play it. If that was all you knew,
the reason for giving the flute to the first child would be strong. (p. 13)
But that is not everything that Sen tells us about in his allegory because Bob points
out that he should be given the flute, because unlike the other two, he has nothing to
play with, no pretty things with which to occupy himself. This is because Bob is
poor and the two girls concede this. Carla, in contrast, also claims the flute because
She has been working diligently for many months to make the flute . . . and just when she
had finished her work, “just then,” she complains, “these expropriators came along to try to
grab the flute away from me.” (p. 13)
18 S. Andresen et al.
In our qualitative interviews, we read this allegory out loud to the children and
ask them to tell us how they would decide. As Sen suspected, very different criteria
and deliberations are applied and all of them have something in their favor. Sen uses
this allegory to work out how theorists in different schools of social justice can find
good reasons for reaching their decision: Economic egalitarians, who find material
inequality unjust and wish to overcome it, would favor Bob who has no possessions;
utilitarians would probably want to give the flute to Anne, the only musician in the
group; and libertarians, who emphasize productivity, would have to favor Clara
because she made the flute.
This rough sketch of ideas in justice theory should particularly emphasize what
childhood studies can borrow from it. Our attempts to express this empirically and
the results obtained from the Third Study reveal a great deal of potential in interre-
lating theories of childhood and theories of justice.
What does empirical research have to tell us about justice in children; or, to put it
better, about the development of an orientation toward justice in children? How
important is it for children to feel that they are treated fairly, and do they develop a
sense of injustice? Here we can link up with the work and insights of moral psychol-
ogy and particularly with the studies of Jean Piaget and their continuation in the
work of Lawrence Kohlberg. Piaget was interested in children’s legal awareness and
how they deal with rules in groups. His observations of games of marbles and the
derivation of central developmental stages were a milestone in the discussion on the
development of a moral awareness in children and hence a development oriented
toward just arrangements. One of Piaget’s interests was in when and how children
switch from obeying adults to orienting themselves toward the rules of, for example,
the peer group and toward their desire for cooperation. “What is decisive is the
development of the concept of justice. Whereas for the younger child, what is just
means almost exactly the same as the will of adults, the ideas of equality and reci-
procity already assert themselves toward the end of early childhood” (Piaget 1999,
p. 158, translated from German). Piaget emphasized that the shaping of the environ-
ment and the behavior of adults play a decisive role in this developmental process.
As a result, he called for teachers to actively involve even the youngest students in
their classes.
Recent childhood studies have tended to distance themselves from assumptions
in developmental psychology in order to view children as actors in the construction
of their reality and to find out about their co-constructions. Nonetheless, it is worth
taking a new look at the work of Piaget and examining its potential for justice and
well-being. This is the reason for mentioning these works here—especially because
both the quantitative and qualitative parts of our study show clear age differences in
the appraisal of justice.
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 19
For many years, anthropologists, social anthropologists, and evolutionists have been
studying how human beings build up trust, why they keep or break promises, and
how cooperations can emerge between not only groups of persons who know each
other but also groups of strangers. These questions are also related to fundamental
issues in economics or social theory such as whether competition impedes the striv-
ing for justice and tends to encourage the egoistic behavior. What is interesting is
that we only tend to see a willingness to share in societies once conditions of
exchange and trade have become established. Such findings point to the significance
of economics. Recent empirical research such as the work of the Zurich economist
Ernst Fehr offers much food for thought regarding questions in justice and child-
hood theory. Fehr and his team not only draw on findings from evolutionary research
into the social behavior of primates and findings on the theory of mind but are also
interested in genetically determined characteristics that contribute to the develop-
ment of preferences for justice in human beings. Fehr poses the fundamental ques-
tion whether human beings have a more egoistic or more altruistic orientation and
how and under which conditions altruism develops.
On the basis of experimental studies in various regions of the world and with all
age groups—including very young children—Fehr has worked out that recognition
is the main motor driving just judgments and behavior. This introduces an important
element into the discussion within childhood studies: recognition as a need in inter-
actions between children and adults, but also between peers, and as an aspect of
well-being.
A further essential element in the work of Fehr, however, is research on the sense
of injustice or the rejection of injustice or inequality. This does not mean that
inequality is rejected in general. Whether inequality, according to Fehr, is rejected
because of the sense of justice depends decisively on the circumstances in which it
has emerged, that is, on the context. This reveals links to Rawls’ theory of justice.
However, what can economists tell us about children and their rejection of
injustice?
On the basis of various experimental games with children, Fehr and his col-
leagues have determined that “inequality aversion” develops between the ages of 3
and 8 years and is age-dependent: the younger children are, the stronger their selfish
orientation toward their own interests (Fehr et al. 2008). However, they develop
preferences in the sense of parochialism, that is, a social togetherness. Fehr has also
shown that children prefer to favor their own group, be it in preschool, school, or a
club. The World Vision Child Studies do not observe the behavior and practices of
children and they do not engage in experimental research. Nonetheless, the econo-
mist’s approach reveals much knowledge potential for our research in which we are
asking the children to tell us about their evaluations, their understanding, and the
reasons they give for the decisions they favor.
20 S. Andresen et al.
Until now, hardly any orientation toward justice theory has found its way into recent
childhood studies. The Third World Vision Child Study is also unable to build on a
sound basis in justice theory because it is entering new territory here. However, in a
first step, we draw on earlier findings on the good life from the perspective of chil-
dren. In the Third Study, justice also serves as a strong link between the qualitative
and quantitative assessments. For our in-depth interviews, we developed case sce-
narios that we presented to the children. Alongside the allegory of the three children
and the flute, these contain concrete cases taken from the everyday world of today’s
children that address just distribution, rewards for achievements, keeping or break-
ing promises, punishments, and finally the topics of codetermination and justice.
We have also developed new justice-theory-oriented items for the questionnaire.
Moreover, the results deliver interesting findings on the age dependence of the sense
of justice. Our initial concern is to find out how far the children consider things to
be just in the proximal environment of their family and school, then more broadly
in Germany, and finally in the world as a whole. However, when looking at the sense
and perception of injustice, it is also important to find out which persons the chil-
dren consider being particularly exposed to injustice. Therefore, we finally ask the
children to evaluate specific cases addressing the provision and distribution of
goods.
Detailed results on this can be found in the following Chap. 2. They encourage
us to carry out more systematic work on this topic.
Any comparison of the instruments used in each of the three studies will initially
reveal the same body of items. However, a closer inspection uncovers modified or
new items and scales. Both sticking to a constant set of items and setting new
accents are important to us. Comparable items are needed to document change, but
new ones are necessary to introduce focal topics and further develop our question-
naires and our interview techniques. This also brings us into line with international
advances in this field.
The Third World Vision Child Study also tackles new developments and chal-
lenges in international childhood studies. We are studying well-being, the experi-
ence of poverty, and the sense of justice among children in Germany and gaining
new knowledge about the main areas of life of children between the ages of 6 and
11 years. With the Third Study and the same body of questionnaire items in 2013,
we can perform numerous comparisons with the two prior studies. With our repre-
sentative sample, we can uncover differences between boys and girls, between
urban and rural districts, and also differences due to contexts codetermined by
migration.
1 How Children See Well-Being, Poverty, and Justice: The Focus of the Third World… 21
References
Andresen, S., & Hurrelmann, K. (2007). Was bedeutet es, heute ein Kind zu sein? Die World Vision
Kinderstudie als Beitrag zur Kinder- und Kindheitsforschung. In World Vision Deutschland.
(Hrsg.) Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 35 – 64). Frankfurt a. M.:
Fischer.
Andresen, S., Hurrelmann, K., & Fegter, S. (2010). Wie geht es unseren Kindern? Wohlbefinden
und Lebensbedingungen der Kinder in Deutschland. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.)
Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 35 – 59). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Andresen, S., Hurrelmann, K., & Schneekloth, U. (2012). Care and freedom. Theoretical and
empirical aspects of children’s well-being. Child Indicators Research, 5, 437–448.
Andresen, S., Mailand, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013). Erfahrungen und Erleben von Armut
aus der Sicht von Kindern: »Ich würde meiner Familie was schenken und dafür sorgen, dass sie
nicht so viel in Schwierigkeiten sind.« Unsere Jugend 65. Jg. (S. 123 – 129).
Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung. (2012). Bildung in Deutschland 2012. Ein indika-
torengestützter Bericht mit einer Analyse zu Perspektiven des Bildungswesens im demograf-
ischen Wandel. Bielefeld: W. Bertelsmann Verlag.
AWO-ISS. (2012). Lebenssituation und Zukunftschancen von (armen) Kindern und Jugendlichen.
AWO-Verlag.
Bradshaw, J., Martorano, B., Natali, L., & de Neubourg, C. (2013) Children’s subjective well-
being in rich countries (Working Paper). UNICEF Office of Research, Florence.
Deutscher Bundestag. (2013). Der 14. Kinder- und Jugendbericht. Bericht über die Lebenssituation
junger Menschen und die Leistungen und Bestrebungen der Kinder- und Jugendhilfe Iin
Deutschland. Deutscher Bundestag, Drucksache 17 / 12200. http://dip21.bundestag.de/dip21/
btd/17 / 122 / 1712200.pdf (Stand: 24.06.2013).
Fattore, T., Mason, J., & Watson, E. (2012): Locating the child centrally as subject in research:
Towards a child interpretation of well-being. Child Indicators Research, 5(3), S. 423 – 435.
Fegter, S., & Richter, M. (2013). The capability approach as a framework for research on children’s
well-being. In A. Ben-Arieh, F. Casas, I. Frones, & J. Korbin (Eds.), Handbook of child well-
being: theory, indicators, measures and policies. Dordrecht/Heidelberg/ London/New York:
Springer. Chapter No. 151.
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. In Nature
(Vol. 454, pp. 1079–1083).
Hernandez, D. J., & Marotz, K. G. (2012). Disparities in child well-being across income groups:
Trends in the U.S. from 1985 to 2008. Child Indicators Research, 5(1) S. 93 – 121.
Hinsch, W. (2002). Gerechtfertigte Ungleichheiten: Grundsätze sozialer Gerechtigkeit. Berlin:
Walter de Gruyter.
Hunner-Kreisel, C., & Stephan, M. (Hrsg.). (2013). Neue Zeiten, neue Räume: Kindheit und
Familie im Kontext von (Trans-)Migration und sozialem Wandel. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Ittel, A., & Scheithauer, H. (2007). Geschlecht als »Stärke« oder »Risiko«? Überlegungen zur
geschlechterspezifischen Resilienz. In Opp, G., & Fingerle, M. (Hrsg.). Was Kinder stärkt.
Erziehung zwischen Risiko und Resilienz (S. 98 – 115). München/Basel: Reinhardt.
Luthar, S. S. (1999). Poverty and children‘s adjustment. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Meiland, S., Andresen, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013a) Denkst du, die Kinder sind hier arm?
Sichtweisen und Erfahrungen von Kindern in prekären Lebenslagen. In Fisher, D., Gathen, J.,
Höhmann, K., Klaffke, T., Rademacker, H. (Hrsg.). Friedrich Jahresheft XXXI 2013a, Schule
und Armut (S. 16 – 18). Oldenburg: Friedrich Verlag.
Meiland, S., Andresen, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013b). Erfahrungen und Erleben von
Armut aus der Sicht von Kindern: »Ich würde meiner Familie was schenken und dafür sorgen,
dass sie nicht so viel in Schwierigkeiten sind.« Unsere Jugend 65. Jg. (S. 123 – 129).
Minkkinen, J. (2013). The structural model of child well-being. Child Indicators Research, 6(3),
547–558.
22 S. Andresen et al.
O’Hare, W. P., & Gutierrez, F. (2012). The use of domains in constructing a comprehensive com-
posite index of child well-being. Child Indicators Research, 5, 609–629.
Piaget, J. (1999). Über Pädagogik. Beltz: Weinheim.
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Harvard University Press.
Robert Bosch Stiftung. (2012). Starke Kinder – starke Familie. Wohlbefinden von Kindern in
Städten und Gemeinden. Stuttgart: Robert Bosch Stiftung.
Sen, A. (2010). Die Idee der Gerechtigkeit. München: C. H. Beck Verlag.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2009). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit-Einbürgerung 2008 (Fachserie
1 Reihe 2.1). Wiesbaden.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2011a). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit. Bevölkerung mit
Migrationshintergrund-Ergebnisse des Mikrozensus 2010 (Fachserie 1 Reihe 2.2). Wiesbaden.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2011b). Pressemitteilung Nr. 345 vom 20. September 2011b: Knapp die
Hälfte der Großstadtkinder aus Familien mit Migrationshintergrund. Wiesbaden.
Tophoven, S., Wenzig, C., & Lietzmann, T. (2015). Kinder- und Familienarmut: Lebensumstände
von Kindern in der Grundsicherung. Institut für Arbeitsmarkt- und Berufsforschung für die
Bertelsmann Stiftung.
UNICEF. (2012). Measuring child poverty. New league tables of child poverty in the world’s rich
countries. Report Card 10, Innocenti Research Centre, online unter. www.unicef-irc.org/publi-
cations/pdf/rc10_eng.pdf (Stand: 14.07.2013).
UNICEF. (2013). The state of the world’s children 2013: Children with disabilities. online:
http://www.unicef.org/sowc2013/files/SWCR2013_ENG_Lo_res_24_Apr_2013.pdf (Stand:
14.07.2013).
Zander, M. (2013). Resilienz – Gender – Prekäre Lebenslagen. Wie kann eine gendersensible
Resilienzförderung aussehen? Betrifft Mädchen, 3 / 2013.
Chapter 2
What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different
Faces of Justice
Ulrich Schneekloth and Sabine Andresen
Questions of fairness and justice are of much concern to children. In their world,
these are important and sometimes even existential challenges. This does not just
apply to everyday life in the family, in the circle of friends, at school, and during
leisure, but also to more general problems in our society that they perceive as the
“adult world.” This also confronts them with the question of what role children have
to play in this world.
Classical developmental psychology oriented toward the work of Jean Piaget and
Lawrence Kohlberg reveals a broad consensus that children overcome their early
childhood egocentrism during the transition from early to middle childhood.
Increasingly, they develop the ability to recognize and emulate social rules and
thereby to comply with them (Piaget 1932/1983, 1937/1975; Kohlberg 1964, 1974;
see Oerter and Montada 2002, for a German-language overview). When these
observations and experiments were carried out in either the 1930s or the late 1950s,
it was assumed that it is only from the age of roughly 9 years onward that children
develop the ability to grasp social rules as (potentially changeable) shared agree-
ments and to view themselves as members of a community for which they have to
adopt responsibility (the so-called conventional stage according to Kohlberg; see
Trautner 1991). Younger children between the approximate ages of 4 and 8 years, in
contrast, were assumed to see social rules as indisputable givens that they comply
with through either a fixation on authority or fear of punishment (the preconven-
tional stage; see Trautner 1991).
U. Schneekloth (*)
Kantar Public, München, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
S. Andresen
Department of Education, Institute of Social Pedagogy and Adult Education,
Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
However, more recent studies on (early) childhood development have shown that
even at the age of 3 years, children already reveal a sense of justice oriented toward
the principle of reciprocity in their mutual play. For example, Hamann et al. (2011)
have shown that children distribute rewards relatively fairly, that is, more or less
equally, in return for acts that have been performed collaboratively. Recent experi-
mental studies have also shown that children between the ages of 3 and 8 years
increasingly reveal a willingness to also consider the well-being of others, particu-
larly when these others belong to their own proximal surroundings (neighborhood,
preschool, school). From roughly the age of 7 years onward, they reveal a clearly
recognizable altruism in the sense of a marked orientation toward the well-being of
others (Fehr et al. 2008). These studies suggest that children in this age range do not
judge justice abstractly or in principle, but always in relation to everyday situations
and to familiar individuals or groups. In this context, children develop their own
idea of justice that they then apply to different situations in their daily lives.
According to the findings from these experimental studies, the 6- to 11-year-old
children in the Third World Vision Child Study of 2013 are located exactly in that
age window during which they develop their own understanding of justice in rela-
tion to the well-being of others. What is decisive here is the child’s own environ-
ment, that is, how fair and, where applicable, how cooperative children experience
life to be in their own family, in their circle of friends, or at their school. Moreover,
in this Child Study, we are also interested in how children perceive their social envi-
ronment and how fairly they think people are treated in Germany as a whole. In the
last Child Study in 2010, we already showed that 6- to 8-year-old children are capa-
ble of describing their life situation very clearly and comprehensibly, and they are
also able to report how they evaluate certain general problems in Germany. The
more they have to experience social problems directly in their daily lives, for exam-
ple, through poverty or perceived exclusion, the clearer and more unequivocal their
answers. The fact that 6- to 7-year-olds differ somewhat in the breadth of their
experience and their understanding compared to somewhat older children does not
contradict this statement. Not all the children could answer the questions we posed
in our quantitative assessment equally well. Therefore, when we asked for general
evaluations of social problems, and we could see that particularly 6- to 7-year-old
children were unable to give their own answers, we dropped such questions from
the further course of the interviews. In addition, all children had the basic right not
to answer such evaluative questions if they chose not to.1
In this chapter, we want to present the children’s views on how fairly children are
treated and how fair our society is in general. We developed our instruments in the
quantitative and qualitative parts of the study on the basis of the theoretical ideas
presented in Chap. 1 and the unstructured questions posed during our preparatory
1
A typical example is when children are told that there are no “right” and no “wrong” answers
here, but still reply by asking “I don’t know. Do you know?” Another example is when children do
not want to answer because they are clearly unable to relate the question to their world. In such
cases, interviewers were instructed to use the “don’t know” response category. Any related follow-
up questions were then simply skipped for 6- to 7-year-old children.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 25
pretest survey. We are guided by two principle ideas: first, to classify the children’s
perception of situations, conflicts, and socially determined inequalities as being fair
or unfair; and second, to relate this to the children’s well-being and social living
conditions. Our underlying assumption is that there is a relation between well-being
and the idea of justice, and that the social context in which children are growing up
plays a very significant role in this relation. Hence, we test possible links between
the ideas of justice and social origin. In the following, we shall refer primarily to the
results of our quantitative survey. However, these are also integrated with the find-
ings from the portraits in the qualitative part of our study, because this enables us to
present and interpret 6- to 11-year-old children’s ideas of justice in depth (see
Chap. 8, on the qualitative part of the study in general). The open and unstructured
questions on justice in our pretest already gave us many very informative ideas. We
should like to illustrate the breadth of ideas children have by reporting those of a
6-year-old girl. When asked simply what she understands by justice, what comes to
her mind are the categories of freedom (being allowed to say everything), a just way
of dealing with the mistakes people make (not getting into trouble straight away),
and an open and inclusive community of all children:
That people are allowed to say what they want, that people don’t get into trouble straight
away when they sometimes do something wrong, that all children should be allowed to play
together regardless of whether they come from somewhere else or whether they are poor.
(6-year-old girl)
This focus on the idea of justice and the sense of injustice links up systematically
with a central theme in all three Child Studies: our interest in child well-being.
One major issue in empirical research on justice is to ask people what they feel
to be just and unjust in a society and on which ideas of justice they base these feel-
ings. There can be no denying that these specific ideas and evaluations are closely
related to subjective well-being and the societal framing conditions that contribute
to well-being (see Liebig and Lengfeld 2002, for an overview). This also suggests
using the personal well-being of children in Germany as a starting point for asking
them what they think about justice in the nation. At the end of our representative
survey, we ask children “And, finally, how happy are you with your life in general?”
The children can answer this item on a 5-point smiley scale.2
In all, the 6- to 11-year-olds in our survey report an even slightly higher satisfac-
tion with their own lives compared to the Second World Vision Child Study in 2010
(Fig. 2.1). In this the Third Child Study, 59% of the children give very positive and
2
The question was formulated in line with the life satisfaction surveys frequently used in German
empirical social research. However, surveys of the general population generally use an 11-point
scale (from 0 to 10).
26 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
59 2010
Very positive
59 2013
30
Positive
32
8
Neutral
8
1
Negative
1
2
Very negative
0
a further 32% give positive answers. No more than 8% give a neutral answer, and
only 1% report an explicitly negative life satisfaction.
Naturally, at this point, it is always necessary to bear in mind that children in
particular will always tend to give positive answers when asked about their overall
life satisfaction, and that neutral or negative answers will usually be the exception.3
This makes it all the more remarkable that only 72% of the children with the lowest
social origins, the so-called lower class, give positive answers, whereas 28% (com-
pared to 26% in 2010) give neutral or explicitly negative answers (on the demarca-
tion of the social classes in the Child Study, see Chap. 3 and Appendix 2). No other
social class articulates such a conspicuously different and also negative evaluation
of the personal situation (Table 2.1).
As our analyses in the Second World Vision Child Study in 2010 have already
shown, the lack of life satisfaction in children is due to a combination of household
risk factors (parental poverty and/or unemployment, growing up in a single-parent
family) and what is frequently in consequence a deficit in care and attention from
parents coupled with the children’s own feeling of having no chance in life or of
3
This is also the case in the most recent UNICEF study “Child Well-Being in Rich Countries”
UNICEF, 2013). It cites findings from the “Health Behavior in School-Aged Children 2009/10”
(HBSC-Studie 2011) study of life satisfaction in 11-, 13-, and 15-year-olds. Here as well, 84% of
respondents gave positive answers. Among 11-year-olds, this even reached 86%, which is almost
equal to the 89% measured in the Second World Vision Child Study in 2010.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 27
being unable to cope with the demands they have to face (low expected self-
efficacy). All these factors accumulate particularly in children from the lowest social
class (Schneekloth and Pupeter 2010b, pp. 210–215).
A look at the children’s ratings for important domains in their daily lives reveals
small but subtle differences in the latest Child Study as well. The highest satisfac-
tion is with the circle of friends (94% positive ratings) and leisure (91% positive). A
large majority of children also rate parental care very positively (88% positive). At
84%, a slightly higher percentage of children rate the freedoms granted to them by
their parents very positively or positively, although 16% rate this negatively or neu-
trally. A positive rating of school is found in 79% of the children; 21% give negative
to neutral ratings—making this the largest negative to neutral proportion (Fig. 2.2).
Evidently, in the latest Child Study, just as in 2010, it is the relation between
individual freedoms on the one side and regulations and duties on the other side that
concerns children in this age range. Despite the high level of overall satisfaction,
there are still differences: both in the evaluation of the various areas of daily life and
also against the backdrop of origins and social class. Some individual domains also
reveal age or gender effects: Younger children rate school slightly more positively
than older children; and girls also tend to rate school somewhat more positively than
boys. Vice versa, it is the younger children who somewhat less frequently give posi-
tive ratings on the freedoms granted to them by their parents. Girls, in contrast, give
more positive ratings than boys here. However, when it comes to overall life satis-
faction, we once again find no significant differences in terms of age and gender. We
shall address these points in later chapters in which we shall consider them in detail.
When commencing the topic of justice, it is important at this point to bear in mind
the very positive view of their own lives and see this as a backdrop to the children’s
ideas about “justice.”
28 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
„How happy are you with“ Very positive Positive Negative to neutral
Life in general 59 32 9
Circle of friends 62 32 6
Leisure 58 33 9
Parental care 54 34 12
Freedoms allowed by
43 41 16
parents
School 42 37 21
2.2 Fears
Fears and anxieties are normal phenomena accompanying children throughout their
development. As mid-childhood progresses, the early fears of loss or also the fan-
tasy fears in what is called the “magical phase” give way increasingly to real fears.
This only becomes critical when fears not only accumulate but also exert an influ-
ence on everyday behavior and start to become chronic. However, this is generally
an exception for children in this age range.4
In the Child Study, we also check through a list of fears with the children. We
mention problems related to daily life (such as school grades or the fear of being
bullied) as well as risks related to society (such as poverty or environmental pollu-
tion) and then ask the children whether this hardly, ever, sometimes, or very often
makes them afraid. A comparison of their replies with the results of the last Child
Study in 2010 shows that the proportion of children who “sometimes” or “very
often” are afraid of one or more of the items on our list has declined. This matches
the aforementioned slight increase in overall life satisfaction. Taken as a whole, at
81%, the majority of children report sometimes being afraid in at least one of the
domains surveyed.
The most frequent fear named by 46% of the children (compared to 51% in
2010) is that of poverty (“that there are more and more people who are poor in
4
Only roughly 10% of those anxieties in children and adolescents that meet the criteria for an anxi-
ety disorder actually become chronic (Woodward and Fergusson 2001).
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 29
Very often
9 48 2010
Bad school grades
6 42 2013
8 39
Being bullied or hit
8 34
8 31
Parental unemployment
6 26
Increased number of 12 51
poor people 11 46
10 44
Environmental pollution
9 40
17 43
War
13 39
3
5 17
Increased number
4 19
of foreigners
Germany”). This is followed by the fear of having poor school grades in 42% (48%
in 2010). Then 40% (44% in 2010) mention environmental pollution; 39% (43% in
2010) that there might be a war; 34% (39% in 2010) that they are afraid of being
bullied or hit; 26% (31% in 2010) that they are afraid of their parents becoming
unemployed; and 19% (17% in 2010) that they are afraid of “more and more for-
eigners” coming to Germany. In general, the children report that these topics “some-
times” made them afraid. The proportion of children who reply “very often” is more
or less markedly below 10%. The only exception is fear of war with 13% (17% in
2010) reporting that they are “very often” afraid of this (see Fig. 2.3).
The older children aged 11–12 most frequently report on existing fears and anxi-
eties. Conspicuously high at 52% are fears about poor school grades; at 55%, the
fear of increasing poverty; and at 50%, the fear of increasing environmental pollu-
tion. These real anxieties reflect a growing problem awareness and confirm the
increasing significance of these issues for children as they mature (see Table 2.2).
Girls tend to report more fears than boys. For example, 36% of girls report being
afraid of being bullied or beaten compared to 32% of boys. Fear of growing envi-
ronmental pollution is reported by 42% of girls compared to 38% of boys. This
inverts for the item on the migration of more foreigners to Germany. Only 17% of
the girls are afraid of this compared to 21% of the boys (see Table 2.2).
That girls report fears somewhat more frequently is not a new trend; it could
already be seen in our previous Child Studies. One common explanation for this is
that boys tend to be raised to be “protectors and heroes” whereas girls are raised to
be “those in need of protection,” and that both sexes reproduce corresponding role
30 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
5
In this context, Fehr et al. (2008) talk about “inequality aversion” as the characteristic feature of
(moral) development in children of this age.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 31
The issue of how to achieve social justice is particularly central for modern demo-
cratic societies with their aspirations to civil freedoms, basic social rights, and
chances of participation. It is correspondingly important to look at the socialization
of children in relation to these problems or, from our perspective, how they socialize
themselves in relation to them over the course of their development.
32 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
6
Here, we are referring to empirical surveys and not experimental studies in which children’s
behavior is observed in, for example, a play experiment.
7
This approach is known as “triangulation” in social science research. It combines qualitative and
quantitative methods systematically in order to exploit their different strengths and create useful
synergies.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 33
(if everybody else can do it, I want to be able to do it myself), but it can simultane-
ously also express the wish and the need for all to have equal chances and participa-
tion (everybody should take part).
One interesting finding is that achievement-related justice does not seem to play
any role in the children’s unstructured responses, even though it is such a central
aspect. In the present context, this may also be because the open questioning of
what the children understand by justice was prepared and framed by the fully stan-
dardized questions in the pilot test interview. These examined how fair the children
considered things to be in their environment by asking them to appraise the way
specific groups are treated in Germany (children and adolescents, the aged, the
handicapped, foreigners, poor people). Hence, the frames of reference that the chil-
dren applied when answering the unstructured question on what they understand by
justice had been primed in advance in the sense of ensuring the well-being of others.
As the results on the qualitative part of the study show, the achievement aspect is
also taken into account, for example, when justice is linked to the children’s demands
for mutuality and reciprocity.
34 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
To enable children to appraise justice with the closest possible reference to their
daily lives and experiences, we used everyday questions or little scenarios in both
the questionnaire and the qualitative interviews. The children should judge the sin-
gle scenarios according to what they consider just. The representative part of the
Child Study draws on the categorization of the unstructured answers from the pilot
study. This targeted egalitarian, need-related, procedural, and interactional justice.
Table 2.4 presents a general overview of the results. It reports the percentages of
children per age group and gender who answered very fair or quite fair.
Some families have very little money; others have a great deal of it. Do you think that is
very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
The first question refers to egalitarian ideas about justice using the example of
differences in the distribution of income (and wealth). Two out of three children find
the unequal distribution to be unfair (23% very unfair, 43% rather unfair), and
accordingly, only a minority consider it to be fair. The differences between age
groups actually tend to be slight because, at 14%, younger children somewhat more
frequently give no reply.
Differentiated according to gender, girls at 67% consider it to be slightly more
unfair than boys at 65%. However, a larger difference in the appraisals is found
when replies are differentiated according to the children’s social background. At
81%, lower-class children find the financial inequality between families to be mark-
edly more unfair than the other classes. The unfairness appraisals of upper-class
children are slightly above the average at 69% (Fig. 2.4).
Our findings link up with Fehr et al.’s (2008) experimental studies by also reveal-
ing an inequality aversion. In the eyes of these children, the need for equality means
“unequal is unjust” (egalitarian justice). The feeling of injustice is frequently par-
ticularly marked when the causes of this inequality are viewed negatively, or when
the reasons for some having more and others having less are unknown. Both can
apply in this case. However, the differentiations according to the children’s social
class reveal an important aspect: The children’s evaluations are not independent
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 35
Rather unfair
Very unfair
44
41 46
42 44
37
25 21 23
19
Lower class Lower middle Middle class Upper middle Upper class
class class
from their social circumstances. Evidently, unequal is not just unfair in general;
for children from the lowest social class, it is quite practically oppressive and
discriminatory.
A group of children at the daycare center want to go on a trip together. It has been
decided that rich parents should pay more money for the trip than poor parents. Do you
think that is very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
Answers to the second question on need-related justice using the example that
wealthier families should pay more of the costs take the opposite direction. This is
judged to be fair by 61% of the children (25% very fair, 36% quite fair). Only a
minority of 27% think that this is unfair. Interestingly, the appraisal that it is fair to
impose the stronger and therefore unequal burden on wealthier parents increases
with age (as expected, 21% of the 6- to 7-year-olds disproportionately frequently
give no response). At 65%, boys agree markedly more frequently than girls at 57%.
It is notable that at 69%, children from the lowest social class most frequently con-
sider that unequal payments by poor and rich parents are fair. The lowest level of
agreement at 56% is found in the middle class, whereas 64% of the children from
both the higher classes think that this is fair (Fig. 2.5).
The unequal payments by parents depending on their financial resources can also
be interpreted as a wish for equality, because children may well have experienced
that their peers from poorer families are unable to join them on, for example, school
excursions because they are too expensive. If “those who have more also contribute
more,” then this leads to more equal treatment, because it creates conditions in
which everybody can come (participatory justice). This is joined by the idea of shar-
ing, that those who have more are obliged to give something up and share their
wealth—an idea that the children already expressed in 2010.
36 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
rather fair
very fair
33
38
32 44
34
36
26 22 26
20
Lower Class Lower middle Middle Class Upper middle Upper class
Class class
Fig. 2.5 Justice is when those who have more also contribute more
“A group of children at the daycare center want to go on a trip together. It has been decided that
rich parents should pay more money for the trip than poor parents”
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
Mostly it is adults—and not the children themselves—who decide where and when to
build a children’s playground. This is because they say that they are the ones who know
best about such things. Do you think that is very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very
unfair?
The third question on procedural justice using the example of how frequently
children are unable to contribute to decisions also receives a clear answer. A total of
62% find the lack of codetermination unfair (21% very unfair, 41% rather unfair).
At 12%, younger children somewhat more frequently give no reply, so that the pro-
portion of this group who find this unfair is slightly lower at 57%. At 64%, boys
even find the lack of codetermination somewhat more frequently unfair than girls at
59%. The differences according to social origins are less clear here. Upper-class
children are more self-confident, whereas the appraisals of the other children vary
only slightly (Fig. 2.6).
Equality also means having equal rights. When decisions are made in Germany
about things that are important for children, then “fair” means that the children have
a voice here. Fairness and equal treatment also mean that children should also be
informed about what is being planned and have a voice in any decision making. At
this point, the children’s perspective addresses the procedural principle and is less
concerned with their feeling the need to or even the competence to intervene actively
in the debates. Nonetheless, the view that the majority of children of this age trust
in authority and are unable to question the rules of the adult world is no longer
appropriate. Of course, the process of individual maturation is involved here, and it
continues to be appropriate to assume that children of this age seek to adapt the
views of adults. However, this does not mean that they consider it to be fair to be
excluded from decisions that affect them.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 37
Rather unfair
Very unfair
36 39 42 44
40
25 24 20 21
19
Lower class Lower middle Middle class Upper middle Upper class
class class
What would you think if non-German children had to speak only German not just in
class but also at the class breaks: Do you think that is very fair, quite fair, rather unfair,
or very unfair?
Rather unfair
Very unfair
36
33
29
27
25
22 26
12 14 17
Lower class Lower middle Middle class Upper middle Upper class
class class
Fig. 2.7 Fair also means not always wanting to forbid or prescribe everything
“What would you think if non-German children had to speak only German not just in class but also
at the class breaks”
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
case, that you can do the things you want to do. The clear class dependence in
response behavior additionally indicates that being prescribed which language one
can use with one’s peers is clearly an unjustified disciplining for children from
higher classes and therefore conceived as unequal treatment. Children from the
lower classes, particularly when they are native Germans, reveal a markedly more
distanced attitude here. This leads us to ask which children profit from multilingual-
ism at school. Children who themselves have a low socioeconomic status may feel
more threatened by other languages in the school yard, because they do not under-
stand them and may feel excluded by them during school breaks. Children from
higher classes have more positive experiences through travelling to distant countries
and more often attend schools at which social conflicts less frequently shape
daily life.
As shown above, children’s ideas of justice are shaped strongly by the characteristic
need for equality in this age range. We have also adopted this finding for our qualita-
tive interviews in which we address equal treatment, equal claims, equal rights, and
equal duties.
This once again reveals that equality refers not only to participation (equal
chances) but also to reciprocity (mutuality). As a result, the children are quite capa-
ble of interpreting it multidimensionally. Depending on the situation and the back-
ground, the postulate of equality can lead to different ideas of justice. We shall
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 39
illustrate the different interpretations children use when judging justice by two
examples taken from the qualitative interviews. We have chosen the thoughts of an
11-year-old and those of a 7-year-old. The following case report was read out loud
to both children:
Next month, a famous circus will be coming to town. The children in a school class would
really like to go to the circus and see the show. Although the class has its own savings
account with money to pay for excursions, there is not enough money to pay for tickets for
everybody to go to the circus. The class is discussing what they can do. The children come
up with the idea of holding a garage sale. All the children should bring things they no lon-
ger need from home so that they can be put on sale.
On the day of the garage sale, all the children bring things to be sold. Only Jana and
Emil come with empty hands. Anton and Luise, in contrast, have brought a particularly
large number of things. What do you think: Should all the children be able to go to the
circus?
Following the questions on the ideas of justice, we used the fully standardized sur-
vey to see how children more generally view justice in the family, the circle of
friends, at school, in Germany, and throughout the world. They could give their
answers and ratings on a four-smiley scale (very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very
unfair). If the children either could not or did not want to give an answer, the inter-
viewer entered them into the “don’t know” category.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 41
On the whole, just about every child gives an appraisal of justice in her or his
immediate world, that is, in the family, in the circle of friends, and at school.
However, 29% (and thereby 39% of the 6- to 7-year-olds, 29% of the 8- to 9-year-
olds, and 21% of the 10- to 11-year-olds) are unable to or do not want to answer the
question on whether things are fair in Germany. Findings are similar for the item on
justice in the whole world. Here, 26% (and thereby 45% of the 6- to 7-year-olds,
25% of the 8- to 9-year-olds, but, in this case only 13% of the 10- to 11-year-olds)
are unable to or do not want to answer the question.8 In light of the intensity of
development occurring particularly in middle childhood between the ages of 6 and
11 years, this low level of “don’t knows” does not surprise us. Quite the opposite:
From our perspective, this also reveals the aforementioned ability of children in this
age range to give appraisals of “fairness” based on how they understand their per-
sonal environment and look out beyond this to Germany as well as the whole world.
A look at the answers (Fig. 2.8) reveals that, analogue to their reports on personal
well-being, just about all 6- to 11-year olds (92%) describe the situation in their own
family as being either very fair or quite fair. The same holds for interactions in their
own circle of friends: 90% describe these as being either very fair or quite fair. This
indicates that that the children have a very positive sense of justice particularly
because of the personal intimacy and the major significance of these relationships.
Perhaps interactional justice and procedural justice, which are linked closely to per-
sonal interactions for children and not to abstract procedures, can be experienced
primarily in personal relationships. Here as well, it can be seen that promoting child
well-being and granting children “justice” from the side of the parents is valued in
families.
However, differences emerge when it comes to judging school. Here, 78% rate
how they are treated there as being very or quite fair. At 19%, already almost every
fifth child reports that school is rather or very unfair (for more on this, see the next
Chap. 4 on school).
Only 47% consider that Germany is fair; 24% that it is unfair. In clear contrast,
however, a mere 16% of the children believe that it is a fair world compared to 57%
who consider the way the world works and the way it is to be unfair. What seems to
be decisive for this appraisal is the children’s association of the questions with the
“Third World” and all the problems and injustices to be found there (hunger, war,
and a general lack of protection of children). This information is conveyed by, for
example, the media and evidently the strong impression this makes shapes their
frame of reference.
In the view of a very large majority of the children, family offers a home that you
can rely on and in which things are done fairly. The circle of friends offers a social
environment that is independent of this. Here, you can try things out for yourself
and play with others. Here as well, it is what children feel to be fair that shapes their
interaction. At the same time, children do not view Germany as a whole as an
8
Those 6- to 7-year-olds who were unable to answer the question on justice in Germany and were,
therefore, no longer asked about justice in the world are also assigned to the “don’t know/no
answer” group here.
42 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
Very fair Quite fair Rather unfair Very unfair Don't know
In the family 45 47 5 3
At school 22 56 16 3 3
In Germany 9 38 21 3 29
In the world 3 13 40 18 26
“Island of the Blessed,” despite the high level of satisfaction with their lives.
Nonetheless, compared to the rest of the world, they do feel “privileged” as children
in Germany and in a much better position than those above all in the Third World.
We also studied what children think about “social justice” in Germany by asking
them how fairly they consider certain groups are treated. It is easier for children to
relate their idea of justice to the question of how people are handled—equal or
unequal (unjust) treatment. This does not correspond directly to the usual distribu-
tive concept of empirical justice research with its orientation toward a (welfare)
distribution, but is easier for children to grasp.9 However, here as well, the challenge
facing children is to perform general appraisals of the situation of certain social
groups in Germany. In contrast to the (previously presented) question on general
fairness in Germany, these items focus more on single groups (children and youths,
the aged, the handicapped, foreigners,10 poor people) and are therefore easier for the
children to grasp in relation to their daily lives.
9
The usual question in the Sozialstaatssurvey is “What do you think? How far has Germany
achieved a fair distribution of wealth?” In contrast, personal distributive justice is tapped in, for
example, ALLBUS with the question: “Compared to how other people live here in Germany, do
you consider that you receive your fair share, more than your fair share, or somewhat less or some-
what more?”
10
Instead of the term foreigner, which is based on formal nationality, research in the social sciences
uses the category migration background. This places the focus on one’s own origin or also the
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 43
Very fair Quite fair Rather unfair Very unfair Don't know
Children or
11 55 21 3 10
adolescents
Old people 19 45 20 3 13
Disabled 13 38 28 7 14
Foreigners 10 38 34 6 12
Poor people 7 23 42 15 13
Includes only those 6-to 7-year-olds who were able to answer the question on justice in Germany
The proportion of children who are unable to answer this question lies between
10% and 15%. We already dropped these questions in advance for those of the
youngest children (6- to 7-year-olds) who had been unable to answer the previous
question on general fairness in Germany.11 Considering these cases as well, the
proportion of children without valid reports rises up to between 21 and 25% depend-
ing on the research question.
In the following, we shall refer only to those children who were asked the ques-
tions on “social justice” in Germany (Fig. 2.9). The majority say that children and
adolescents (11% very fair, 55% quite fair) and also the aged (19% very fair, 45%
quite fair) are treated fairly. Results are comparable, though slightly lower for the
treatment of the disabled (13% consider this to be very fair in Germany and 38%
fair) and likewise for the treatment of foreigners (10% very fair, 38% quite fair). In
contrast, the majority rate the treatment of poor people in Germany negatively (30%
fair and 57% rather unfair).12
origin of one’s parents as well as the associated cultural roots and commitment. Nonetheless, “for-
eigner” [Ausländer] continues to be the usual term in everyday speech.
11
For older children who were also unable to answer the questions, we also used the option of a
“don’t know” category.
12
According to the Sozialstaatssurvey, approximately 80% of the adult population last reported in
2008 that Germany had either “rather not” or “completely not” attained a fair distribution of wealth
(Glatzer 2009). According to Allensbach (2013), 69% of the over-16-year-old population reported
“unfair” when answering the question: “Are the economic conditions here in Germany—by which
44 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
2.6.1 F
orming Groups: Attitudes of Children Toward Social
Justice in Germany
I mean what people own and how much they earn—basically fair or unfair?” According to the
German General Social Survey (ALLBUS) in 2010, approximately 45% of the adult population
considered that they received less than their fair share (ALLBUS: Datenreport 2011).
13
This is also confirmed in a reliability and factor analysis. All five questions form the same dimen-
sion and can therefore be viewed and analyzed together.
14
We did this by adding up the values (very fair = 1, quite fair = 2, don’t know = 3, rather unfair =
4, very unfair = 5) and then grouping the children according to the central tendency. All weightings
above the median indicate a negative judgment of social justice; we viewed those higher than the
(statistical) standard deviation as a very negative judgment. Weightings below the median, in con-
trast, represent a positive appraisal of social justice in Germany, and those lower than the standard
deviation as correspondingly very positive.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 45
Partially negative
Fairly positve 30%
35%
Only children who were able to answer the question on justice in Germany
The majority of children in this group also consider that the social groups we
asked about are treated fairly (fair treatment of children and adolescents: 78%, fair
treatment of foreigners: 62%). However, only a minority of 27% consider that poor
people are treated fairly.
Children with a partially negative view of social justice in Germany: 30%.
About one-half of the children in this group consider the treatment of children
and adolescents as well as the aged to be fair. In contrast, only one in five consider
the treatment of the disabled or foreigners in German to be fair, and only a small
minority of 9% consider that poor people receive fair treatment.
Children with a very negative view of social justice in Germany: 13%.
The majority of children in this group consider that nearly all the social groups
surveyed are treated unfairly in Germany.
Hence, in all, children’s opinions on social justice in Germany vary greatly. At
57%, the majority take a positive position. Nonetheless, this still leaves 43% with a
more negative view of social justice in the country. Viewed in context, at 64%, lower
class children significantly more frequently have a negative perception of social
justice in Germany. At 49%, the appraisal of children growing up in a one-parent
family is also significantly more negative. Independent of social status, girls at 46%
also significantly more frequently give more negative appraisals than boys. Younger
children, in contrast, tend to view things more positively. Looked at in context, the
place of residence (Eastern or Western Germany, urban or rural), in contrast, is not
significant. A migration background is also, in itself, without effect. According to
46 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
our multivariate statistical analysis, the existing bivariate differences are due far
more to social origin (Table 2.5).
It can be seen that alongside age and gender (indicating that girls have a greater
sensitivity to injustice in the treatment of disadvantaged persons), the social situa-
tion of the children shapes their view of social justice.
As a result, it is also not very surprising that the majority of children with a very
positive or positive personal life satisfaction also evaluate social justice in Germany
positively, whereas the majority of children with a negative to neutral evaluation of
their personal life satisfaction also evaluate “social justice in Germany” negatively.
Here as well, it is the social class of origin and everyday social experiences that
decisively influence personal life satisfaction and the appraisal of “social justice.”
An important backdrop when appraising justice is also whether the children them-
selves feel disadvantaged in their daily lives or whether this is generally not the
case. In the current 2013 World Vision Child Study, we asked about perceived dis-
advantages in relation to different domains (Fig. 2.11).
About one-third of the children report feeling disadvantaged (sometimes or
often) because of their age. Thirty-four percent of 6- to 7-year-olds and 36% of 8- to
9-year-olds say this slightly more often than 10- to 11-year-olds at 29%. Because of
their gender, 20% of girls feel disadvantaged in daily life compared to only 8% of
boys. Although younger girls report this somewhat more frequently, the difference
between boys and girls is retained in all age groups. Because of what they look like,
14% of children, 15% of girls, and 12% of boys feel disadvantaged; and 12% of the
children report feeling disadvantaged because their parents do not have much
money. We asked only children with a migration background about disadvantage
due to origin. Of these children, 21% report feeling disadvantaged in daily life
because their parents do not come from Germany.
Here as well, we decided to summarize different experiences of disadvantage
that cannot be clearly separated from each other in order to gain a global picture. We
once again formed a summary indicator and grouped children according to whether
they reported disadvantage hardly ever, sometimes, or often.15
At 56%, the majority of children report being hardly ever disadvantaged. The
second group contains 34% of the children. These report feeling sometimes disad-
vantaged. In most cases, these disadvantages are because of age (two-thirds in this
group) and sometimes because of their gender or their external appearance. The
third group contains 10% of the children. They report often being disadvantaged in
15
We also summed the ratings here (often = 3, sometimes = 2, hardly ever/not at all = 1, don’t
know/no reply = 0), and grouped them according to their central tendency. Up to and including the
median, there are practically no experiences of disadvantage; deviations above the median and up
to the standard deviation, sometimes; and above the standard deviation, often.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 47
daily life. Most frequently, this is because of their age (four out of five in this group),
but also because of their gender or external appearance (more than two-thirds), and
finally because their parents do not have enough money (more than one-half of this
group).
What is decisive here is, once again, the social background and the family situa-
tion. Both significantly more frequently result in children feeling disadvantaged in
daily life (Fig. 2.12). At 68%, two out of three lower-class children report disadvan-
tage (sometimes and often combined). Findings are similar for children who have
an unemployed parent (68%). Notably, 51% of children in single-parent families
and 50% of children in families with three or more children tend to report being
disadvantaged in daily life compared to children living in other types of family. The
significance of this finding was also confirmed in a multivariate statistical analysis.16
Here we carried out an ordinal regression on our newly formed and grouped variable “Perceived
16
disadvantage in daily life: often, sometimes, or never.” Gender, social class, form of family, unem-
48 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
Often
Because of my age 4 29
Sometimes
Fig. 2.11 Domains in which children feel being treated unfairly in everyday life
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
Often
All children 10 34
Sometimes
Unemployed parent(s) 26 42
Migration background 16 35
Single parent 14 37
Statistically significant variables in a multivariate regression analysis after controlling for age
The same applies for children with a migration background. When asked about felt
disadvantage, at 51%, children with a non-German background report significantly
As this chapter has shown, children demand fairness in the ways they interact
with each other personally along with equal opportunities so that everybody can
take part. In the modern sociopolitical discussion, this attitude is labeled “participa-
50 U. Schneekloth and S. Andresen
tory justice.” It focuses on ensuring and systematically extending the right of all
individuals in society “to live in this world” in the sense of enabling them to deter-
mine for themselves what they rationally wish to strive toward. What is remarkable
is how well the children’s perspective and their desire for equality fit in with the
debate on social equality. At the end of the day, the main concern in the “grown-up”
world is also that “you share with the others, and don’t leave anybody just standing
there” (7-year-old girl).
References
Allbus: Datenreport. (2011). Ein Sozialbericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Bonn:
Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung.
Allensbacher Institut für Demoskopie. (2013). Was ist gerecht? Gerechtigkeitsbegriff und -wah-
rnehmung der Bürger. Allensbacher Archiv, IfD-Umfrage 11001, Allensbach.
Dalbert, C. (1983). Die Gerechte-Welt-Skala (GWS). In Preiser, S. (Hrsg.), Soziales Handeln
im Kindes- und Jugendalter (Dokumentation von Forschungs- und Diagnoseinstrumenten).
Weinheim: Beltz, S. 139.
Dalbert, C., Montada, L., & Schmitt, M. (1987). Glaube an eine gerechte Welt als Motiv:
Validierungskorrelate zweier Skalen. Psychologische Beiträge, 29, S. 596 – 615.
Essau, C. A. (2003). Angst bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Stuttgart: UTB.
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. In Nature
(Vol. 454, pp. 1079–1083).
Glatzer, W. (2009). Gefühlte (Un)Gerechtigkeit. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte, 47 / 2009, S. 15 – 20.
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encourages
equal sharing in children but not chimpanzees. In: Nature, doi:10.1038/nature10278. http://
wkprc.eva.mpg.de/pdf/2011/Hamann_Warnek_Greenberg_Tomasello_2011.pdf (Stand:
20.08.2013).
HBSC-Team Deutschland. (2011). Studie Health Behaviour in School-aged Children – Faktenblatt
»Lebenszufriedenheit von Kindern und Jugendlichen”. Bielefeld: WHO Collaborating Centre
for Child and Adolescent Health Promotion.
Kohlberg, L. (1964). The development of moral character and ideology. In M. L. Hoffmann (Ed.),
Review of child development research. New York: Wiley.
Kohlberg, L. (1974). Zur kognitiven Entwicklung des Kindes. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp.
Liebig, S., & Lengfeld, H. (Hrsg.). (2002). Interdisziplinäre Gerechtigkeitsforschung. Zur
Verknüpfung empirischer und normativer Perspektiven. Hamburg: Campus.
Liebig, S., & May, M. (2009). Dimensionen sozialer Gerechtigkeit. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte,
47 / 2009, S. 3 – 8.
Morschitzky, H. (2009). Angststörungen. Diagnostik, Konzepte, Theorie, Selbsthilfe (4. Auflage).
Heidelberg: Springer.
Oerter, R., & Montada, L. (Hrsg.). (2002). Entwicklungspsychologie (5. vollständig überarbeitete
Auflage). Weinheim: Beltz.
Piaget, J. (1932/ 1983). Das moralische Urteil beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett.
Piaget, J. (1937 /1975). Der Aufbau der Wirklichkeit beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett.
Schneekloth, U., & Pupeter, M. (2010b). Wohlbefinden, Wertschätzung, Selbstwirksamkeit:
Was Kinder für ein gutes Leben brauchen. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in
Deutschland 2010b. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 187 – 221). Frankfurt a. M: Fischer.
Sen, A. (2010). Die Idee der Gerechtigkeit. München: C. H. Beck Verlag.
2 What’s Fair and What’s Unfair: The Different Faces of Justice 51
Trautner, H. M. (1991). Lehrbuch der Entwicklungspsychologie (Bd. 2: Theorien und Befunde).
Göttingen: Hogrefe.
UNICEF. (2013). Child well-being in rich countries. A comparative overview (Innocenti Report
Card 11 (UNICEF)).
Woodward, L. J., & Fergusson, D. M. (2001). Life course of young people with anxiety disorders
in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40,
1086–1093.
Chapter 3
Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also
Marked Differences In Life Conditions
Ulrich Schneekloth and Monika Pupeter
In their everyday lives, their own family is a kind of “safe haven” for children.
Normally, it meets their everyday needs while providing quality relationships, emo-
tional security, and privacy (Schneewind 2008). The family is characterized by its
specific social capital: the strong ties forged by the close and emotionally based
relationships between its members (Coleman 1988). Accordingly, the family is
where children and adults interact with a strong emotional commitment. However,
as the two earlier Child Studies have shown, the forms the family takes can vary
greatly. First, it can contain either just one or several children. Second, parents can
be married to each other or live together in a domestic partnership; they can be
separated but both rearing their children together or be single parents; and, finally,
they can be biological parents, adoptive parents, or a combination of the two.
In this chapter, we wish to describe the familial and social background to the
children’s lives. Because we have already reported on this in detail in the two earlier
Child Studies, this study will focus more on the relevant trends that have become
recognizable since the first Child Study in 2007. Alongside the family situation, we
shall consider a possible migration background (in the parents) as well as the chil-
dren’s social origins. Of course, one important aspect here is whether there is also a
potential risk of poverty. In this new Child Study, we have broadened the indicators
used to describe experiences of poverty from the children’s perspective. The new
findings this has generated will also be reported in this chapter. It will close by
reporting on how the children themselves rate their family care situation.
As reported in the previous Child Studies, there have been marked changes to the
characteristic form of family life over the last decades. The typical family in post-
war Germany from the 1950s onward was the nuclear family composed of father,
mother, and (at that time still) two children all living together in one household until
the children reached adulthood. The division of labor was organized generally with
the man as the sole wage earner who ensured the family livelihood and the woman
as responsible for the domestic tasks of looking after the home and children-at least
as long as the children were still small or attending school.
It is not just recently that this ideal of the “conventional nuclear family” has lost its
decisive role as a lifestyle model. This change can already be seen in the forms of
family in which today’s 6- to 11-year-old children are growing up.
We classified the different types of family according to the following features:
• “Marital status” of the parents
• “Completeness” in the sense of a one- or two-parent family with either biological
or adoptive parents
• “Size,” that is, the number of children living in the household as well as the num-
ber of generations
What has not changed at all is that children (between the ages of 6 and 11 years)
still live predominately in nuclear families (Fig. 3.1).1 Three-generation families,
that is, children, parents, and grandparents all living together under one roof, are a
very rare exception (they make up less than 1%). In contrast, family structures have
become more differentiated: Only 39% of children in this age range live in a “two-
child family” together with a sibling, and only a further 20% in a family with two or
more siblings. A total of 14% of 6- to 11-year-olds are growing up as an only child.
What has remained stable at approximately 17% over the last 6 years since our first
Child Study is the proportion of children growing up with a single parent.2 Findings
are similar for nonmarried life partnerships and also for adoptive families (married,
but at least one nonbiological parent) in which 5% and 4% of the children respec-
tively are growing up.
1
In this context, the nuclear family means the “two-generation family” living together in one
household with (biological) parents. Children living in residential homes are generally not consid-
ered here, because they still make up less than 0.5% of children in this age range despite a slight
absolute increase over the last few years.
2
Here, we are referring to the results of the parent survey in which we gathered some important
sociodemographic background information on the children’s family situation.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 55
If we just look at the number of siblings, regardless of which type of family they
belong to, we can see a trend toward a slow but continuous growth in the proportion
of children growing up in households without siblings over the last 6 years
(Table 3.1).3 We had already observed this trend in the last Child Study. Against this
background, we also see no reason to change our conclusion that the proportion of
children growing up without their own siblings will probably increase even further
in the future. Because a major proportion of children will accordingly experience
neither older nor younger siblings in their own family, experiences with “peers” will
need to be organized in other ways. In early childhood, this can be provided either
informally through self-organized nursery groups but also, of course, through insti-
tutional care services such as day nurseries or preschools.
3
Here, we are referring specifically to those siblings (of all age groups) who are living in the same
household. However, also including those siblings who have left the parental home and are
(already) living in their own households does not alter the trend reported here.
56 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
Taken together, 73% of children are growing up in a “normal family” with their
married biological parents. A further 21% are living in a recombined family either
with a single parent or also with one adoptive parent. Finally, 5% are growing up
with unmarried (biological or also nonbiological) parents. Even though these pro-
portions have remained relatively stable over the last 6 years, as pointed out in the
last Child Study, this makes absolutely no difference to the need to respond to the
challenge to deliver child-friendly social policies that will also ensure material secu-
rity and equal opportunities for children growing up in patchwork families or with
unmarried parents.
3.2 L
abor Participation of Parents: A Distribution of Roles
and No Longer “Just the Father’s Business”
In the last Child Study, we already pointed to the trend toward a changing distribu-
tion of roles within families. Being gainfully employed and thus securing the fami-
ly’s subsistence is no longer primarily the concern of fathers. In the current Child
Study, we also surveyed the parents alongside their children, and these findings
confirm the further decline in the traditional “one-male-breadwinner” family
(Fig. 3.2).
Only 32% of children are still growing up in nuclear families (in this case, fami-
lies with two parents without taking marriage status into account) in which only one
parent—usually the father—is gainfully employed. In our last Child Study in 2010,
this group still contained 40% of children, and in our first Child Study of 2007, it
Fig. 3.2 Labor
42
participation of Parents One parent employed 40
Children in Germany 32
aged 6–11 years (2007:
8–11 years) (%) 25
One parent full-time, one
30
part-time/both part-time
35
10
Both parents full-time 10 2007
13 2010
2013
10
Single parent, full- or part-
11
time
12
8
Parent(s) unemployed 5
4
5
Other 4
4
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 57
was even 42%. These children are joined once again by 12% who are growing up
with a single parent who is simultaneously gainfully employed. The most frequent
constellation that is now found in 35% of 6- to 11-year-olds is two gainfully
employed parents—mostly the father working full-time and the mother either part-
time or at least marginally. “Both parents working full-time” now applies to 13% of
the children in our age group, thereby showing a slight increase. Only 4% of chil-
dren are growing up in families with one (or at least one) unemployed parent, and a
further 4% in other constellations (parents or single parent in training or studying,
parents or single parent not gainfully employed for other reasons).
There is a tendency for somewhat older children to have two gainfully employed
parents. However, the real relationship is to the number of children in the house-
hold: the more children in the household, the greater the probability that only one
parent—generally the father—will pursue gainful employment. When there is only
one child (in the household), this applies to 17%; for children with one sibling in the
household, to 31%; and for children with two or more siblings, to 56%. Vice versa,
19% of single children have two full-time employed parents, 12%, of those with one
sibling, and no more than 8% of those with two or more siblings.
The increase in the gainful employment of both parents is due decisively to the
strong recovery of the labor market in Germany over the last 6 years—and above all
since the last Child Study in 2010. This allows more people in general to find gain-
ful employment. The significance of the tendency to engage in gainful employment
and, in this case, the accompanying difference in the distribution of roles within the
family becomes even more visible when we also take into account where the chil-
dren live—in this case, differentiated according to the old and new German states
(West vs. East). Typical for the former Western states (including Berlin) is now two
gainfully employed parents for 47% of the children, although generally with the
father in full-time and the mother in part-time or marginal employment (36%). Only
11% of the children live in families in which both parents are employed full-time.
In the Western states, 34% of children are growing up in families with one gainfully
employed parent. In former East Germany (excluding Berlin), in contrast, at 30%, a
large proportion of the children in this age range are growing up in families in which
both parents are gainfully employed. For 25%, it is generally the father who is full-
time and the mother who has part-time or marginal employment. Only 19% of the
children in the East are growing up in nuclear families with only one gainfully
employed parent (Table 3.2).
58 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
3.2.1 M
others More Frequently Want Their Own (Part-Time)
Gainful Employment
In the present Child Study, we ask the mothers providing information on their chil-
dren to appraise their own employment situation.4 Results are very clear: Whereas
mothers who are already employed are generally satisfied with this, the majority of
not employed mothers would like to have their own (part-time) gainful
employment.
Among the employed mothers of children in our age range, it is those who work
part-time who are most satisfied with their working hours (Table 3.3): 74% report
that “things are fine the way they are.” Findings are similar for marginally employed
mothers with 63% reporting that they are satisfied. Nonetheless, 35% of this group
4
Strictly speaking, the children’s mothers in our survey are not a representative random selection.
We recruited a representative sample of children and then gathered some background information
from one of the parents. In 83% of cases, this was the mother; in the remaining 17%, in contrast,
our information came from the father. In these latter cases, we have no self-reports on personal
preferences from the mothers. There are many signs that the father more frequently gave informa-
tion when the mother had no time because, for example, she was gainfully employed. However,
because we analyzed the desired working hours separately for employed and nonemployed moth-
ers, potential distortions should tend to be negligible.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 59
would like to work more. Among mothers with full-time employment, the majority
at 59% are also satisfied with their working hours. However, 35% would prefer to
work fewer hours.5
The situation of mothers who are not gainfully employed is completely the oppo-
site (Table 3.4). Of those who describe themselves as unemployed, 31% would like
to work full-time and 55% part-time. From the otherwise not gainfully employed
mothers, that is, those who are studying, in training, or who describe themselves as
housewives, 42% say that they would like to work part-time. However, 54% say that
“things are fine the way they are.” Finally, interesting reports come from the group
of mothers who are currently on parental leave (generally because of a younger
sibling living in the household). Of the mothers on parental leave, 43% would like
to work part-time, whereas 54% report that things are fine the way they are. In this
exceptionally small group in numerical terms, we are unable to distinguish between
mothers who are purely on parental leave and those in marginal or part-time employ-
ment. Nonetheless, their reports are interesting and underline the mothers’ desire to
have their own jobs and generally to work part-time.
To enable a comparison, here are the findings on the actual labor market partici-
pation of the mothers providing the information: in full-time employment: 20%
(West: 17%, East: 40%), part-time: 43% (West: 44%, East: 35%), marginal: 11%
(West: 12%, East: 6%), unemployed: 4% (West: 3%, East: 9%), and not working for
other reasons: 22% (West: 23%, East: 10%).
Combining the findings on desired and actual participation in employment,
mothers from the old German states (including Berlin) most frequently prefer a
part-time job. A total of 43% reported that they would prefer to work part-time or
that they are satisfied with their own part-time employment; or if they work f ull-time,
that they would prefer to work fewer hours. Only 19% prefer to work full-time,
whereas 24% of this subgroup prefer marginal employment. Among mothers from
the new German states, 39% prefer part-time employment, whereas 45% desire
5
A recent study based on the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) reveals that women working
full-time more frequently desire fewer working hours per week, whereas women working part-
time desire more working hours (Holst and Seifert 2012). However, these results are drawn on the
population of all women in gainful employment and not just those with children (aged 6–11 years).
60 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
f ull-time employment and 10% would most prefer marginal employment.6 Both the
desire for a gainful employment in principle and the desire for full-time employ-
ment are markedly stronger in Eastern compared to Western Germany.
3.2.2 E
arly Childhood Care Helps to Ensure Parental Labor
Market Participation
These different traditions in labor market participation and hence in the distribution
of family roles have quite material backgrounds. When we asked parents whether
their children had at some stage in the past attended a childcare institution, we found
marked East–West differences—particularly for children under the age of 3 years.
In former West Germany, 19% of parents report that the child we are surveying had
attended a day care. In former East Germany, this is 61%. In contrast, more than
90% of parents in both East and West report that their child had attended a
Kindergarten (Table 3.5).7
The different traditions in familial gainful employment in former East and West
Germany were—and continue to be—accompanied by a different density of ser-
vices in childcare institutions. This applies particularly to the care services for
6
Because of the differences in the questions given to employed and not employed persons, prefer-
ences do not discriminate completely between the two groups. For example, as soon as a mother
reported that she worked part-time but would prefer to work less, we assumed that she wished to
be marginally employed. The same applied in the other direction for full-time employed and also
for marginally employed persons.
7
These retrospective reports cannot be compared directly with official statistics. We asked whether
the children had at any time attended a day nursery or a Kindergarten. If, instead, as in the official
statistics, we had asked how many children per age cohort were attending a Kindergarten at that
particular point in time, we would have gained lower proportions—above all in the West. Part of
the detailed differences between East and West are due to terminological variations as well as to
the different forms of care. For the sake of simplicity, we limited our retrospective survey to insti-
tutional forms of care (day nursery, Kindergarten, daycare center) without explicitly mentioning
family day carers or other forms of privately organized care. The available official data on institu-
tional childcare nonetheless confirm the main trend and in particular the East–West differences in
the daycare of children under the age of 3 years (Bertelsmann Ländermonitor Frühkindliche
Bildungssysteme, Kinderbetreuung).
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 61
c hildren under the age of 3 years. This is why it was and continues to be usual for
women in Eastern Germany to take up employment comparatively soon after the
birth of a child as well as more frequently in general. There were and continue to be
better framing conditions for this in the form of a greater number of childcare
places.
Retrospective findings underline that women whose children had already
attended childcare services under the age of 3 years far more frequently take up
gainful employment again than women who looked after their under-3-year-old
children at home. In Western Germany, 25% of the mothers of children who had
attended a day nursery according to parent reports are in full-time employment
today compared to 15% of those whose children had not attended a day nursery,
whereas 44% of both groups work part-time. In Eastern Germany, the figures are
even 45% with day nursery compared to 29% without day nursery who currently
work full-time and 36% with day nursery compared to 33% without day nursery
who work part-time. Accordingly, it seems as if women who use childcare services
when their children are under the age of 3 years will more probably go to work
(again) as their children grow older.
In 2008, Germany passed a law in the support and care of children under the age
of 3 years at day facilities and in day care (KiFöG) that set an August 1, 2013, dead-
line. Since then, parents have the legal right to a childcare place for their child once
she or he attains the age of 12 months. This has created framing conditions designed
to level out regional disparities in the provision of daycare for children. However, its
practical implementation continues to be plagued by numerous difficulties, particu-
larly regarding the expansion of care provisions this requires. Currently, it does not
look as if the legal right to a daycare place for children under the age of 3 can actu-
ally be implemented throughout Germany in the foreseeable future.8
3.3 P
ersonal Cultural Backgrounds: “German” Children,
Migrant Parents, and Religious Traditions
A total of 34%, that is, every third child in our 2013 World Vision Child Study, has
a migration background. Most of them are born in Germany with German national-
ity and have parents who migrated to the country. In formal legal terms, according
to parent reports, 94% of children are German according to German citizenship
laws, 89% have German citizenship alone, and slightly more than 5% have dual
nationality—generally German citizenship along with the nationality of a non-
German parent.9 Accordingly, the children’s migration background refers predomi-
8
On the current state of implementation, see the most recent interim report on the evaluation of the
Childcare Funding Act (Kinderförderungsgesetz - Kifög) for 2012 (BMFSFJ, 2013a)
9
In general, these are children born in Germany of non-German parents who are obliged by
German law (29 StAG) to declare which nationality they have decided to take before the end of
their 23rd year of life.
62 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
nantly to their background of origin, or more precisely the origin of their parents.
Therefore, in the following, we talk about native German children (66%) as well as
children with a migration background (34%) or, sometimes, German children with
a migration background (28%) and non-German children who possess exclusively
non-German citizenship (6%).10
For 21% of the children with a migration background, either one or both parents
have, according to their own reports, emigrated from Turkey. Roughly a further 7%
have an Arabic background, and almost 4% come from Africa. Approximately 16%
come from the former Soviet Union and 16% from other East European countries
(Ukraine, Poland, and Rumania), frequently as ethnic German immigrants who
have German citizenship. A further 8% come from the countries making up the
former Yugoslavia. Almost 15% of the parents of children with a migration back-
ground come from Western Europe, whereas the remaining 14% are distributed
across other countries.11
It is conspicuous that children in Germany today have a whole range of different
backgrounds. Every third child in this age range has an international and therefore
also a multicultural background. A very large majority of children are not migrants
but were born in Germany. As a result, the trend toward a growing proportion of the
population with a migration background will increase further regardless of whether
more or less people migrate directly to Germany. At the current point in time, the
proportion of migrants in different regions of Germany continues to vary greatly. In
our Child Study, 38% of children in the old federal states (including Berlin) have a
migration background compared to only 9% in the new federal states. Findings are
similar for the urban–rural distribution. In urban central areas and conurbations,
almost one-half of the children have a migration background compared to no more
than 25% in the peripheries or rural areas.
10
One methodological comment at this point: Only 2.4% of the children in our actual sample were
not German citizens and, taken together, 25.4% had a migration background. Hence, in this regard,
the current sample is “better” than that in previous years. However, according to the official micro-
census, 32.7% of children had a migration background of whom 5.5% were foreigners without
German citizenship in the reference year 2011. Because of the significance that is now assigned to
a migration background, we decided to adjust our sample here by weighting it to make it more
representative. We took exactly the same approach as in the past two Child Studies and adjusted
the proportion of non-German children; however, we did not explicitly adjust the proportion of
children with a migration background.
11
These findings also match the nationalities of origin reported in the microcensus. However,
because of the low case numbers, we cannot differentiate further here.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 63
homes with a Christian faith (mostly either Catholic or Protestant depending on the
region). For at least 10% of the children, the mother is Muslim; for 13%, the father.12
A total of 20% of the mothers and 23% of the fathers have no formal religious
belief; other religions are almost completely nonsignificant (Table 3.6).
In our last Child Study, we talked about the “three religious cultures”: the nonre-
ligious East, the more plural West, and from a further perspective, the more strongly
religious migrants. This description of conditions remains unchanged. A total of
33% of the families of the children we surveyed “sometimes” or “very often” say
prayers (36% from the old and 13% from the new federal states). A total of 44% of
children with a migration background report that prayers are said at home (Table 3.7).
12
In this case, we always consider only the children with parents providing information on their
partner. These reports are missing for single parents. Because the proportion of single parents in
Germany is much lower among migrants from Muslim cultures (less than 10%), slightly more
fathers than mothers are reported as being Muslim.
64 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
In our last Child Study in 2010, 39% of the children reported that prayers are said at
home: 43% from the old federal states, 15% from the new federal states, and 51%
of children with a migration background.
In terms of religious affiliations, 37% of the children from a Christian parental
home report that they pray at home (23% “sometimes” and 14% “very often”). As
to be expected, only 9% of children from a parental home with no religious affilia-
tion (both parents with no religious affiliation) report saying prayers at home. In
contrast, 64% of children with either one or two Muslim parents pray at home (37%
“sometimes” and 27% “very often”).
3.3.2 T
he Majority of Native German Children Do Not Attend
Church Services on a Regular Basis
In all, 28% of the children attend a church, a mosque, or other place of worship
“every week” or “once or twice a month.” Of these, 30% come from the old federal
states, 11% come from the new federal states, and 33% have a migration back-
ground (Table 3.7). In our 2010 Child Study, the total was 27%, with 30% coming
from the old federal states, 10% coming from the new federal states, and 28% with
a migration background. Looking at the religious affiliations in the parental home
here as well, 34% of the children from a Christian parental home attend church
services (22% “once or twice a month” and 12% “every week”). A total of 10% of
children whose parents have no religious affiliation report attending church ser-
vices, whereas 42% of children with a Muslim parent report attending services in a
mosque (25% “once or twice a month” and 17% “every week”).
Results show some slight fluctuation over time. Praying seems to be slightly in
decline (from 39% to 33%), whereas attending religious services continues to be the
usual practice for 28% of children. All in all, religion continues to have an influence
on everyday life for only a smaller proportion of the children, and this depends on
whether they are growing up in the old or the new federal states. Children with a
migration background have a stronger religious involvement. This would seem to be
due particularly to the fact that by upholding religious traditions, parents can also
uphold and communicate the cultural values of their country of origin. For migrants,
particularly those with roots in a Muslim culture, religiosity contributes to their
sense of identity and is accordingly more important in everyday life than is gener-
ally the case for native German families.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 65
65 65 67
Mostly German
35 33 33
We also asked the children with a migration background which language they gen-
erally speak at home in their family. Looking at the trend, 67% of the children with
a migration background, and thus slightly fewer than in our two prior Child Studies,
report generally speaking German at home. Approximately one-third speak the
native language of their parents at home (Fig. 3.3).
The question on the language that is spoken at home should not be used as a
direct indicator for the frequently criticized lack of German language competence.
Multilingualism is not a deficit. It is far more the case that children have very robust
language acquisition abilities and can easily learn more than one language.
Nonetheless, the precondition for this is the presence of both languages in everyday
life and also, when needed, their focused promotion (see Jampert et al. 2009; Tracy
2007, on growing up multilingually). On the one hand, language transports and
conveys culture and tradition: the stronger the cultural ties of the family with the
country of origin, the more important it is for parents to speak their native language
at home and, in particular, with their own children. On the other hand, it is often
completely practical considerations or habit that leads parents to speak their native
language at home.
66 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
43
31
26
3.3.4 T
he Less Well-Educated the Class of Origin, the More
Frequently German Is Not Spoken at Home
Everyday language can also be a risk factor for children with a migration back-
ground. Language acquisition and language competence are general intervention
topics for children from less well-educated parental homes. This is just as much the
case for children whose parents were born and grew up in Germany as it is for those
whose parents migrated and speak another native language. A migration background
and the use of the native language in the family can given a corresponding context,
naturally also represent a hindrance in acquiring German.
The five-level class index used to differentiate the social backgrounds of children
in the World Vision Child Study also delivers important findings here. Its construc-
tion is based decisively on the parents’ educational background (see also Sect. 3.5
and Appendix 2, for the following).
It is conspicuous to see that, at 43%, children in the two lower social classes far
more frequently speak their parent’s native language at home. For middle-class chil-
dren, the proportion is 31%, and in the two upper classes, it is 26% (Fig. 3.4).
Disproportionally frequently, the parents’ native language tends to be used in the
homes of children with a Turkish (56%) and Arabic background (45%), but also in
the homes of children whose parents come from Russia and the former Soviet Union
(40%). Results are similar for children from Muslim parental homes at 52%. Here,
results correlate with class membership. Children with the corresponding migration
backgrounds belong markedly more frequently to the poorly educated lower classes
than is the case for children with parents who have migrated from other European
countries or from overseas (so-called OECD migrants).
However, another finding gives cause for concern: Looking at the children with
a migration background who speak more German at home, we find that parents
report that 28% of them used to attend a day nursery and 93% a Kindergarten. For
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 67
children with a migration background who speak the native language of their par-
ents more at home, in contrast, only 15% used to attend a day nursery and 77% a
Kindergarten. At 23%, the proportion of children who had not attended a preschool
care institution is by far the largest here (children with a migration background in
total: 23% attending a day nursery and 88% attending a Kindergarten). For native
German children, in contrast, parents report that 27% had attended a day nursery
and 96% had at some time attended a Kindergarten.
The results underline the significance of focused language interventions for cer-
tain groups of children with a migration background. Institutional care provisions
could play an important role in this. Particularly in the last 10 years and in the con-
text of what has been called a sustainable family policy, the function of the classic
daycare facilities in Germany has changed from what was primarily a care service
designed to reduce the strain on the family to an agency of early childhood educa-
tion. In this context, the comprehensive care of preschool-age children is conceived
as an active education process that offers additional opportunities for interventions.
As reported in the Twelfth Report on Children and Youth to the German federal
government (BMFSFJ 2005):
The family can pass on to and initiate in the child only that which is available to it within
the framework of its social and cultural resources. The educational background of the par-
ents, the real life situation, and the concrete living conditions have a strongly modifying
influence on which opportunities for development and education are available to children in
their family environment. (p. 33, translated)
Therefore, the report justly points out that a major task for society is to deliver qual-
ity interventions within preschool childcare that are appropriate to a child’s state of
development.
The results of our Child Study also show that early childhood language interven-
tions will succeed only if they are applied in a “culture-sensitive” manner and link
up with the traditions and values of the family of origin.
3.4 E
xperiencing Poverty: A Harsh Reality for Some
Children
In Germany, the poverty risk for children (under 18 years) currently varies depend-
ing on the source of the data between about 16% and 19% (EU-SILK 2010: 17.5%,
2011: 15.7%; Mikrozensus 2011: 18.9%; SOEP 2010: 16.5%). This places it 3–4
percentage points above the rate for the general population (BMAS 2013, p 110).
Measurements of poverty are based on the relative risk of poverty rate, that is, the
proportion of children and youths living in households with less than 60% of the
average net income of all households in Germany at their disposal.13 The measure
This was weighted according to need under the assumption that the economy of size in larger
13
households decreases the need per household member. According to the new OECD scale, the
main bread earner in a household is weighted with the factor 1.0. All other household members
68 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
over the age of 14 are weighed at 0.4 and children under 14 at 0.3. Hence, in a four-person house-
hold with two adult parents and two children under the age of 14, the net income of the household
is divided by the need factor 2.0 (1.0 + 0.4 + 0.3 + 0.3) and allocated to the individuals.
14
In both the Second Child Study and the pretest for 2013, it proved inexpedient to present our
poverty indicators to all children. The two introductory questions served to screen for the group of
children who have probably experienced deprivation in their everyday lives.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 69
• Sometimes we cannot afford to buy things for school such as exercise books or
pens
• I can’t join a club or learn to play a musical instrument because my family cannot
afford to pay for it
• We hardly ever have children’s birthday parties because they cost too much
• From time to time, we get food for free, for example, from the “Tafel” (an orga-
nization that collects surplus food from supermarkets to distribute to the poor)
• Sometimes I am cold in winter because I do not have any warm clothes
• Within the last year, my parents have had to borrow money from my own
savings
• Every year we take at least a one-week vacation away from home
• In our home, we normally eat breakfast at home before I go to school
• I generally have at least one hot meal a day15
In this context, we no longer define poverty exclusively in relative terms by com-
paring it to the average income in society, but in material terms based on the restric-
tions to access and participation that children experience in their everyday lives
(labeled “experienced poverty” in the following).
A total of 77% of the children give positive answers to the statement “We have
enough money for everything we need,” whereas 13% give negative answers and
10% do not reply (compared to 70% “yes,” 16% “no,” and 14% “no answer” in
2010).
In contrast, as in 2010, 21% of the children in the present Child Study report that
“We are often short of money in our family,” whereas 60% reject this statement and
19% give no answer (compared to 21% “yes,” 61% “no,” and 18% “no answer” in
2010).
We then went on to ask whether the children eat breakfast before they go to
school or whether they generally eat a hot meal every day. A total of 88% say they
eat breakfast; 12% said they do not. In contrast, 98% of the children say that they
generally eat a hot meal every day compared to 2% who say they do not.
By combining the two introductory statements on the financial situation (“We
have enough money for everything we need”: yes; and “We are often short of money
in our family”: no) as shown in Fig. 3.5, we find that, as in 2010, roughly one-
quarter of the children indicate experiencing financial constraints in their everyday
lives (2013: 24%, 2010: 25%).16
If we also include the single “poverty indicators” used to determine the popula-
tion living in poverty and look at the percentages for all children (and not just those
experiencing financial constraints who were given the questions), we can see that
11% of children report that they generally do not go on vacation for financial rea-
sons; 8% that they hardly ever go to the movies or the open-air swimming pool for
15
This statement was presented to all children. However, in this context, we include only the
answers from those for whom the introductory questions had indicated financial constraints.
16
Currently, in 2013, 69% report no financial constraints compared to 65% in 2010. Currently, 7%
give no reply to both statements compared to 9% in 2010.
70 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
Poverty indicators
Fig. 3.5 Material deprivation and experienced poverty from the children’s perspective
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
financial reasons; and 6% that their families cannot afford to pay for their member-
ship of a club or for them to engage in other activities such as learning to play a
musical instrument. Likewise, 6% report that they can hardly ever have a birthday
party, 5% that their parents have had to borrow from the child’s own savings during
the past year, and 4% that they sometimes cannot immediately afford the things they
need for school (exercise books, pens, etc.), 2% that they need to get free food from
the “Tafel,” 2% that they sometimes lack warm clothing in winter, and 2% that they
do not get a warm meal every day.
As mentioned above, we revised the poverty indicators for 2013. Among those
indicators that can still be compared, proportions even seem to have increased
slightly since 2010. However, statistically speaking and when taking the numbers of
cases into account, the deviations are mostly not significant.17
17
This is subject to further methodological limitations such as the larger proportions of children
who gave no answer to the introductory questions in 2010. Therefore, we did not compare the
results on the single poverty indicators that had already been assessed in the last Child Study.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 71
3.4.1 A
voiding Poverty Means Strengthening Parents’
Participation on the Labor Market
Taking all poverty indicators together, 18% of all children report experiencing at
least one of the indicators we surveyed when assessing experienced poverty in the
family; 12% report two or more indicators; and 8% report three or more experiences
of poverty.18
A central safeguard against poverty is for the parents to be regularly employed.
Differentiated according to participation in the labor market, children with two par-
ents holding down regular jobs have the lowest rates of experienced poverty. When
both parents are employed full-time, only 12% of the children report experiencing
poverty, and when one parent is employed full-time and the other part-time or even
both are employed part-time, then the proportion of experienced poverty even goes
down to 9%. The latter indicates that models in which both parents may be employed
part-time are evidently to be found more often in families earning a higher income.
The rate is markedly higher at 18% in families in which only one parent is employed.
A completely different dimension of exposure to poverty is found, in contrast,
among 30% of single parents even when they have a job. However, if none of the
parents living in the household work, the rate of poverty reported by the children
even reaches 55% (Fig. 3.6).
Findings are quite clear here as well. The way to avoid poverty is to strengthen
the parents’ participation on the labor market. Under the given conditions in society,
a partnership model with a joint gainful employment of both parents also seems to
be most appropriate here in order to secure the material framing conditions that will
ensure that children are not excluded from major fields of social life for financial
reasons. As pointed out in Sect. 3.2, this also corresponds to the parents’ wishes. In
cases in which this cannot be achieved, appropriate social provisions should ensure
that children exposed to poverty receive corresponding assistance without any great
access barriers, in other words, in easy ways. The results on experienced poverty as
the children see it nonetheless show the importance of guaranteeing the possibility
to go on day excursions; to be given midday meals in Kindergarten, daycare center,
and school; and to participate in music, sport, and recreation in associations and
groups.
18
In the latest reports on the social situation in Europe, material deprivation (MD) is measured
through indicators based on data from the EU Statistics on Income and Living Conditions
(EU-SILC). According to the EU concept of MD, a person is considered materially deprived if
three of the nine following items are missing from the household for financial reasons: (a) coping
with unexpected expenses; (b) one week annual holiday away from home; (c) avoiding arrears (in
mortgage or rent, utility bills- or hire purchase instalments); (d) a meal with meat, chicken, fish, or
vegetarian equivalent every second day; (e) keeping the home adequately warm; (f) a washing
machine; (g) a color television; (h) a telephone; and (i) a personal car. The more items are missing
in a person’s life, the more the person is considered materially deprived. According to the EU
concept of MD, we speak of serious MD if four out of the nine items are missing (Sikorski and
Kuchla 2011, p. 485). Current results of EU-SILC data analyses (2011) show that 5.9% of 6- to
11-year-old children in Germany experience serious MD.
72 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
30
18 18
12
9
All children One parent One parent full- Both parents full Single parent Unemployed/Other
employed time, one part- time employed
time, both part-
time
3.5 S
ocial Background: Social Origins Continue
to Determine the Difference
In the present Child Study, we once again use the 5-point class index to analyze
social origins that we already introduced for the first World Vision Child Study in
2007. We assign children to a social class of origin on two dimensions: first, the
parents’ educational background (education dimension); and, second, the material
state of the household (material participation dimension). This taps the children’s
central home-related and material starting and framing conditions. It focuses on the
family’s level of education within the context of a sufficient availability of the nec-
essary financial resources.
Empirically, we draw on parental reports on their school-leaving qualifications
supplemented by a rating of the number of books in the household gathered during
the child survey, the parental evaluation of their financial situation, and the residen-
tial status (rented accommodation or home ownership). This information is com-
paratively easy to ask for and can, therefore, be gathered almost completely within
a child study without any exceptional effort (on the formation of the index, see
Appendix 2).
Lower class and lower middle-class children accordingly come from less well-
educated parental homes that also tend to have low incomes, whereas children from
the upper middle class and the upper class have more highly educated family back-
grounds and can also draw on a higher income (Schneekloth and Pupeter 2010a,
pp. 75–79).
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 73
2007
2010
Parents‘ social origins 2013
32 30
29 29 28 29
19 18 16 15 16
12
9 9 9
Fig. 3.7 Social Origins: Educational background and family’s financial resources
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007: 8–11 years) (%)
Over the course of time, our index shows a slight trend toward upward social
mobility since our first Child Study in 2007 (Fig. 3.7). One particularly conspicuous
feature is the higher education that the current parent cohorts were able to obtain
during their school education phase. In 2007, a Hauptschulabschluss [basic second-
ary school] was the highest general school qualification for 27% of mothers and
31% of fathers; mittlere Reife [intermediate], the highest for 45% of mothers and
34% of fathers; Abitur [general university entrance] or Fachabitur [specific univer-
sity entrance] for 26% of mothers and 32% of fathers. In the current 2013 Child
Study, the proportion of Hauptschule graduates is 22% among mothers and 24%
among fathers; the proportion with mittlere Reife is 40% among mothers and 34%
among fathers; and the proportion with Abitur or Fachabitur is 35% among mothers
and 39% among fathers.19
As a result, the proportion of children from the lower middle class has dropped
from 19% in 2007 to 16% in 2013 and the proportion of children from the middle
class from 32% to 29%. In contrast, the proportion of the upper middle class has
risen from 28% to 30% and that of the upper class from 12% to 16%. Because we
calculated the class index itself in the same way for each survey, the social change
that has occurred is clear to see. Hence, there has been a slight increase in the “edu-
cational capital” of the families. Nonetheless, the complexity of both working and
everyday life has also increased in our society. This makes it all the more worrying
when we see that the proportion of children from the lowest social class has remained
constant at 9%. In this the most poorly educated and also the most socially deprived
class, the “educational losers” continue to accumulate—those who have not man-
aged to move upward and have remained excluded. At this point, our class index
19
These reports also refer to parents rearing 6- to 11-year-old children (in 2007: 8- to 11-year-olds).
For single parents, in contrast, we have no information on the other parent. This means that our
reports on fathers are also incomplete here and refer only to fathers and stepfathers living in the
same household.
74 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
Table 3.10 Whether my parents give me enough of their time: Trends from the children’s
perspective
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007: 8–11 years)
Columns in % 2007 2010 2013
My mother gives me enough of her time
Yes 67 64 64
Sometimes yes, sometimes no 27 29 29
No 5 6 6
Mother not present 1 1 1
My father gives me enough of his time
Yes 34 32 34
Sometimes yes, sometimes no 43 44 42
No 16 16 16
Father not present 7 8 8
favorable starting conditions and therefore a clear need for social and societal
support.
3.6 C
are and Attention: Reconciling Family
and Working Life
How do the children in our survey rate the care they receive at home? In the last
Child Study, we already showed that children are generally highly satisfied with
their parental care, that is, how their parents look after them. This has not changed.
No more than 11% give a negative to neutral (2010 Child Study: 18%); 34%, a posi-
tive; and 54%, even a very positive rating (2010: 38% positive and 44% very posi-
tive). We asked the children the following question: “Generally speaking, how
happy are you about the way your parents look after you?” Once again, the children
could reply to this with our 5-point smiley scale.
We gain a more differentiated picture as soon as we pose more specific questions
and ask the children whether they think that their parents give them enough of their
time (Table 3.10). We have been posing this question ever since the first Child Study
in 2007. Two out of three children (64%) consider that their mothers give them
enough of their time; 29% reply “sometimes yes, sometimes no”; and 6% say their
mothers do not give them enough of their time. The general trend in the findings is
very consistent. The slight deviation in 2007 is due to the fact that we surveyed only
8- to 11-year-old children at that time. The older the children, the greater the propor-
tion who are satisfied with the time their mothers devote to them (6- to 7-year-olds:
61%, 8- to 9-year-olds: 63%, 10- to 11-year-olds: 70%). At 63%, 6- to 7-year-old
boys are more satisfied than girls at 57%. The same applies for 8- to 9-year-olds:
64% of boys compared to 61% of girls. Among 10- to 11-year-olds, this inverts
slightly with 67% of boys being satisfied with the time their mother spends with
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 77
Table 3.11 Whether my parents give me enough of their time by age and gender
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
All 6–7 8–9 10–11
Columns in % children Girls Boys years years years
My mother gives me enough of her time
Yes 64 64 65 61 63 70
Sometimes 29 29 28 29 31 25
No 6 7 6 9 5 4
Mother not present 1 0 1 1 1 1
My father gives me enough of his time
Yes 34 34 34 35 33 35
Sometimes 42 43 40 37 45 41
No 16 14 18 20 15 14
Father not present 8 9 8 8 7 10
them compared to 73% of girls. However, the general trend is for the ratings of boys
and girls to remain on a comparable level (Table 3.11).
Results are similar for rating time with fathers. Here, however, only one in three
children (34%) consider that their father gives them enough of their time, 42% reply
“sometimes yes, sometimes no,” and 16% say their fathers do not give them enough
of their time. No clear trend can be ascertained here, because although children’s
reports very slightly from study to study, statistical differences are only slight.
Differentiating according to age also reveals a similar picture: In each age group,
approximately one-third of the children are satisfied with the time their father
spends with them. However, 20% of the younger children aged 6–7 years answer
“no” when asked whether their father gives them enough of their time. Differentiating
according to gender also reveals slight differences with boys somewhat more fre-
quently saying “no” than girls.
3.6.1 F
ourteen Percent of Children Complain About Explicit
Parental Care Deficits
Summarizing the children’s ratings on whether their parents give them enough of
their time reveals the following picture (Fig. 3.8): At 33% (2010: 31%), one-third of
children report that all parents give them enough of their time. In this case, “all”
means “all parents who live with them.” For single parents, this is only one parent;
for patchwork families, this may also be more than two parents.
The majority of children reveal a typical constellation: one parent either suffi-
cient time or sometimes yes, sometimes no. This is reported by 44% of the children
(2010: 47%). In contrast, one single parent not sufficient time (answer “no,” but
excluding single parents) is named continuously by 9% of children.
78 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
33 2007
All parents sufficient 31 2010
33 2013
45
One parent sufficient; 47
one or both sometimes
44
9
One of two parents
9
insufficient
9
2
All parents insufficient 2
3
Fig. 3.8 Deficits in parental care: Children’s reports on time parents spend with them
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007: 8–11 years) (%)
Insufficient care is reported by 11% of children with “one parent insufficient; the
other sometimes yes, sometimes no” (2010: 1%) and 3% (2010: 2%) with “all par-
ents insufficient” (here including single parents). We classify both these constella-
tions as a care deficit. Hence, according to their own reports, this holds for 14% of
children.
3.6.2 G
ainful Employment of Parents Does Not Have
to Be Accompanied by Care Deficits
Our latest Child Study also confirms the finding that increased gainful employment
of parents and sufficient time for children do not have to contradict each other
(Fig. 3.9). The least care deficits are named by children whose parents have jobs. If
only one parent is gainfully employed, then 9% of children report a care deficit. If
both parents are gainfully employed (either one full-time or both part-time), it is
also no more than 8% who report a care deficit. If both parents are gainfully
employed full-time, this proportion rises to 16%.
However, the situation is markedly different for children of gainfully employed
single parents. Here, 32% complain about a care deficit. However, at 29%, a similar
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 79
17 17 16
13 13 14
9 9 8 8 8
6
All children One parent One parent full- Both parents full Single parent Unemployed/Other
employed time, one part-time, time employed
both part-time
Fig. 3.9 Deficits in parental care and parents’ labor market participation from the children’s
perspective
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007: 8–11 years) (%)
level is reported by children whose parents are unemployed or do not pursue gainful
employment for other reasons.20
3.6.3 R
econciling Family and Working Life: How the Parents
See It
In the latest Child Study, we ask not only the children but also parents about recon-
ciling family and working life.21 A total of 14% of parents say reconcilability works
“very well” and a further 52% that it works “well.” This compares with 26% for
“just about okay,” 5% for “less well,” and 1% “not well at all” (2% gave no reply;
see Fig. 3.10).
The child’s age and gender make no difference here. As to be expected, the eco-
nomic situation of the family plays a strong role in rating reconcilability. In every
second family in which one of the children surveyed reports (at least) one of the
poverty indicators we surveyed (see Sect. 3.4), the reconcilability of family and
working life is reported to work only “just about okay” (33%) or “(less well) not
well at all” (17%).
Differentiating the answers of the parents providing the information according to
type of family, we find that every second single parents reports that reconcilability
works either “just about okay” (38%) or “(less well) not well at all” (13%). For
families with three or more children, 23% report that it works “just about okay” and
20
Adding the children who report a lack of time with one parent to the children with a care deficit
results in a comparable trend.
21
Questions on the family and on the parental background were reported by one information source
on behalf of the other—83% by the mother and 17% by the father.
80 U. Schneekloth and M. Pupeter
Alright Reconcilability
Rather
bad/Very bad
40 30
30
26
21 24
16
6 6 7 9
3
All parents One parent One parent full- Both parents full Single parent Unemployed/Other
employed time, one part-time, time employed
both part-time
Fig. 3.10 Labor market participation and reconciling family and working life — the parents’
perspective
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
9% “(less well) not well at all.” Among two-child families, 23% report that reconcil-
ability works “just about okay” and 4% “(less well) not well at all”; and in one-child
families, 27% “just about okay” and 2% “(less well) not well at all.”
Finally, differentiating according to participation on the labor market, we find the
following picture: The most frequent complaints come from 49% of single parents
who are gainfully employed: 40% report that reconcilability works “just about
okay” and 9% that it works “not well at all.” This is followed by 46% of those fami-
lies in which both parents are unemployed or do not work for other reasons. Here,
30% report that reconcilability of family and working life works “just about okay”
and even 16% that it works “(less well) not well at all.”
When both parents are gainfully employed full-time, 37% complain about recon-
cilability: In this case, 30% report that reconcilability works is “just about okay”
and 7% “(less well) not well at all.” As before, these percentages are lower than
those in the other groups. When one parent works full-time and the other part-time,
or both part-time, then it is only 27% who complain with 24% “just about okay” and
3% “(less well) not well at all.” Results are similar for the constellation “one parent
gainfully employed” at 27% with 21% “just about okay” and 6% “(less well) not
well at all.”
In summary, a constellation of several factors—low level of education, unem-
ployment, and risk of poverty, and/or type of family (single-parent)—lead more
frequently to a moderate to poor reconcilability between family and working life. In
a constellation of two full-time gainfully employed parents, there are also more
frequent reports on problems with reconciling family and working life, but they are
less frequent than among parents in precarious employment situations. In contrast,
families that have organized their division of labor with a combination of full- and
part-time gainful employment do not report problems in reconciling family and
working life more frequently than families in which only one parent goes to work.
3 Family Backgrounds: Great Variety But Also Marked Differences In Life Conditions 81
From both the children’s and the parents’ perspective, both caring for and having
time for children seem to work best when the family situation is stable and predict-
able. It is highly evident that this is best resolved when parents have succeeded in
ensuring a viable participation on the labor market. In contrast, the family care situ-
ation is markedly problematic and deficient when conditions are unstable—be this
because a single parent does not have enough time through own gainful employment
or because both parents are unable to achieve a sufficient intensity of care because
of unemployment or other precarious situations. Hence, time is an important pre-
condition for a good and reliable care of children. However, here as well, a potential
“more” does not always mean a “better.” Alongside time, it is, above all, the quality
of the relationship that is important—hence, taken together, the “quality time.”
According to both the children and their parents, this can work better in a stable
family constellation based on shared and well-coordinated gainful employment of
both parents than in an economically and/or emotionally insecure situation that both
children and parents perceive to be unsatisfactory or stressful.
For single parents, there is no alternative here to having appropriate institutional
childcare facilities at their disposal. Otherwise they can hardly avoid being depen-
dent on transfer payments to counter poverty (e.g., Hartz IV or social welfare ben-
efits). However, it is also notable that families with more than two children also
more frequently complain about problems in reconciling family and working life.
This is certainly due to the lack of family friendliness in German society. This is
perceived by both the children (see Sect. 2.7) and their parents. Here as well, meet-
ing the demand for more time, money, and infrastructure—the most important strat-
egies for delivering a sustained family policy according to the 7th Family Report to
the German government (BMFSFJ 2006)—would also make an important contribu-
tion to ensuring the well-being of both children and their parents.
References
Schneewind, K. A. (2008). Sozialisation in der Familie. In Hurrelmann, K., Grundmann, M., &
Walper, S. (Hrsg.), Handbuch Sozialisationsforschung (7. vollst. überarbeitete Auflage, S.
256 – 273). Weinheim: Beltz.
Sikorski, U., & Kuchler, B. (2011). Wer muss worauf verzichten? Einschätzungen zur Wohn- und
Lebenssituation der privaten Haushalte. Wirtschaft und Statistik, 5 / 2011, S. 484 – 492.
Tracy, R. (2007). Wie Kinder Sprachen lernen. Und wie wir sie dabei unterstützen können.
Tübingen: Francke.
Chapter 4
School: An Increasingly Important Field
of Experience
Monika Pupeter and Klaus Hurrelmann
School plays a major role in the daily lives of 6- to 11-year-olds—and not just in
terms of the time spent there and the structuring of the day through school atten-
dance. It also imposes completely new social and intellectual demands on children,
because they now have a range of different things to learn, and they need to acquire
not only knowledge but also basic competencies such as reading, writing, and arith-
metic. School admission also marks the beginning of a new phase of life in a new
community in which children first have to find and claim their place. This is because
school is a social location in which children can meet their friends and forge new
friendships. Hence, alongside confronting them with the actual contents of learning,
it also places greater demands on children’s abilities to socialize and adapt to their
surroundings while simultaneously coming to terms with new spatial conditions.
Put briefly, school has many facets for children, and for each individual child, school
admission is accompanied not only by opportunities, scopes for making new dis-
coveries, and new chances but also by constraints, disappointments, and trials to be
mastered. School becomes an increasingly important life space for childhood
experience.
In most German federal states, elementary school lasts only 4 years. As a result,
one of the most pressing questions regarding the further academic career already
emerges in the third year of school: Which transition option will become available
at the end of elementary school? Will the parents’ wishes be fulfilled enabling the
child to transfer to a Gymnasium or another school offering a university entrance
track? This parental wish has become even stronger in recent years, particularly
M. Pupeter (*)
Kantar Public, München, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
K. Hurrelmann
Hertie School of Governance, Berlin, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
because of the long-lasting labor market problems and the financial crisis. It is par-
ticularly apparent in the transition rates to Gymnasium and other schools offering a
university entrance track that are now far above 50% throughout Germany
(Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung 2012, p. 253). This places children under
a great pressure to maintain their position and perform well right from the start of
elementary school.
This summarizes the reasons why the topic of “school” plays such a major role
in the World Vision Child Studies. As the latest study shows once again, the children
themselves also have a lot to say about the topic. They are very aware of the impor-
tance of this life domain, and when it comes to questions about school, they express
clear opinions and attitudes. These are what we shall be addressing in the present
chapter: We shall start by reporting on the children’s perspectives and aspirations
for their further school careers and how these differ in line with their social ori-
gins—that is, the status of the parental home. Then we shall address what children
expect from an all-day school and how they would like it to be. This is followed by
an overview of children’s appraisals regarding how far and in which domains they
can codetermine daily life at school and in school lessons as well as the design of
their school. Finally, at the end of the chapter, we shall ask how fair children per-
ceive their school to be and how satisfied they are with their daily schooling.
The results of international comparative studies (above all, IGLU, PISA, and
TIMSS) have shown that social origins shape a child’s school achievement more
strongly in Germany than in most other countries (Baumert et al. 2006; Bos et al.
2010). When the parents are well off economically, possess a high social status, and
are themselves well-educated, then this transfers to the expectations and mostly also
to the actual school achievements of their children. When the parents have either
low or no school-leaving qualifications, and/or are in an economically weak posi-
tion, then their children mostly do comparatively less well at school (Hadjar and
Becker 2006; Quenzel and Hurrelmann 2010).
The reason for this close relationship is that parents in Germany are traditionally
granted particularly strong freedoms in shaping and influencing how their children
are reared and educated. Compared to other countries, day nursery and preschool
provisions and all-day schools expanded much more slowly after World War 2. It
was only new legislation to promote child development in the late 1990s (the
Kinderförderungsgesetz) that finally resulted in parents having a legal right to early
childhood care in a daycare center for children over the age of 12 months from
August 1, 2013, onward. Even before it came into force, this law triggered an
impressive increase in the proportion of under-3-year-old children attending
preschool. All-day schools have been promoted since the German government
introduced a program to invest in future education and care (the Investitionsprogramm
Zukunft Bildung und Betreuung) in 2003. Nonetheless, even today, it is still
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 85
q uestionable whether the reforms introduced so far have been sufficient to o vercome
the high level of unequal starting conditions in families.
The World Vision Child Study can be used to analyze the relation between chil-
dren’s social origins and the educational track they are pursuing in 2013. We shall
first look at which grade the 6- to 11-year-old children are attending at the beginning
of 2013, the time point of the survey (Table 4.1). The total column shows that the
children are distributed relatively equally across Grades 1– 5 (from 17% to 19% per
grade). Roughly 8% of the children are already attending 6th grade. However, major
differences emerge when we take social origins into account: Whereas more than
one-quarter of the lower class children (26%) are attending 1st grade, this is the case
for only 17% of the upper class. In the 5th and 6th grades, the numbers reverse with
21% of the 6- to 11-year-olds coming from the lower class and 33% from the upper
class.
This unequal distribution of the children across school grades is due to children
from higher social classes already having better starting positions right at the begin-
ning of schooling. Because their parents have prepared them better for school
admission, these children are less frequently told to wait one further year before
starting school, and a larger proportion can take advantage of the “can rule” permit-
ting early school enrolment under favorable circumstances. Together with the
focused support that these children receive from their parents at home, this enables
them to adapt more quickly to the rhythm of work and the social rules of school life
than children from lower social classes. Hence, they start off with a temporal advan-
tage that they can build on increasingly over the course of elementary school.
Eventually, this leads to greater achievement and far better chances of transferring
to the academic track of the Gymnasium (Hadjar and Becker 2006; Maaz et al.
2010; Stubbe 2009).
Table 4.2 shows how elementary school careers influence transitions to the dif-
ferent types of secondary school. The majority of the 6- to 11-year-olds surveyed
86 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
Table 4.2 Type of schools children attended (Parents’ reports) of different social origins
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
Lower Upper
All Lower middle Middle middle
Columns in % children class class class class Upper class
Elementary school 68 68 67 70 70 64
Hauptschule 5 12 10 6 1 1
Realschule 8 4 9 8 7 7
Gymnasium 11 1 4 7 15 21
School with several 4 2 3 5 5 6
tracks
Special needs school 4 13 7 4 2 1
are still attending elementary school, but the one-third who have already left are
distributed very unequally across the different types of secondary school. Whereas
the majority from the lower class are attending Hauptschule (12%), the majority
from the upper class are attending Gymnasium (21%) or schools offering a range of
tracks (6%; composed of comprehensive schools, integrated secondary schools, city
district schools, etc.) providing direct access to the academic university entrance
track.
When we compare these findings with those from 2007 and 2010, we can see
hardly any changes. In the two prior World Vision Child Studies as well, the propor-
tion of lower class children attending a Gymnasium was, according to parent reports,
about 1% compared to about 20% of upper class children. The comparison across
time also reveals hardly any changes in the attendance of other types of school.
How can we interpret these findings? Our impression is that the relation between
social origins and school careers has consolidated over the 6-year period covered by
our studies. It is evident that the aforementioned education policy reforms (expan-
sion of preschool and all-day provisions) and the intensive public discussion on the
topic of unequal educational opportunities have had no measurable impact. The
social inequalities that children bring with them from their parental homes continue
to be neither balanced out nor overcome by the educational effects of school despite
the policy reforms introduced for this purpose (Hurrelmann et al. 2011).
As in the two previous World Vision Child Studies, we also asked the 6- to 11-year-
olds in the current study what sort of school-leaving qualification they would like to
attain. The question given to elementary school children was: “What type of school
do you want to go to later when you leave elementary school: Hauptschule
(Hauptschulabschluss), Realschule (Realschulabschluss), Gymnasium (Abitur), or
Don’t know/Don’t care?” We asked children who were already attending a
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 87
1
We used an in-depth multivariate analysis to test all factors influencing whether 6- to 11-year-old
children consider that they will complete secondary schooling with an Abitur (university entrance
qualification). The children’s social origins proved to have the greatest explanatory power. The
influence of the child’s age also persisted in the analysis. A further factor was the children’s lei-
sure-time behavior. Looking at the leisure types, compared to the “all-rounders” at 58%, the
“media consumers” had a much lower interest in attaining the Abitur at 29%. Residential structure,
which essentially reflects the structure of school provisions, contributes a further explanatory
88 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
16
27
39
56
37 71
41
32
17
19 Abitur
6
6 2 11 Realschule
1
30 26 Hauptschule
23 23
17
Don't know
Lower class Lower middle Middle class Upper middle Upper class
class class
Among the upper class children, 71% wish to attend the Gymnasium after leaving
elementary school; 11%, the Realschule; and only 1%, the Hauptschule. In contrast,
only 16% of lower class children aspire to the Abitur; 37% want a Realschulabschluss;
and 17%, a Hauptschulabschluss. Hence, more than four times as many children
from the upper class than the lower class aspire to the most desirable highest school-
leaving qualification, the Abitur. Whereas 17% of the upper class children are
uncertain about which school-leaving qualification they aspire to, this proportion is
almost twice as high among lower class children at 30%. The children from the
middle of these three social classes give reports that lie between these two extremes.
These findings can be interpreted as indicating that even early in life, children in
Germany sense the effects of their social origins on their later educational careers.
They are at least intuitively aware of the social status of their parental home, how
this influences their current achievement position, and which perspectives and
developmental chances it provides. Apparently, their family, school, neighborhood,
and environment gives them so many hints and signals regarding their achievement
and developmental potential that they are able to derive an appraisal of their own
chances in further education. As they grow older, this appraisal becomes increasingly
effect. Rural areas have fewer schools leading to higher school-leaving certificates. Children in
rural areas who wish to attend a Gymnasium generally have to accept longer routes to school. This
aspect also plays a major role in considerations on which qualification to strive toward. Big cities,
in contrast, offer several different types of school to choose from that are also in easy travelling
distance. This is also reflected in the anticipation of being able to achieve the Abitur (49% vs.
39%).
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 89
Table 4.4 Desire to attain university entrance qualification (Abitur) across time
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
Lower Lower middle Middle Upper middle Upper
Columns in % All children class class class class class
6- to 11-year-olds
2010 50 19 30 45 64 76
2013 45 16 27 39 56 70
8- to 11-year-olds
2007 49 21 32 36 68 82
2010 53 16 28 50 70 82
2013 48 19 27 42 61 73
more precise: “Don’t knows” become less frequent, and the differences in future
perspectives become larger and larger.
Scientific studies have shown how important educational aspirations are for the
motivation and learning style of students. In the majority of cases, those who set
themselves high goals also cultivate the necessary energy and persistence to attain
them (Hopf 2010). This is why the school-leaving qualification aspirations gathered
here are such strong predictors of the educational goals that children actually attain.
The trends to be seen in these relations have changed hardly at all compared to
the 2007 and 2010 World Vision Child Studies. We evaluate this as indicating two
trends: First, as pointed out above, despite all the debates on reform, there has been
no actual improvement in educational chances in relation to social origins. Second,
despite all the problems that educational policy is facing in relation to this great
inequality, we can at least conclude that the pressure on children to attain the high-
ranking Abitur school-leaving qualification has not increased any further since
2007.
A closer look at the data across the years on the aspirations for an Abitur school-
leaving qualification confirms this second conclusion. As Table 4.4 shows, the chil-
dren’s desire to attain the Abitur has not increased over the course of our studies;
indeed, it has slightly weakened. Because the 2007 survey did not yet include 6- to
7-year-olds, the upper part of the table reports only findings for 2010 and 2013.
However, the trend is clear: In all social classes, a lower percentage is striving
toward the Abitur in 2013 compared to 2010. Findings are not so clear in the lower
part of the table that compares the results on 8- to 11-year-old children across all
three World Vision Child Studies. Despite strong variations in the percentages, one
can predominantly see a decline in the aspiration toward the Abitur school-leaving
qualification. Moreover, ideas on which secondary school and which school-leaving
qualification can be attained seem to have become less clear over time, as indicated
90 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
by the increase in the proportion of “don’t know/don’t care” answers (not reported
in the table).
If such a trend persists in the next World Vision Child Study, it could be evaluated
as a sign of uncertainty regarding whether one can or should strive toward which
particular school-leaving qualification at elementary school age. Because the
children’s reports also reflect their parents’ perspectives, the findings may also point
to a shifting trend and be one first sign of an easing of the prestige- and status-
dominated decisions on the future career paths of children. Many parents are
uncomfortable with the pressure of having to get their children to grasp the signifi-
cance of the “elementary school Abitur” and already take this seriously in their third
year of schooling. As a result, they prefer schools offering a range of educational
pathways that also include the Abitur but make it possible to postpone this decision
until a later time when already attending secondary school.
The gradual introduction of all-day schools in Germany, since 2003 has been a reac-
tion to the social fact that both parents increasingly go out to work. However, it
simultaneously follows the goal of balancing out the aforementioned differences in
educational opportunities and enabling children from poorly educated parental
homes to receive additional support. School courses in the afternoon supplemented
by sports, musical and cultural “leisure-time” activities should particularly support
children with a low education background and offer them developmental opportuni-
ties going beyond the traditional framework of the half-day school with its emphasis
on teaching and imparting knowledge (Bremm 2013; Fischer et al. 2011).
2007 13
2010 18
2013 23
conurbations (23%) is higher than in other regions (Table 4.5). The difference
between the old and new German states is particularly large in elementary schools:
It is particularly high in the East at 41% compared to 16% in the West (no table).
An analysis of the statistical relations after holding the regional supply factors
(old/new German states and settlement structure) constant reveals no further signifi-
cant explanations of all-day school attendance based on personal and social vari-
ables (no table). Individual characteristics such as age and gender seem to play only
a secondary role. A migration background in the children’s families also reveals no
particular effect. The main explanatory power for attending an all-day school lies in
the supply structure. Accordingly, the decisive aspect is where a family lives and
whether they have a realistic opportunity to choose freely in favor of an all-day
place for their children.
As pointed out above, the expansion of all-day schools called for by policymakers
also has to be seen as a response to the unsatisfactory performance of children from
the lower social classes in international comparisons. Broadening and deepening the
learning opportunities in all-day schools should supplement the familial support for
children’s educational careers and improve their relatively less favorable educa-
tional opportunities.
What do the results of the current Child Study tell us about this? In 2013, roughly
every third child from the lower class reported attending an all-day school; for upper
class children, this was only every fifth, and for middle-class children, slightly more
than every fifth (Fig. 4.3). At first glance, this outcome would seem to basically sup-
port the political intentions behind setting up this new type of school: Lower class
children coming from particularly poorly educated parental homes personally report
an above-average use of all-day school provisions.
When we compare these findings with those from the 2007 and 2010 World
Vision Child Studies, we can see interesting trends. In 2007, the differences in the
attendance of all-day schools between children from the lower class and other
classes were much less marked than in 2013 (Fig. 4.4). Since 2007, children from
all classes, and particularly those from the upper class as well, have increased their
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 93
31
25
22 22
20
Lower class Lower middle Middle class Upper middle Upper class
class class
Upper class
8
9 Upper middle
2007 16 class
12
Middle class
15
Lower middle
10 class
18
Lower class
2010 15
20
29
21
23
2013 21
24
35
participation in this type of school. At the same time, there has been a renewed
increase in the proportion of lower class children, and in 2013, a good 10% more of
them attend than all other social classes.
Our study is unable to say whether more frequently attending all-day schools has
also resulted in better school performance among lower class children. The KMK
statistics can be interpreted as indicating that the high attendance rates for all-day
94 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
schools are due particularly to the use of the partially compulsory and completely
compulsory forms of provisions. However, these provisions are found to be particu-
larly frequent in the types of school attended by an above-average proportion of
lower class children; that is, comprehensive schools, special education schools, and
Hauptschule (Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung 2012, p. 81). By applying
(mostly compulsory) all-day provisions, these schools should achieve particularly
intensive and sustained learning effects and compensate the influence of the paren-
tal homes. However, there are various reasons for attending these types of school,
and attendance is not always voluntary. Future studies will reveal whether this edu-
cational policy is really a success under these circumstances and whether it can
compensate the disadvantages in the educational careers of those from poorly edu-
cated parental homes. Our results, however, cast some doubt on this.
Support for our doubts comes from the children’s reports on their acceptance of all-
day schools. We first asked children whether they would prefer to attend a school in
which teaching ends at midday. This is rejected by 61% of the children attending
all-day schools; 30% would prefer a morning-only school, and 10% are undecided.
Hence, although the acceptance of this type of school is good among the students
attending it, it is not exactly overwhelmingly high.
Differentiating the answers to this question according to the children’s social
origins shows that only 41% of lower class children accept this type of school. This
is well below the average of 61%. In contrast, three-quarters of the upper class chil-
dren find all-day school good. Hence, the group of children in which a particularly
large proportion attends this type of school is simultaneously the group that is least
satisfied with all-day schooling. Our study cannot explain why this is so. Perhaps it
relates to the aforementioned fact that attending an all-day school is not always
voluntary for lower class children and that a difficult student clientele accumulates
at these schools. The more than averagely high acceptance of all-day schools among
upper class children could correspondingly be traced back to the particularly high
proportion of students that the KMK reports as voluntarily attending all-day schools
(Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung, 2012, p. 80).
The very differentiated answers that we find throughout the World Vision Child
Study when asking children to describe their own life situation leave us in no doubt
about their ability to appraise their situation at school both appropriately and accu-
rately. As a result, we consider the relatively high level of dissatisfaction and the
worryingly large lack of appreciation of all-day schooling among lower class
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 95
children to be based on sound and well-justified reasons. The most recent German
LBS-Kinderbarometer study confirms this. A questionnaire survey of 4th- to 7th-
grade students revealed that the less happy children feel at school, the more fre-
quently they reject afternoon school provisions (LBS-Kinderbarometer 2011,
p. 153).
This has important implications for the discussion on extending all-day school-
ing. The children’s statements show not only how inadequate it is to simply increase
the quantity of places in all-day schools without ensuring an appropriate quality and
sufficient resources but also the problems that arise when all-day school attendance
is not voluntary. As our study shows, such a strategy does result in more lower class
children attending this type of school. However, given their internal self-distancing
from these schools, it is questionable whether they profit from this. The future con-
cern is not just a quantitative expansion of all-day provisions. It will be far more
necessary to pay greater attention to the quality of the care and services provided
and to ensure that the children themselves appreciate them. At the end of the day,
children who spend almost the entire day attending such a school will benefit from
it only if they feel happy there and are able to experience the services provided as
beneficial to their development (see the research findings reported in Stecher et al.
2011).
As documented in the last two World Vision Child Studies, children have very
precise ideas about what a “good” all-day school should be like. Studies on urban
facilities for children including playgrounds show repeatedly that these are accepted
most readily and best fulfil their goals when their future users are actively involved
in their planning and design (Stange et al. 2009). Such findings should be trans-
ferred to the school environment and particularly to that of the all-day school. The
need is to develop a program for afternoon schooling that the children accept
because they have helped design it.
Our surveys show that children attending half-day schools can also imagine tak-
ing part in school activities in the afternoon. In 2010, the majority of half-day stu-
dents (76%) expressed an interest in afternoon sports (Leven and Schneekloth 2010,
p. 171). The children were also interested in art and theater groups (65%), projects
(56%), and homework supervision (38%). In contrast, only 17% of half-day stu-
dents were in favor of normal teaching lessons in the afternoon, and only 8% were
not interested in any of the proposed afternoon activities. These findings are also
confirmed by the latest LBS-Kinderbarometer survey reporting that children are
less interested in using the afternoon to revise the morning’s lessons in more depth
or to receive extra tuition. What they are interested in is new and exciting subjects
and lessons, topic-related projects, sport and leisure-time provisions, and also quite
simply phases of creative relaxation (LBS-Kinderbarometer 2011, p. 154).
In summary, the majority of children are in favor of all-day schools, but they
have very precise expectations regarding what this new type of school in Germany
should be like. They are only unreservedly in favor of all-day school when this
brings subject-related and social benefits. The value of drawing on the children’s
subjective well-being as an indicator for the quality of their socioecological habitat
becomes clear. If the all-day school delivers additional incentives and services, the
96 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
The type and extent of child codetermination of lessons and school life varies
greatly from school to school. The only standardized child participation in school is
that regulated by laws and directives: the right to elect a class speaker. Just about
anything going beyond this established participation structure is at the discretion of
the teaching staff: the integration of students in determining the rules in the class, in
shaping the daily life at school ranging from how the classroom is decorated to joint
decisions on school trips and projects, and the organization of events or activities
involving more than one class. Whether or not they are involved in such tasks and
procedures makes a great deal of difference for children. Numerous studies confirm
a close relation between greater possibilities of codetermination in a school and
both the children’s subjective well-being and their trust in their teachers (Bacher
et al. 2007).
In the 2013 World Vision Child Study, we wanted to ascertain how children appraise
the possibilities of codetermination in their schools. We presented items referring to
seven domains in which the students and their teachers can negotiate the rules and
conditions of daily school life. We asked the children:
In your school, are you allowed to help decide:
• How your classroom is decorated?
• Whom are you allowed to sit next to?
• How the desks, chairs, and tables are arranged in your classroom?
• On working out the rules in your class; in other words, how everybody treats
each other in the class?
For children attending 2nd grade and above, we added two more questions:
• Where you will all go on school outings?
• On suggesting possible topics for future class projects?
• On organizing school events?.2
2
The response categories available to the children were “often,” “sometimes,” and “hardly ever.” A
greater number of younger children gave no answer, which was reflected in a slightly higher num-
ber of “no reply” and “don’t know” responses—particularly for the questions given to children
only from Grade 2 onward.
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 97
Often
Organizing school events 20 40
Sometimes
| Reports on school events, school outings, and class projects from children in 2nd grade and above
The results are presented in Fig. 4.5. The most frequent domain of codetermina-
tion is reported to be “whom you are allowed to sit next to.” A total of 30% of chil-
dren report that they can decide this “often”; 41%, “sometimes”; and 28%, “hardly
ever.” When it comes to “working out the rules in your class,” 26% report that they
can codetermine this “often”; 34%, “sometimes”; and 37%, “hardly ever.” When
“deciding how your classroom is decorated,” 23% said they can codetermine this
“often”; 40%, “sometimes”; and 34%, “hardly ever.”
From 2nd grade onward, we asked the children about codetermining school
events, where to go on outings, and topics for their projects. This produced the fol-
lowing results: For helping to decide “on organizing school events,” 20% say they
can do this “often”; 40%, “sometimes”; and 35%, “hardly ever.” For “where you
will all go on school outing,” 16% say they can do this “often” and a further 30%,
“sometimes,” whereas one-half (50%) can do this “hardly ever.” When “suggesting
possible topics for future class projects,” 12% report that they can codetermine this
“often”; and a further 38%, “sometimes.” In contrast, 45% say they can do this
“hardly ever.” From all seven of the domains surveyed, the children can least fre-
quently codetermine “how the desks, chairs, and tables are arranged in your class-
room.” Only 10% of the children say they can do this “often”; a further 27%,
“sometimes;” but 61%, “hardly ever.”
98 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
15
Working out the rules in class 25
35
13
How is classroom decorated 20
33
30
Whom do we sit next to 30
31
11
Organizing school events 15
26
5
Where to go on school outings 11
24
5 6–7 years
Prospective class projects 9
17
8–9 years
7
How are desks arranged 9
14 10–11 years
| Reports on school events, school outings, and class projects from children in 2nd grade and above
4.3.2 C
odetermination in Relation to Age, Frequency,
and Gender
There are major differences between age groups with a tendency for older students
to have more codetermination than younger ones. At 30%, just about every third
10- to 11-year-old reports “often” participating in decisions on at least three of the
domains surveyed. Nonetheless, 26% of children in this age group report never
being integrated into school life in this way. Among the 6- to 7-year-olds, only 5%;
and among the 8- to 9-year-olds, 17% report that they “often” codetermine at least
three domains, but 57% of the 6- to 7-year-olds and 40% of the- to 9-year-olds say
that this is “hardly ever” the case. An inspection of the individual domains also
reveals that older children more frequently report “often” on all domains except for
“whom you are allowed to sit next to.” This is about equal in all three age groups at
circa 30% (Fig. 4.6).
The distribution of school codetermination according to age also reflects the type
of school the children attend. Children in secondary schools report far more fre-
quent codetermination than those attending elementary schools. Age effects can
also be seen within elementary schools. Children in higher grades more frequently
report that they are “often” involved in codetermination than those in lower grades
(no table).
Child codetermination in school varies in terms of its extent and frequency. We
find that 40% of all children aged 6–11 years do not feel well-integrated in the seven
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 99
9
10
Girls 18
28
35
7
10
Boys 16
23
44
2
3
6–7 years 14
24
57
8
9
8–9 years 14
29
40
14
16
10–11 years 21
23
26
domains surveyed and feel unable to codetermine any of the domains “often. “Boys
gain this impression far more frequently than girls (44% vs. 35%), and 6- to 7-year
olds at 57% far more strongly than 10- to 11-year-olds at 26% (Fig. 4.7).
These reports show the broad possibilities for further extending the degree of
participation in life at school. This is particularly the case for the youngest children,
who are basically 1st- and 2nd-grade elementary school students and, above all, it is
the case for school beginners. Here, the teaching staff should focus on thinking
about new and stronger forms of participation that will let the children know right
at the beginning of their school careers that their wishes and opinions are being
valued and taken seriously. Up to now, many schools do not seem to have taken full
advantage of these opportunities. This is poor educational policy, because impres-
sions gained right at the beginning of schooling are very decisive. When children
become aware right from the start of their school careers that they are able to partici-
pate in organizational and negotiation processes, their commitment to achievement
also increases (Olk and Roth 2007). The first years at school are particularly deci-
sive and set the pattern in every way. This is why they also form the foundation for
all later experiences in life at school.
In this respect, there is a great need to improve things for boys. As pointed out
above, their educational aspirations are lower than those of girls (Table 4.3). This
relates to their notably lower frequency of codetermination in school. In all seven
domains, girls perceive stronger codetermination at school than boys. Girls have a
clear advantage in all activities relating to the communicative situation in the class-
100 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
28
Whom do we sit next to 33
23
Working out the rules in class 29
21
How is classroom decorated 25
19
Organizing school events 20
Boys
16
Where to go on school outings 16 Girls
11
Prospective class projects 13
10
How are desks arranged 11
| Reports on school events, school outings, and class projects from children in 2nd grade and above
room and during lessons. Girls more “often” report codetermination in “working
out the rules in your class” (29% vs. 23% in boys), “whom you are allowed to sit
next to” (33% vs. 28%), and deciding “how your classroom is decorated” (25% vs.
21%; see Fig. 4.8).
A look at the close relations between codetermination, commitment, and perfor-
mance shows that these findings deliver important indications regarding the urgently
needed efforts to improve the performance of boys. One strategy to encourage more
intensive integration of boys in lessons and school life could be to use increased
commitment to social rules and regulations to achieve greater commitment to per-
formance. Previous research on these topics has shown how important it is to build
up the same trust in and willingness to get involved in social activities at school in
boys as in girls. This is necessary to counteract their current reluctance to comply
with the social demands of the school that is often due to the performance-deriding
influences of the male peer group. Codetermination could be one way to encourage
their interest in learning and achieving (Hurrelmann and Schultz 2012).
Chapter 2 has already pointed out how believing that they have their say in major
decisions in their life world is fundamentally important for children’s sense of jus-
tice or fairness. The more they see themselves as being integrated into the process
4 School: An Increasingly Important Field of Experience 101
Table 4.6 Relation between perceived justice at school and frequency of codetermination
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
Number of domains that are “often” codetermined
Columns in % Total None One 2–3 4 or more
Very fair 22 20 23 22 31
Quite fair 56 51 56 62 58
Rather unfair 16 20 16 13 8
Very unfair 3 4 2 2 2
Don’t know, no reply 3 5 3 1 1
of formulating and further developing rules, the greater their subjective sense of
experiencing fairness.
In the next section, we want to transfer this finding to a life space that has become
so important for children today: the school. We particularly want to look at how the
codetermination children are able to perceive relates to their sense of justice in their
school. Then we shall also turn to the question of how satisfied children are with
their school.
4.4.1 R
elation Between the Sense of Justice
and Codetermination
The 2013 questionnaire asks “And how fair do you think things are in general?” It
poses this question in relation to the family, to the circle of friends, and to school. In
relation to school, the question is “And in your school? Are things there very fair,
quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?”
The majority of children surveyed judge their school to be “very fair” or “quite
fair.” As Table 4.6 shows, 22% find their school “very fair” and 56% “quite fair.”
Negative reports are very weak at 16% “rather unfair” and 3% “very unfair.” Hence,
at 78%, the majority of children give their school quite good grades in the domain
of balancing interests and distributing opportunities. Although, as reported in Chap.
2, the family and circle of friends are evaluated even more positively, this high
fairness rating is quite remarkable considering that it is being given to an institution
that makes decisive decisions on awarding those certificates that determine future
educational careers, and, in turn, the opportunities for social development. Hence,
the majority of children consider that the school operates and makes its judgments
according to comprehensible criteria. This positive appraisal by children can be
interpreted as indicating that they hold their teachers in high esteem.
Table 4.6 also shows how strongly the sense of justice relates to the level of
potential codetermination in the school: the greater the number of domains in which
they can participate and the greater the variety of design options, the higher their
judgments on fairness. Children who report that they are often able to codetermine
in four or more domains significantly more frequently consider that their school is
102 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
“very fair.” Children who rate justice negatively (taken together, only just 10%) are
infrequent in this group. However, in the group of children with no possibilities of
codetermination, 24% find school either “rather unfair” or “very unfair.”
How far children consider their teacher to take their opinion seriously (assessed
in Chap. 7) also relates to their sense of justice (no table). On average, almost one-
third of the children say that they feel respected by their teacher. The proportion of
children who see their school as being “very fair” is markedly higher here at 43%.
Children who see their school as unfair, in contrast, are well below the average here.
A further finding is also interesting in this context: Children who rate school life as
“rather unfair” or “very unfair” classify themselves as performing less well than the
average student.
These findings show how closely the children’s sense of justice relates not only
to the degree of inclusion in school affairs but also to the recognition they receive
from their teachers both personally and in terms of assessing their performance. The
children’s replies suggest that inclusion in school activities and their teachers’
respect for their opinions and achievements can positively promote their sense of
justice. Children who feel involved in the daily issues at school and taken into
account when decisions are made evidently have a better understanding of how
schools are run and how they reach their decisions, have a more empathic under-
standing of how things are interrelated, and therefore perceive the school as being
more fair than those who feel excluded and remain passive. This can be seen clearly
in the three domains of codetermination that are most important for the children
(Fig. 4.9).
28
14
30
24
34
21
As the figure shows, children who rate their school as being “very fair” report
being able to codetermine how the classroom is decorated almost twice as fre-
quently (at 28%) as children who do not consider their school to be fair (14%). This
difference can also be seen in choosing whom they sit next to and codetermining the
rules in the class. The more strongly children are involved in shaping important
procedures and the more they can also exert an influence on their social surround-
ings, the most positively they rate their school on the fairness scale. This clearly
indicates starting points for reform. Not only satisfaction but also the sense of jus-
tice relate (at least indirectly) to the children’s achievement at school. Any plan to
strengthen achievement cannot just be attained through a greater encouragement of
learning in the teaching of individual subjects, but also through including children
in the sociospatial design of the workplace known as school.
When asked how much they like school, the children’s answers on a 5-point smiley
scale are generally positive. A total of 42% give very positive answers and 37%
positive ones (Table 4.7). Only 16% give a neutral rating; 3%, a negative one; and
2%, a very negative one. This means that 79% of the children associate school with
positive feelings. Compared to 2010, this rating has gone up 9% (pointing to posi-
tive developments in the elementary school domain).
Girls in general are far more satisfied with school than boys with 82% giving
“very positive” and “positive” reports compared to 75% among boys. This confirms
the trends we have already reported for the domain of educational aspirations and
particularly for which school leaving qualification children aspire to (Table 4.3).
The distribution of ratings on satisfaction with school also varies according to age.
Younger children have a far more positive perception of school with 59% of 6- to
7-year-olds finding it “very positive” and a further 26% finding it “positive.” Among
older age groups, the “very positives” decline. Whereas 40% of the 8- to 9- year-
olds consider school to be “very positive” and a further 40% to be “positive,” among
the 10- to 11- year-olds, this drops to 32% “very positive” and 40% “positive.”
104 M. Pupeter and K. Hurrelmann
Very negative
Negative
Quite fair 2 16 45 37 Neutral
Positive
Very positive
Very fair 16 21 72
Fig. 4.10 Relation between satisfaction with school and sense of justice
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
The children’s sense of justice also shows a close relation to general satisfaction
with school. Students who consider school to be “rather unfair” or “very unfair” are
also less satisfied with school in general and vice versa (Fig. 4.10). More than two-
thirds of the students who view school as “very fair” are also very satisfied with
their school (72%). This indicates once again how strongly the children sense
whether they are treated fairly or not and whether they are included in important
decision making in daily school life. Positive experiences in these domains increase
satisfaction with the school.
References
Fischer, N., Holtappels, H. G., Rauschenbach, T., Stecher, L., & Zürchner, I. (Hrsg.). (2011).
Ganztagsschule; Entwicklung, Qualität, Wirkungen. Weinheim: Beltz Juventa.
Hadjar, A., & Becker, R. (Hrsg.). (2006). Die Bildungsexpansion. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Hopf, W. (2010). Freiheit-Leistung-Ungleichheit. Bildung und soziale Herkunft in Deutschland.
Weinheim: Beltz.
Hurrelmann, K., & Schultz, T., (Hrsg.). (2012). Jungen als Bildungsverlierer. Weinheim: Beltz
Juventa.
Hurrelmann, K., Quenzel, G., & Rathmann, K. (2011). Bildungspolitik als Bestandteil moderner
Wohlfahrtspolitik. Zeitschrift für Soziologie der Erziehung und Sozialisation, 31, S. 313 – 328.
KMK Sekretariat. (2012). Allgemein bildende Schulen in Ganztagsform in Ländern der
Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Bonn und Berlin: KMK.
LBS-Kinderbarometer. (2011). Stimmungen, Trends und Meinungen von Kindern aus Deutschland.
Recklinghausen: RDN Verlag.
Leven, I., & Schneekloth, U. (2010). Die Schule: Frühe Vergabe von Lebenschancen. In World
Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2010 (S. 161–185). Frankfurt a. M.:
Fischer.
Maaz, K., Baumert, J., Gresch, C., & McElvany, N. (Hrsg.). (2010) Der Übergang von der
Grundschule in die weiterführenden Schulen. Berlin und Bonn: BMBF.
Olk, T., & Roth, R. (2007). Mehr Partizipation wagen. Gütersloh: Bertelsmann Stiftung.
Quenzel, G., & Hurrelmann, K. (Hrsg.). (2010). Bildungsverlierer. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Stange, W., Meinhold-Henschel, S., & Schack, S. (2009). Mitwirkung (er)leben. Handbuch
zur Durchführung von Beteiligungsprojekten mit Kindern und Jugendlichen. Gütersloh:
Bertelsmann Stiftung.
Stecher, L., Krüger, H. H., & Rauschenbach, T. (Hrsg.). (2011). Ganztagsschule – neue Schule?
Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
Stubbe, T. (2009). Bildungsentscheidungen und sekundäre Herkunftseffekte. Münster: Waxmann.
Chapter 5
Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not
For All Children
Agnes Jänsch and Ulrich Schneekloth
Together with attending school, spending leisure time both together with and apart
from the family is a major life domain for 6- to 11-year old children. It offers oppor-
tunities to gain important experiences and acquire knowledge beyond that at school.
The 2007 and 2010 Child Studies have already shown how strongly leisure time
differs according to not only age group and social class but also personal disposi-
tions. To make the different patterns of leisure activities easier to grasp, we devel-
oped a leisure typology (Leven and Schneekloth 2007) that permits a more detailed
analysis of the specific preferences of media consumers, normal leisure users, and
all-rounders.
After giving a general report on the current leisure-time activities of 6- to
11-year-old children while paying particular attention to age effects, gender effects,
and the use of electronic media, we shall take a closer look at the three leisure types
and how they use their leisure time. We are particularly interested in how family
support structures differ in the three groups and how their leisure behavior has
changed since 2007. A further important aspect is institutional leisure activities.
How are children integrated into clubs or other service structures in the musical and
cultural domain, and have children from financially and socially disadvantaged
families managed to gain more access? Then we shall take a look at one specific
aspect of leisure activities: the use of the Internet and mobile phones as components
of contemporary communication that are becoming increasingly just as important
for children as they are for adults.
These different aspects of our analysis finally lead us to ask how far children
themselves are satisfied with their leisure time and which factors they consider to be
decisive for their level of satisfaction.
5.1.1 W
ide Range of Popular Activities from Making Things
to Television and Sports
As in the previous Child Studies, we also asked the children how frequently they
engage in different activities in their leisure time in 2013. They could use one of
three response categories: “hardly ever,” “sometimes,” and “very often.” Since the
first Child Study, our intention has been to develop a list that will cover the entire
spectrum of childhood leisure activities. Therefore, we have updated and extended
our list repeatedly. Compared to the first two Child Studies, we have modified our
list of possible leisure activities in line with two goals: (1) to make our survey more
gender-neutral by avoiding a one-sided bias in favor of typical activities for girls;
and (2) to adequately reflect the entire breadth of childhood leisure activities by
broadening single items or summarizing related activities. For example, “riding my
bike” has been extended to include “inline skating or “skateboarding.” “Making
things” first introduced in 2010 has been modified to “making things with tools” and
“building things with Lego©” has been supplemented by “or playing with
Playmobil©.” We have changed “doing things with animals,” to “engaging with
nature or animals” as a relevant leisure-time category, and we introduced “playing
outside on the street” as being a typical childhood leisure activity. Finally, we added
“listening to audio plays or stories” to our list.
According to their own reports, the children in our survey most frequently play
at home with their toys (54% “very often”). They also engage particularly frequently
in sports (53%), listen to music (52%), watch television (50%), or meet their friends
(51%).
Alongside these possible leisure activities, a relatively large proportion of chil-
dren (30% and more) report engaging in the following activities “very often”: “rid-
ing a bike, inline skating, or skateboarding” (38%), “reading (looking at) books or
magazines” (31 %), “handicrafts, painting, or drawing” (35%), “engaging with
nature or animals” (32%), and “playing outside on the street” (30%).
For a slightly smaller proportion of children, “playing a musical instrument,
making music” (20%), “PlayStation, Nintendo, Wii, computer games” (23%),
“building things with Lego or playing with Playmobil©” (28%), “doing things
together with my family” (27%), and “listening to audio plays or stories” (24%) are
a very frequent part of their daily lives.
In contrast, only 14% of the children report “theater group, dance, or ballet” as
an activity they engage in “very often”; and, at 8%, “making things with tools” is
one of the less popular leisure activities. Both options are relatively gender-typical
activities, although we shifted “theater” to the front of the first item in order to make
it more accessible to boys. A separate analysis reveals that 26% of the girls report
belonging to a theater, dance, or ballet group “very often” compared to only 3% of
boys. Parent reports on club memberships reveal that ballet and dance are clearly
more popular than theater among girls. Boys, in contrast, are clearly in the majority
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 109
when it comes to making things with tools; 13% report that they engage in this
activity “very often” in their leisure time compared to only 3% of girls.
Table 5.1 presents an overview on the frequencies of the various leisure activities
itemized according to gender and age group. To make the table easier to follow,
activities are ranked according to how frequently they are named by all children and
not according to the sequence in which they were presented in the questionnaire
(see the appendix for the questionnaire).
5.1.2 A
ge-Specific Structure of Leisure Activities: Range
of Activities Increases and Children Have a Wider Range
of Different Media Devices
Whereas some leisure activities such as looking at books or magazines and playing
on the street are engaged in equally frequently by children of all age groups, others
reveal an age-specific course. Younger children far more frequently report playing
at home “very often” than older children. A total of 72% of 6- to 7-year-olds fre-
quently play with their toys at home, compared to only 37% of 10- to 11-year-olds.
The activities “making things, painting, or drawing,” “building things with Lego or
playing with Playmobil©,” and “listening to audio plays or stories” also reveal a
110 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
similar age trend. Although this may be due to different leisure preferences, it may
also be due to external factors: Whereas younger children generally receive far more
care and supervision from their parents and thereby have closer ties to their home
environments, older children are already granted more autonomy and freedom, and
they can take advantage of this to engage in other leisure pursuits that are more
frequently outside the parental home. The increased naming of activities such as
“sports,” “meeting friends,” and “riding a bike, inline skating, or skateboarding”
support this hypothesis because these all tend to take place outside the home.
Nonetheless, 10- to 11-year-olds also engage in activities predominantly at home
such as “listening to music,” “PlayStation, Nintendo, Wii, computer games,” or
“playing a musical instrument, making music.” However, all these pursuits require
children to have certain equipment at their disposal – be this a hi-fi system, a
PlayStation, or a musical instrument – and this is far less frequently the case for
younger children. Sixty-five percent of 6- to 7-year-olds report having their own
radio compared to 75% of 10- to 11-year-olds. Whereas 12% of the youngest chil-
dren have their own laptop or computer, among the older children, this is 45%.
Eighteen percent of the 6- to 7-year-olds have their own games console (42% their
own Gameboy) compared to 41% of 10- to 11-year-olds (68% their own Gameboy).
Their own CD or MP3 player is found in 64% versus 82% of children’s rooms and
a DVD or Blu-Ray player in 13% versus 29%. This age-related increase in media
equipment is also reflected in the use of the available equipment, and thereby con-
tributes to different leisure activities in different age groups. What is conspicuous
here is that this does not apply to the frequency of watching television. Whereas
20% of the youngest age group and 43% of the 10- to 11-year-olds report having
their own television, the extent of television consumption remains surprisingly con-
stant across age groups. Approximately one-half of each age group reports watching
television “very often.” Evidently, even when younger children do not have a televi-
sion in their own room, the one in the parental household is relatively freely acces-
sible to them.
5.1.3 G
ender-Specific Differences in Leisure Activities: Sport
and Media for Boys; Music and Creative Activities
for Girls
Leisure preferences do not just differ according to age but also according to gender.
Boys name movement-related activities more frequently: that is, they engage more
often in sports (61% vs. 45%); ride bikes, inline skates, or skateboards more (41%
vs. 35%); and play on the street more (34% vs. 26%). However, they also engage
more in passive leisure options such as watching television (54% vs. 45%) and play-
ing with PlayStation or computers (29% vs. 17%). Although girls engage less fre-
quently in sports such as swimming or soccer, they are more active, as mentioned
above, in the field of “theater group, dance, or ballet” (26% vs. 3%). Hence, they
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 111
combine sports with music and creative activities in their leisure time. They tend to
be more creative in general, as shown by the frequency reports for “handicrafts,
painting, or drawing” (46% vs. 25%), “reading (or looking at) books or magazines”
(36% vs. 27%), and “playing a musical instrument, making music” (26% vs. 15%).
In addition, girls listen to music more frequently than boys (58% vs. 47%) and
spend more time “engaging with nature or animals” (37% vs. 28%).
As already reported in the 2010 Child Study under the heading Der kulturell-
musische Freizeitbereich – Jungen verlieren hier den Anschluss [Boys are losing
touch with the cultural and musical domain], boys in general far less frequently
belong to cultural and musical clubs and groups than girls. According to parent
reports, 47% of all girls have at least one club membership in this domain compared
to only 19% of boys. At 27% in girls and 16% in boys, the most popular cultural and
musical leisure activity is membership of a music group or music school. Girls also
very frequently belong to a ballet group or dance club (25%), whereas this is very
much the exception among boys at only 2%. Painting and theater groups are among
the least frequently attended organized leisure activities in both genders with only
3% of girls and 1% of boys belonging to a painting group. Whereas 4% of girls
participate in a theater or movie group, boys are almost completely absent here. In
particular, multiple memberships are something for girls – 11% of them belong to
more than one organized group in the cultural and musical domain compared to
only 1% of boys.
5.1.4 C
omputer Games: An Everyday Pastime for Many
Children
The most recent study on children’s use of the media, computers, and the Internet in
Germany, the KIM study (Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbunds Südwest
2013) confirms that adults often disapprove of “the use of digital games – on com-
puters, game consoles, or the Internet. Nonetheless, for one-quarter of all children
(particularly boys), playing such games is one of their favorite leisure activities and
is – similar to television and radio – something they take for granted in daily media
life” (p. 46, translated).
As Table 5.1 shows, for 23% of the 6- to 11-year-olds in the current Child Study,
computer games and game consoles are not just part of daily life but are even played
“very often.” Since 2007, the proportion of 8- to 11-year-olds1 who report playing
with a computer or game console “very often” has declined slightly from 29% to
26%. However, since 2010 when the breadth of use was assessed in more detail
(“What about computer games, Gameboy, PlayStation, and the like? How often do
you play with them?”), it has proved to be relatively constant. Among 6- to
1
The group of 6- to 7-year-olds was not included in these analyses, because it was not surveyed in
2007, making a direct comparison across all children impossible.
112 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
2010 42 15 10 24 6 1
6–7
2013 40 9 15 24 3 4
2010 26 13 16 30 10 4
8–9
2013 29 12 15 26 8 7
2010 20 10 15 36 10 9
10 – 11
2013 23 9 15 30 9 13
Hardly ever
Not very often
Maximum one hour a week
Several times a week but not more than one hour a day
Every day but not more than one hour at a time
Several times a day or more than one hour a day
| Failure to reach 100% due to “Don’t know/No answer” responses
Fig. 5.1 Frequency of playing computer games by age groups over time
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
1 1-year-olds,2 14% in 2010 and 15% in 2013 play computer games “at the most, one
hour a week”; 31% in 2010 and 27% in 2013, “several times a week for up to one
hour a day”; 9% in 2010 and 7% in 2013, “every day, but up to one hour a day”; and
5% in 2010 and 8% in 2013, “several times a day for more than one hour a day.” In
contrast, roughly 30% of children at each assessment “hardly ever” spend their lei-
sure time playing with computers and game consoles.
Figure 5.1 presents the frequencies of computer game use in different age groups.
It reveals a clear increase with age: Whereas 40% of 6- and 7-year-olds hardly ever
play with a computer or game console, this drops to 29% in the intermediate age
group, and to 23% in the 10- to 11-year-olds.
A comparison between those girls and boys who “very often” or “sometimes”
spend their time playing computer games reveals a gender effect: When girls are
asked to give more detailed reports on the extent of their use of computers or game
consoles, 24% say that they hardly ever or only sometimes play with computers.
Only 13% of boys report such a low usage. Twenty-three percent of girls spend a
maximum of one hour a week on computer games compared to 18% of boys. In
contrast, 65% of boys report using computers several times a week or even every
day, whereas, at 50%, notably fewer girls report such intensive use (Fig. 5.2).
Both the findings on the increasing frequency of playing computer games as
children grow older and the gender differences found here are in line with the results
of the German KIM study on children and the media. This confirmed a greater affin-
2
This question has been posed only since the 2010 Child Study that also surveyed 6- to 7-year-old
children. This permits a comparison of trends across age groups.
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 113
18
Not very often
10
6
Hardly ever
3
ity to digital games among boys and a decline in the number of nonplayers with
increasing age (Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbunds Südwest 2013).
5.2 A
Typology of Leisure Time: Groups Differ in Reading
and Media Consumption
The typology of leisure time developed for the World Vision Child Study is based
on the list of leisure activities reported in Table 5.1. A factor analysis of the fre-
quency reports revealed four different domains of leisure activities. These could be
labeled Culture, Sport/Exercise, Media, and Play (at home). These factors then
formed the starting point for a cluster analysis that resulted in a typology containing
three groups of children: “media consumers,” “normal leisure users,” and
“all-rounders.”
According to the present data for 2013, approximately one-quarter of the chil-
dren can be assigned to media consumers (26%) and one-quarter to the all-rounders
(25%). The remaining one-half (49%) are normal leisure users. This corresponds
roughly with the distribution across the different groups found in both the First and
Second World Vision Child Studies.
Figure 5.3 shows that whereas some leisure activities are engaged in equally
frequently by children in all three groups, others are particularly characteristic of
one of the two extreme groups: media consumers or all-rounders. Hence, these
activities can be used to differentiate between the two. For example, media consum-
114 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
- Sports 55 53 51
- Listening to music 59 55 43
- Meeting friends 55 49 50
- Watching television 13 48 87
- Nature or animals 52 30 19
- Reading 61 29 8
- Lego™/Playmobil™ 25 28 29
- PlayStation/Computer games 2 16 56
- Making music 48 15 4
36 10 2
- Theater/Dance/Ballet
- Making things with tools 8 9 6
ers are characterized by a particularly frequent use of electronic media such as tele-
vision, computers, and game consoles. They very infrequently engage in creative
and cultural activities such as reading, making things, and painting or making
music. Normal leisure users can be found in all activities; however, there are no
domains in which they stand out through extreme engagement or a complete lack of
interest. Finally, as already reported in the previous Child Studies, all-rounders
engage particularly frequently in musical and cultural activities in which media
consumers show hardly any interest. The former read, make things and paint a lot,
make music, play theater, or they take ballet lessons. They are also characterized by
a particularly low television consumption and extremely infrequent use of computer
games.
As already seen in 2010, all-rounders engage in significantly more different lei-
sure activities in 2013 as well. However, as well as differing in the number of leisure
activities, the leisure types also differ in their content. Around one-third of all-
rounders say that they very often take part in a theater group, dance, or ballet,
whereas less than 10% of the normal leisure users and media consumers engage in
such activities very often. Results are similar for playing a musical instrument:
Almost one-half of the all-rounders do this frequently, but normal leisure users and
media consumers do so markedly less often. All-rounders also predominantly view
handicrafts, painting, and drawing as an integral part of their leisure time. More than
one-half of them report engaging in such activities very often compared to only
roughly one-third of normal leisure users and one-fifth of media consumers.
This difference between all-rounders and media consumers is particularly drastic
when it comes to reading – a leisure activity that is a particularly important basic
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 115
cultural technique in the musical and cultural domain. More than 60% of all-
rounders report that reading is one of the activities they engage in very often com-
pared to less than 10% of media consumers. Evidently, in many cases, media
consumers drop reading as a source of information and inspiration with all its
creative potential in favor of the markedly more passive medium of television. A
total of 87% of the children in this group report watching television very often com-
pared to only 13% of all-rounders. A clear difference can even be found when look-
ing at those children in both groups who report watching television very often:
Media consumers most frequently report watching television for 1–2 hours per day;
all-rounders, for 0.5–1 hour per day. This also shows how the children’s evaluation
standards differ: Whereas all-rounders already consider 30–60 min a day of televi-
sion as very often, this category stands for several hours a day in media-oriented
children. A correlation analysis reveals a significant negative relation between the
amount of television watching and the frequency of reading:3 Children who watch a
lot of television have less to do with books and vice versa.
Media consumers are also characterized quite clearly by a more frequent use of
further electronic media such as PlayStation, Nintendo, Wii, and other computer
games. In contrast, all-rounders hardly use these, with only 2% reporting that they
play with them often. More detailed questions on the extent of use reveal that 14%
of all-rounders play computer games several times a week or even daily compared
to 71% of media consumers. Hence, all-rounders and media consumers differ not
only in their self-reports on how far computer games belong to their preferred lei-
sure activities but also in the number of hours they report playing them. Here as
well, a rank correlation reveals a significant negative relation between the extent of
media use and the frequency of reading.
5.2.1 D
ifferent Family Incentive Structures: Broad Range
of Media Equipment Versus Books and Doing Things
Together
It is not just the reported leisure activities in the musical and creative domain and
media use that differentiate clearly between the various leisure groups. They also
show fundamental differences in what is available to them within their family
framework.
Whereas 44% of media consumers report having their own television in their
room, this is the case for only 18% of all-rounders. Approximately one-third of
media consumers have their own DVD or Blu-ray player compared to only about
10% of all-rounders. Likewise, an own computer (32%), a game console (51%), and
a Gameboy (72%) are to be found far more frequently in the media consumers’
rooms than in those of the all-rounders (computer: 24%, game console: 14%,
12
Three or more bookshelves
18
(more than 200 books)
38
15
About two bookshelves (101–
24
200 books)
25
40
About one bookshelf (25 –100
36
books)
28
19
About one shelf (11–24 Media consumers
14
books)
7 Normal leisure users
10 Versatile children
Only a few (up to 10 books) 4
1
Fig. 5.4 Number of books in the family home by different leisure types
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
Gameboy: 44%). However, the two groups hardly differ when it comes to having
their own mobile phone: 42% of media consumers and 38% of all-rounders.
However, at 48%, media consumers more frequently have their own Internet access
than all-rounders (42%).
The comparatively greater range of electronic media in the children’s rooms con-
trasts with the presence of books in the home (Fig. 5.4). When we ask both media
consumers and normal leisure users how many books there are in their homes, their
most frequent answer is “about one shelf (25–100 books).” All-rounders, in con-
trast, most frequently answer “three or more shelves (more than 200 books).” Even
when this is not explicitly literature for children, that is, books with which the
respondent’s themselves can spend their leisure time, answers do indicate the status
of reading in that household. Whereas all-rounders evidently grow up particularly
frequently in families in which books are considered to be important and are
correspondingly granted a great deal of space within the family home, media con-
sumers and normal leisure-time users live in less book-friendly environments.
Spending leisure time actively together is also particularly characteristic of fami-
lies with all-rounders. We find that 43% of these children report very frequently
going on excursions together with their families. This contrasts with only 26% of
normal leisure users and 14% of media consumers. In addition, parents of all-
rounders grant their children more codetermination about potential leisure activi-
ties. Whereas 68% of media consumers say that they can “sometimes” codetermine
what the family does together during leisure time, this compares with 75% of nor-
mal leisure users and even 83% of all-rounders. Interestingly, all-rounders have
more codetermination over what they do in general. A total of 90% are allowed to
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 117
codetermine what they do in their leisure time compared to 84% of normal leisure
users and 83% of media consumers (see also Chap. 7 on codetermination).
Hence, all-rounders receive more family support and encouragement in general,
and this expresses itself not only in an emphasis on musical and creative activities
but also in a broader family leisure program. Their particularly caring parents do not
prescribe the planning activities together with the family or leisure time in general.
In contrast, they coordinate them more closely with their children than parents of
the other two leisure types.
5.2.2 S
ocial and Material Constraints as Risk Factors
for Excessive Media Consumption
We computed a nominal regression to take a closer look at which factors in the fam-
ily influence the membership of one of the three leisure groups (Table 5.2).4 Results
show that girls belong more frequently to the all-rounders and less frequently to the
media consumers than boys. This is not surprising because there is a relatively
strong agreement between the leisure pursuits that girls report engaging in particu-
larly often and those preferred by all-rounders. Already in 2010, we found that three
times as many girls as boys were all-rounders. This is also confirmed in the present
study, along with the finding that this relation inverts for media consumers. The age
groups differ insofar as children in the youngest age group belong significantly less
often to the media consumers than members of the other two age groups.
Computations also show that children from the lower social classes are less likely to
belong to the all-rounder group than those from the more educated classes, and
there is a very strong probability of finding them among the media consumers. A
care deficit from one or both parents is also a risk factor for children who spend their
leisure time as media consumers. In addition, children who report having experi-
enced poverty significantly less frequently belong to the group of all-rounders com-
pared to their more affluent peers.
Hence, both material and social deficits within the family continue to impose
decisive constraints on the versatility of children’s leisure time. On the one hand, the
parents’ lack of financial resources makes it harder for them to offer their children a
comparable range of opportunities as in more affluent families. On the other hand,
deficits in social care from either one or both parents can lead a child to turn to the
media as an alternative source of stimulation. In contrast, no fundamental cultural
differences can be ascertained. A possible migration background does not, in itself,
have any effect in either one or the other direction. What is decisive here is how
class membership moderates the relation. Whereas 20% of native German children
belong to the lower class or the lower middle class, this applies to 34% of children
Table 5.2 Significant relations between leisure types and personal and social variablesa
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
% (per line) Versatile children Normal leisure users Media consumers
Total 25 49 26
Gender
Girls 38b 49 13
Boys 12 49 39b
Social origin
Lower class 7b 47 46b
Lower middle class 13b 53 34
Middle class 20 50 30
Upper middle class 31b 48 21b
Upper class 45b 44 11b
Experienced poverty
No experience of poverty 27 48 25
Constraints 22 51 27
Specific experience of 14b 52 34
poverty
Age
6–7 years 26 51 23b
8–9 years 26 47 27
10–11 years 23 48 29
Parental care and attention
No deficit 26 50 24
Deficit in one parent 20 45 35b
Care deficit 20 44 36b
a
The relationship was tested with a multivariate nominal regression on the variable “leisure”
Variables included: age, gender, social origin, experience of poverty, care deficit, and migration
background
b
For ease of presentation, bivariate values are presented as percentages in the table. Notable devia-
tions for variables that were also significant within the multivariate statistical analysis are printed
in bold (p < .05)
5.2.3 L
eisure-Time Types and Their Activities Across Time:
Increasingly Less Time for Reading
activities. When tracked across time in all three leisure groups, we can see a slight
decline in television consumption. Whereas in 2007, 56% of children reported
watching television very often, this dropped to 48% in 2010 before rising again
slightly to 49% in 2013.5 In all three survey years, frequent computer use varies
between 25% and 30%, and about one-third of all children read very often.
However, the two extreme groups reveal a somewhat different trend: Whereas
television viewing remains on a constantly high level of approximately 80% to 90%
in media consumers, it is declining among all-rounders. Whereas in 2007, 24% of
the latter still reported watching television very frequently; in 2013, it is only 12%.
It is not only the proportion of all-rounders who watch a lot of television that is in
decline, but also the amount of time they spend watching each day. Of the 8- to
11-year-old all-rounders who reported watching television very often in 2007, 23%
watched for 2–3 hours per day. In 2013, this has dropped to only 14%. Among
media consumers, in contrast, we observe an opposite trend. Whereas in 2007,
approximately 30% watched television for 2–3 hours or more a day, in 2013,
approximately 40% of those who say they watch television very often do so for 2–3
hours or more.
All-rounders also reveal a decline in playing computer games. Whereas approxi-
mately 60% of 8- to 11-year-old media consumers report playing on computers or
game consoles very often in all three surveys, the proportion of all-rounders here
has dropped from 10% to 2%. Moreover, when asked in more detail, the proportion
of all-rounders who report spending time on computer games several times a week
or even daily has dropped from 18% in 2010 to the aforementioned 14% in 2013.
For the media-oriented children, in contrast, the proportion reporting several times
a week or daily continues to be 71%.
Regarding reading, we can initially determine no clear trend. About 10% of
media consumers report reading “very often” in all three surveys. In 2007, 63% of
the all-rounders reported very often spending their leisure time reading; in 2010,
this even rose to 76%, and in 2013, this returned to almost the same frequency as in
2007 at 62%. Hence, the subjectively perceived frequency of spending time with
books seems to remain more or less constant or is subject to fluctuations that fail to
form any clear trend. However, when questioned more closely as to how often they
read or look at books in their homes, we can see that the frequency of spending time
with books is declining. Among media consumers, it was 9% in 2007, 5% in 2010,
and now 6% in 2013 who spend time with books every day. Among all-rounders,
this was 42% in 2007, 39% in 2010, and is now only 28% in 2013. It is precisely in
this group in which frequent reading is a fundamental part of leisure that we can see
a particularly significant decline in its actual frequency.
5
To ensure comparability, only the trend calculations were included for 8- to 11-year-olds covering
the years 2007, 2010, and 2013.
120 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
As pointed out above, family factors exert a major influence on how children shape
their leisure. Whereas those from more affluent social classes more frequently
belong to the all-rounder group and spend their leisure in a variety of ways with a
focus on musical and creative activities, children from the lower social classes tend
toward a more one-sided media-oriented use of leisure. Government interventions
such as special social transfers to children (for extra tuition or school materials)
should help to ensure that children from lower social classes can also take advantage
of social, cultural, and educational facilities. Funds are made available for these
children to, for example, join a sports club or attend a music school. On the one
hand, this can offer them experiences that their own families are unable to provide
because of limited income, and, on the other hand, it could compensate for any
potential care deficits. In view of the finding that 10% of children from the lower
class report a lack of attention from one parent and even 28% a lack of attention
from both, it is precisely the second aspect that would seem to indicate the most
important task for extrafamilial and leisure services.
Germany has a wide range of institutional leisure and cultural provisions. These
are traditionally organized in the form of clubs and associations – particularly when
it comes to sports. There are also church-run provisions as well as what are fre-
quently private and commercial services provided by music, dance, and ballet
schools. In the Child Study, we asked parents to tell us whether their child is a
member of such a club, association or other kind of organized group.
With a few individual exceptions, trends reveal a slight decline in memberships
of clubs and groups or the use of other such provisions since the last Child Study.
Between 2007 and 2010 in contrast, we found a slight increase.6 Among 8- to
11-year-olds, who have been surveyed since the first Child Study, 74% belonged to
at least one club in 2007 and 80% in 2010, whereas in 2013, 78% of parents report
that their child is organized in at least one regular group or association. As before, it
is membership of sports clubs that is strongest among the 8- to 11-year-olds. At
57%, more than one child in two is active here. This is followed by musical and
cultural provisions (music groups/music schools: 21%, dance club/ballet: 13%).
Activities in church-run groups are reported for 7% of children. Further member-
ships and use of provisions are listed in Table 5.3.
6
We have surveyed 6- to 7-year-olds only since 2010. However, the increase in memberships iden-
tified here could be ascertained independent of age.
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 121
Table 5.3 Membership in clubs and organized groups or use of other such provisions
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007: 8–11 years)
Positive answers per cell in % 2007 2010 2013
Clubs, groups, or use of other provisions (multiple responses)
Sports club 58 62 57
Music group/music school 21 23 21
Dance club/Ballet 10 10 13
Painting/drawing group 2 2 2
Theater or movie group 3 3 2
Church group 11 10 7
Girl guides/boy scouts 2 2 2
Nature or animal protection society 1 1 1
Organized group in a child or youth club 4 3 2
Traditional costumes and folklore clubs 1 1 1
Other 7 7 5
None of the above 27 22 25
Table 5.4 reports the proportions of children who are members of clubs, belong to
organized groups, or use other musical and cultural provisions across time broken
down according to social origins. Trends show a conspicuous decline particularly
among 6- to 7-year-olds. This applies above all to children from the lower classes.
There are also fluctuations among the 8- to 9-year-olds and the 10- to 11-year-
olds, although these do not relate to social class so clearly. Looking just at the lower
class, the proportion of 8- to 9-year-olds belonging to clubs or groups in our first
Child Study in 2007 was 35%. This rose to 49% in 2010, and has now dropped back
to 37%. Among the 10- to 11-year-olds, memberships in the lowest social class have
fluctuated from 58% over 47% to a current 53%. Among the 6- to 7-year-olds who
have been included since our second Child Study, the rate is currently 18% com-
pared to 30% in 2010.
5.3.2 S
ocial Class and Not Type of School Determines Club
Membership
When interpreting these findings, it is worth considering the influence of the type of
school children are attending. Those attending an all-day school have less free time
at their disposal for clubs and groups. Moreover, the all-day school provisions rang-
ing from sports to music and theater groups already cover several activities that are
otherwise organized in clubs. As reported in Chap. 4, the proportion of all-day stu-
dents rose from 13% in 2007 to 17% in 2010 and is now 24% in 2013. Moreover,
lower class children attend all-day schools more frequently than those from higher
122 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
social classes (31% all-day students in the lower class vs. 20% in the upper class).
Hence, the greater attendance of all-day schools already at elementary school age
and particularly among children from lower social classes could correspondingly
lead to a lower participation in clubs and groups.
Interestingly, however, the proportions of both all-day and half-day students who
are members of a club or an established group have converged strongly in recent
years. Whereas in 2007, 25% of the 8- to 11-year-old half-day students and 37% of
the all-day students of the same age were not in any club or using a corresponding
service, in 2010, these figures had dropped to 19% and 27% respectively. In the cur-
rent survey, 25% of the parents of half-day and 27% of the parents of all-day stu-
dents report that their child does not belong to a club or engage in any other
provisions (Table 5.5). The only relevant difference is found in sports clubs: These
are used more frequently by children in this age range attending half-day (58%)
than all-day schools (52%). Hence, attending a half-day or all-day school does not
seem to be decisive for membership of clubs or groups in this age range.
However, as Table 5.4 has shown already, class-related differences can be seen
clearly in all three Child Studies. Across all survey times and age groups, the fre-
quency of club memberships increases clearly in line with social status. The current
data from the third Child Study indicate that it is particularly children from the low-
est social class who continue to be insufficiently integrated into the great range of
club provisions available in Germany – even though these are basically open to
every child.
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 123
Table 5.5 Membership in a club, participation in an organized group, or use of other such
provisions by type of school
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
Half-day All-day
Positive answers, Per cell in % Total school school
Clubs, groups or use of other provisions (multiple responses)
Sports club 56 58 52
Music group/Music school 21 21 22
Dance club/Ballet 13 13 13
Painting/drawing group 2 2 2
Theater or movie group 2 2 2
Church group 7 8 5
Girl guides/Boy scouts 2 2 1
Nature or animal protection society 1 1 1
Organized group in a child or youth club 2 3 2
Traditional costumes and folklore clubs 1 1 1
Other 5 5 5
None of above 25 25 27
5.3.3 M
usic and Sport Groups Also Fail to Reach the Lower
Class
As well as asking about club memberships, the items in the parent questionnaire tap
a whole series of further group activities or other provisions. In the following, we
want to take a closer look at memberships of sports clubs and music groups or music
schools. These are both activities that the German Federal Ministry of Labor and
Social Affairs has named as being exemplary for the goals of government interven-
tions to increase the participation of socially disadvantaged children (BMAS 2013).
Due to the influence of age on the frequency of club memberships, we shall examine
the different age groups separately here as well.
Across all social classes, 58% of 8- to 9-year-olds and 58% of 10- to 11-year-
olds belonged to a sports club in 2007. In 2010, 58% of parents reported such a
membership for their 6- to 7-year-old children; 63%, for their 8- to 9-year-olds; and
64%, for their 10- to 11-year-olds. In the current study, 52% of parents of 6- to
7-year-olds report that their child is a member of a sports club compared to 59% of
the parents of 8- to 9-year-olds and 58% of the parents of 10- to 11-year-olds.
Hence, there is a degree of fluctuation around 60% for the two older groups and
between 50% and 60% for younger children. This does not reveal any clear trend
toward more or less frequent club memberships in the sports domain.
The proportion of music group and music school memberships is also relatively
stable in all age groups. ln 2007, 20% of 8- to 9-year-olds and 21% of 10- to 11-year-
olds belonged to a music group. In 2010, it was 20% of 6- to 7-year-olds, 26% of
8- to 9-year-olds, and 22% of 10- to 11-year-olds. In the current survey, the
124 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
musical instrument or making music “very often.” In 2010, this rose to 5%, and in
2013, to 8%. Hence, there is a very slightly positive trend, but this is still far from
catching up with the opportunities for participation available to the children of fami-
lies in a better social position.
If we also look at children attending a half-day school separately here in order to
rule out any confounds between attendance of an all-day school and memberships
of clubs or groups, we can see, as already pointed out above, that the decline in club
memberships cannot be explained exclusively through an increase in attendance of
all-day schools and the accompanying availability of leisure provisions within the
school. The lower class 8- to 11-year-old half-day students are slightly less fre-
quently members of a sports club or a music group in 2013 compared to either 2007
or 2010.
5.4.1 S
ocial Class Continues to Be Decisive for Access
to the Internet
5.4.2 E
xtent of Internet Use: Use of the World Wide Web Is
Equally Widespread in All Leisure Types
Children reveal just as strong a trend toward increasing use of the Internet as that
found in adults. However, this applies only to older children from the age of 10 to
11 years onward. In both 2007 and 2010, 13% of 8- to 9-year-olds reported regu-
larly spending time on the Internet. Currently, this is 11% of this age group. Among
10- to 11-year-olds, the proportion regularly using the Internet every week has risen
from 29% in 2007, across 32% in 2010, to a current 36%. According to their own
reports, only 4% of 6- to 7-year-olds regularly use the Internet each week. We shall
have to wait and see how things develop further. However, due to the increasing
availability of mobile web-enabled end devices, we can assume that regular Internet
use will increase even further among children.
Children who use the Internet regularly do so mostly for up to 1 hour (26%) or
up to 2 hours (23%) a week. The frequencies for the other categories (“no more than
half an hour,” “up to 3/4/5 hours,” and “more than 5 hours” per week) are all roughly
10%. A breakdown according to age groups in Fig. 5.5 shows that the proportion of
10- to 11-year-olds who spend more than 2 hours on the Internet each week is
higher than that of 8- to 9-year-olds. Whereas 8- to 9-year-olds most frequently
(37%) use the Internet for up to 1 hour per week, and only 25% report using it for
more than 2 hours, 44% of the older children spend more than 2 hours a week
online.7
7
Because of the low number of cases of regular Internet users in the group of 6- to 7-year-olds (29
cases), we report no frequencies for the youngest age group here.
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 127
Looking at the different leisure types, it is interesting to see that when we ask
media consumers and normal leisure users who are regularly online how often they
use the Internet each week, they most frequently answer “up to 1 hour,” whereas the
most frequent answer among all-rounders is “up to 2 hours.” We find that 36% of
media consumers, 41% of normal leisure users, and 36% of all-rounders use the
Internet for more than 2 hours a week. Hence, we can see no clear differences
between the single leisure types regarding the extent of activities on the Internet.
However, when asking 6- to 11-year-olds about their Internet use, we are naturally
not just interested in how much they use it but also what they do on the Internet and
how the age groups and leisure types differ here.
128 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
37
Up to 1 hour
24
25 8-9 years
Up to 2 hours
22
10-11 years
10
Up to 3 hours
13
3
Up to 4 hours
11
6
Up to 5 hours
8
5
More than 5 hours
12
Fig. 5.5 Frequency of Internet use per week by different age groups
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years who use the Internet regularly each week (%)
5.4.3 W
hat Children Do on the Internet: Clear Differences
Between Age Groups and Leisure Types
Table 5.8 presents an overview of children’s activities on the Internet. It reports the
frequencies of the categories receiving “very often” answers and includes only chil-
dren who are regularly online.
Gender differences in Internet use are conspicuously low. The only marked
exception is online games that are played by more than 40% of boys compared to
only one-quarter of girls. All other functions such as downloading music and videos
or making and communicating with friends over Facebook or chat lines are used
almost equally frequently by both sexes.
In contrast, there are clear age differences. Whereas younger children focus more
on online games and watching and downloading videos, older children reveal an
increasing participation in social networks and chat lines. Although only 8% of 8-
and 9-year-olds report using Facebook and the like, social networks are the most
frequent activity on the Internet reported by 10- to 11-year-olds at 33%. Chat lines
are also gaining in importance: Whereas these are a very frequent form of Internet
use in only 15% of 8- and 9-year-olds, this rises to almost one-third in the oldest
group. Older children also more frequently use e-mails as a means of communica-
tion. This is certainly not just an age-dependent change in interests, but also reflects
an increase in reading and writing competence. Although social networks have very
graphic displays and are based on pictures just as much as writing, basic reading and
writing competencies are essential to tell things to other users and understand what
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 129
they say in return. This is particularly true of chat lines that are frequently not char-
acterized as being orthographically and grammatically correct, but by a high speed
of information flow. An interesting fact in relation to the use of social networks is
that Facebook, for example, has a minimum age of 13 years, so that the respondents
in the Child Study do not yet even belong to the official circle of users. Nonetheless,
children succeed in using the online service by either pretending to be older than
they actually are or by gaining the help of older persons. This is in line with the
findings of the latest KIM study reporting that Facebook is, at 17%, the most popu-
lar website among 6- to 13-year-olds (Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbunds
Südwest 2013). Twitter, in contrast, does not play a role in any of the age groups
surveyed in the present Child Study.
Whereas there are no clear differences between the various leisure types regard-
ing the amount of Internet use, they differ strongly in the ways they use it. Media
consumers particularly frequently take part in online computer games and chat lines
compared to the other two groups. They also use Facebook and other social net-
works slightly more frequently and download more frequently than all-rounders.
Normal leisure users and all-rounders, in contrast, use the Internet particularly fre-
quently for specific searches, and all-rounders use it particularly to send e-mails.
Hence, media consumers tend to use the Internet to extend the entertainment media
they already have at their disposal in order to play games online and engage in fast
and uncommitted communication through chat lines. All-rounders, in contrast, use
the Internet more for obtaining information and for specific and purposeful
communication.
130 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
5.5 T
he Mobile Phone: Simply Taken for Granted by Many
Children
The 2010 Child Study has already shown that the mobile phone is no longer just a
part of daily life for youths; it is also increasing something that is simply taken for
granted by 8- to 11-year-olds. Whereas in 2007, only 17% of 8- and 9-year-olds and
56% of 10- to 11-year-olds had a mobile phone, this already rose to 27% and 66%
in 2010. Nowadays, the proportion of children in both age groups with a mobile
phone is even slightly higher. In 2013, 31% of the middle group and 72% of the
oldest group say they have their own mobile phone. The latest KIM study
(Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbunds Südwest 2013) also reports compara-
ble findings on children with mobile phones. There continues to be a leap in the
frequency of having one’s own mobile phone between the 8- to 9-year-old and 10-
to 11-year-old children. This matches the findings of the German FIM study on the
family and the media that reports 10 years as the average age for children to have
their own mobile phones (Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbunds Südwest
2012). The sudden leap reflects the transition from elementary to secondary school.
Whereas 59% of 10- to 11-year-olds attending an elementary school report having
their own mobile phones, this rises to 78% among those attending secondary
schools. The decision not to give their child a mobile phone at elementary school
but to do so when changing to secondary school could relate, on the one hand, to
restrictive rules at elementary school (mobile phones not being allowed); but, on the
other hand, to the greater need to exchange information due to varying teaching
hours at secondary schools. The youngest age group reveals a slight decline from
9% to 6%. Perhaps because mobile phones have become so much more expensive
since the widespread introduction of smart phones, parents are unwilling to pur-
chase such valuable instruments for their children.
The gender difference found in the last two Child Studies is also found in 2013.
In 2007, 39% of girls and 34% of boys reported having a mobile phone. In 2010,
this had risen to 50% of girls and 44% of boys among the 8- to 11-year-olds. In
2013, 56% of girls and 49% of boys in this age group report having their own
mobile phone. On the one hand, this may be because parents trust their daughters to
use their mobile phones more responsibly. On the other hand, it could also reflect a
greater concern for girls’ welfare by ensuring that they are able to contact their par-
ents at any time.
The various leisure types reveal only slight differences in having their own mobile
phone: 43% of all media consumers, 39% of normal leisure users, and 38% of all-
rounders. An examination of age groups reveals that media consumers in the young-
est age group tend to have their own mobile phone more often (9%) than their peers
in the other leisure groups (6%). However, because the difference is only slight and
the number of cases is low, this finding should be interpreted with caution.
To analyze the effect of various personal, social, and structural features on the
probability of having one’s own mobile phone more precisely, we computed a logis-
tic regression (Table 5.9). This showed that children in the oldest survey group sig-
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 131
nificantly more frequently have their own mobile phone than children in the younger
groups. The gender effect reported above also proves to be statistically significant.
Likewise, children living in urban areas more frequently have their own mobile
phone than those growing up in rural areas. The finding already reported in 2010
that children growing up in single-parent households more probably have their own
mobile phone than those living in other family constellations proves to be a stable
trend. In 2007, 47% of 8- to 11-year-olds living with a single parent had their own
mobile phone compared to 34% living in other types of family. In 2010, this rose to
52% versus 46%. And now, in 2013, 62% of the 8- to 11-year-olds living in single-
parent households have their own mobile phone compared to 50% living in other
types of family. Being easy to reach and able to quickly make flexible rearrange-
ments seem to be particularly important in families with only one caregiver who
needs to coordinate different obligations such as going to work, caring for children,
and organizing leisure activities. In contrast, membership of a specific social class
has no significant effect on the probability of having one’s own mobile phone.
132 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
Hence, independent of income and education, parents equally frequently give their
children a mobile phone to use. Likewise, the statistical comparison between nor-
mal leisure users and the two extreme groups of media consumers and all-rounders
reveals no significant differences in having one’s own mobile phone.
5.6 S
atisfaction With Leisure is Also a Question of Social
Class
The two previous Child Studies showed that social variables impact decisively on
how satisfied children are with their leisure time. Social origins and experienced
poverty determine the children’s life worlds and thereby also impact directly on
their subjective satisfaction with the leisure activities available to them.
Satisfaction scores are generally high. A total of 59% of all children surveyed are
very satisfied with their leisure time, 33% are satisfied, and only 8% view it as being
negative or neutral. In the present survey, the children are even slightly more satis-
fied than they were in the 2010 Child Study. Because of the very high general level
of satisfaction, in the following, we shall compare only “very positive” responses
with the frequency of other responses.
A comparison of the various social classes reveals that very positive ratings are
significantly less frequent in lower class children compared to their middle-class
peers, and that the former tend to give negative to neutral (17%) or positive (53%)
ratings (Table 5.10). The drastic nature of these findings becomes apparent only
when we compare them directly with the reports of the other groups in which the
proportion of negative to neutral ratings lies between 6% and 11%, and positive
ratings between 30% and 32%. Results are similar for experienced poverty. Children
who have already experienced poverty directly in their own daily lives also tend to
rate their leisure time less positively than their peers who have no prior exposure to
poverty. Seventeen percent of them rate their leisure as negative to neutral and 39%
as positive. Children who have already experienced constraints in their daily lives at
least once due to lack of money also less frequently rate their leisure as being very
positive compared to their materially more affluent peers. These reports can be com-
pared with the ratings from children who have not experienced poverty and con-
straints in daily life. Only 7% view their leisure as being negative to neutral, only
31% as being positive, whereas the majority of 62% view it as being very positive.
As before, we have to assume a clear dependence of children’s satisfaction on
external material factors. Up to now, it has not been possible to enable lower class
children to experience their leisure as positively as children from more prosperous
classes do every day.
Nonetheless, there are some signs of a positive trend. Whereas in 2010, 28% of
lower class children still rated their leisure as being negative to neutral, this has now
dropped to 17% in 2013. These changes are not to be found in the very positive rat-
5 Leisure Time: Varied and Colorful, but Not For All Children 133
Table 5.10 Significant relations between satisfaction with leisure and personal and social
variablesa
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
% (per cell) Negative to neutral Positive Very positive
All children 8 33 59
Social origin
Lower class 17b 53b 30
Lower middle class 11 32 57
Middle class 8 32 60
Upper middle class 6 31 63
Upper class 7 30 64
Experienced poverty
No experience of poverty 7 31 62
Constraints 6 45b 49
Specific experience of poverty 17b 39b 44
Leisure type
Media consumers 11 40b 49
Normal leisure users 8 34 58
Versatile children 6 26b 68
a
The relationship was tested with a multiple analysis of variance
Variables included: age, gender, leisure type, migration background, social origin, and experienced
poverty
b
For ease of presentation, bivariate values are presented as percentages in the table. Notable devia-
tions for variables that were also significant within the multivariate statistical analysis are printed
in bold (p < .05)
ings, but in the positive ones. In 2010, 38% of children from the lowest social class
viewed their leisure as positive; in the current Child Study, it has risen to 53%. Even
when we are far from having achieved a leisure time that all children perceive as
being very positive, there are at least first signs of a shift in the right direction.
Nonetheless, this slightly positive trend cannot be explained through participation
in groups and clubs, because, as reported in Sect. 5.3, this is declining in the lower
class. It also cannot be explained by the higher proportion of all-day students in this
social class. In agreement with the findings reported in Chap. 4 showing that it is
particularly children from the lowest social class who are more often dissatisfied
with their all-day school, there is even a decline in satisfaction with their own lei-
sure compared to 2010 that is linked closely to the institution they are attending.
Whereas in the 2010 Child Study, 43% of all-day students from the lower class still
rated their leisure time very positively, this has dropped to only 29% in 2013. Here
we can see a shift from very positive ratings toward the intermediate category of
positive ratings. This can also be understood as indicating that the rapidly imposed
expansion of all-day schools has yet to deliver qualitatively convincing results. The
success of this initiative will also have to be measured in terms of how the children
being cared for in these institutions appraise them. In contrast, we can see a positive
trend in half-day students from the lower class. In 2010, 29% of them rated their
leisure as negative to neutral and 42% as positive. In 2013, only 16% of these
134 A. Jänsch and U. Schneekloth
c hildren still give a negative to neutral rating, and 53% of them view their leisure
activities positively. The available data provide no clear explanation for this increase
in satisfaction among half-day students from the lower class, because this group
also reveals a decline in memberships of clubs and groups.
A comparison of the satisfaction ratings in all three leisure groups reveals that
media consumers less frequently give a very positive rating than normal leisure
users and significantly more frequently rate their leisure as positive. Vice versa,
all-rounders more frequently rate their leisure as very positive and significantly less
frequently give only a positive rating.
In contrast, we can find no statistically significant influence of age and gender or
of a possible migration background on the children’s ratings.
The perceived possibility of codetermination when shaping their own leisure
time also proves to be an important factor for children’s satisfaction. Among those
whose parents let them help decide how they spend their leisure together, 60% view
their leisure as “very positive” compared to only 54% among those who are given
little or no codetermination. The relevance of the possibility of codetermination
becomes even clearer in answers to the question: “Can you decide for yourself what
you do in your free time?” Whereas 61% of the children who answered “generally
yes” rated their leisure as “very positive,” among those who said “generally no,”
only 41% gave very positive ratings. As presented in more detail in Sect. 7.1, pos-
sibilities of codetermination in daily life are an important aspect of participatory
justice in children’s lives. If adults integrate children into the decision-making pro-
cesses that concern them and they thereby feel they are being treated fairly, then
they also report greater satisfaction.
We computed a rank correlation to see whether there is a relation between satisfac-
tion with one’s own leisure time and general satisfaction with life. This shows a highly
significant positive relation between the two variables. Children who report a greater
satisfaction with their lives in general are also more satisfied with their leisure time.8
References
BMAS (Bundesministerium für Arbeit und Soziales). (2013). Das Bildungspaket. Das ist drin im
Bildungspaket. Verfügbar unter. www.bildungspaket.bmas.de.
Leven, I., & Schneekloth, U. (2007). Die Freizeit. Anregen lassen oder fernsehen. In World Vision
Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 165 – 200).
Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest. (Hrsg.). (2012). FIM-Studie (2011): Familie,
Interaktion & Medien. Untersuchung zur Kommunikation und Mediennutzung in Familien.
Stuttgart: Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest: LFK.
Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest. (Hrsg.). (2013). KIM-Studie (2012): Kinder
+ Medien, Computer + Internet, Basisuntersuchung zum Medienumgang 6- bis 13-Jähriger in
Deutschland. Stuttgart:Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest: LFK, LKM.
Agnes Jänsch and Monika Pupeter
Alongside children’s reference persons in the family and school, it is friends who
are the most important social group in their lives. Children meet their peers in the
neighborhoods they live in, at school, perhaps when attending institutional daycare
centers in the afternoons, and also in the clubs to which they belong. And where
children interact with their peers, they can also build up friendships (Pupeter and
Schneekloth 2010). In different phases of life, friendships take different forms and
fulfil different developmental tasks. In early childhood, they particularly take the
form of joint play, and they enable children to practice solving conflicts and control-
ling their emotions. In middle childhood, the focus is on being socially accepted by
one’s peers and avoiding rejection; and an important task during this phase is to
acquire rules for expressing emotions adequately. In adolescence, friendships help
young people to explore and define themselves. In this phase, they develop an
understanding of the role feelings play in social relationships (Parker and Gottman
1989, as cited in Oerter and Montada 1998). Numerous studies have confirmed the
importance of stable friendships for not only emotional well-being but also social
competencies and problem-solving abilities in children of all ages (Salvas et al.
2011). Friends can offer emotional support when stressed, while simultaneously
serving as a source of cognitive development and knowledge acquisition (Prazen
et al. 2011).
In the transitional phase from infancy or preschool age to adolescence, friend-
ships develop a new quality, because it is during this phase that children themselves
begin to build up confidence and trust between equal-ranking partners and thereby
also to find solutions when conflict situations arise. In the following, we want to
start by taking a closer look at the extent of the circle of friends in 6- to 11-year-old
children during this important phase of social development and analyzing which
factors are relevant for the formation of peer relationships. Then, we want to ask
where children meet their friends, whereby we shall also pay particular attention to
the possibilities of virtual contacts. Finally, we shall turn to the topic of trust and
being appreciated in friendships in order to look at how children appraise their sat-
isfaction with this aspect of their lives.
6.1.1 P
ersonal Freedoms as an Important Precondition
for Friendships
To gain an idea of the size of the peer groups in which children locate themselves,
we first asked them to tell us how many friends they have. At 34%, the most frequent
response to this question is “10 or more.” About one-quarter report 6–9; another
one-quarter, 4–5 friends. This leaves 13% who count only two to three peers in their
circle of friends, and 1% who report having only one friend or no friends at all. A
comparison of boys and girls reveals no significant gender differences. As Fig. 6.1
shows, the namings in the different response categories are about equal in both
groups.
In contrast, there is a marked increase in friendships as children grow older
(Table 6.1). Whereas 17% of 6- to 7-year-olds cultivate friendships with two to three
other children, only 11 to 12% of older children report having such a small circle.
Only roughly one-quarter of the members of the youngest group view 10 or more
children as their friends, whereas this grows to 33% in the 8- and 9-year-olds and
even 44% in the 10- to 11-year-olds. This increase in the size of the circle of friends
also reflects the children’s growing autonomy. Forty-five percent of the 6- and
7-year-olds report “meeting friends” as a very frequent leisure-time pursuit com-
pared to 51% of the intermediate age group and 55% of the oldest children (see
Sect. 5.1). Hence, as they grow older, children acquire more opportunities to meet
35 34
Girls 27
24 25 26
Boys
13 13
1 2
0 to 1 friend 2 to 3 4 to 5 6 to 9 10 or more
40
38 38
2007
26 25
23 24 24
2010 22
2013 14
11 12
1 1 1
0 to 1 friend 2 to 3 4 to 5 6 to 9 10 or more
their peers and thereby cultivate and intensify their friendships. In addition, older
children report having more rights to codetermine whom they meet and how many
friends they can invite back to their homes (see Sect. 7.1). Even 81% of the young-
est group feel that they have the right to codetermine their choice of social contacts.
By the age of 10–11 years, this proportion rises even further to 92%. Forty-six per-
cent of 6- to 7-year-olds, 43% of 8- to 9-year-olds, and 54% of 10- to 11-year-olds
can decide how many friends they can bring home with them. Whereas adults exert
a stronger control over the social contacts of younger children, the greater freedom
of decision making granted to older children enables them to purposefully build up,
cultivate, and deepen contacts with peers toward whom they feel a special empathy.
As a result, they are able to acquire an increasingly large circle of friends. As
reported in Sect. 5.3, older children are also more frequently members of clubs or
leisure-time groups that also offer opportunities to make further social contacts and
extend their circle of acquaintances and friends.
Over time, in contrast, results are highly consistent. As Fig. 6.2 shows, approxi-
mately 40% of 8- to 11-year-olds report a large circle of friends containing 10 or
more peers in all three Child Studies. Roughly one-half of children report having
between 5 and 9 friends. And slightly more than 10% of respondents have only a
small or no circle of friends. Without reporting on the type and quality of these
138 A. Jänsch and M. Pupeter
0% 50% 100%
2007 1 1 10 21 27 41
Versatile 2 1 6 14 30 49
2010
children
2013 3 1 9 22 25 43
2007 5 1 13 24 22 40
Normal
2010 6 1 11 24 25 39
leisure users
2013 7 1 11 23 25 40
2007 9 2 19 25 23 31
Media
consumers 2010 10 3 13 23 26 35
2013 11 2 15 26 24 33
social contacts at this stage, we can already see that at least the size of the social
networks of children surveyed in the three Child Studies has not been subject to any
major change up to the present.
6.1.2 F
riends Are Not Made In Front of the Computer or
Television
Marked differences in the size of the circle of friends relate not only to age but also
to the different leisure groups (see Sect. 5.2). Whereas media consumers more fre-
quently report having a relatively small circle of friends, all-rounders generally have
a larger circle with 16% of the former group reporting that they have two to three
friends compared to only 10% of the latter. Thirty percent of media consumers
report having a circle of friends containing more than 10 children compared to 39%
of all-rounders. Table 6.1 presents an overview of responses broken down into age
groups and leisure types.
As Fig. 6.3 shows, relations between the various leisure types are stable across
time. Only about one-third of media consumers report having a circle of friends
containing more than 10 children across all three surveys compared to more than
40% of all-rounders and about 40% of normal leisure users. In contrast, small cir-
cles containing three or less children are more typical for the media consumers. In
all three Child Studies, 16 to 21% of this group reported only one to three friends
compared to 12 to 14% of normal leisure users and 7 to 11% of all-rounders.
6 Friendships Among Peers 139
An inspection of the preferred leisure pursuits of the children reveals that media
consumers meet up with their friends somewhat less often than all-rounders (50%
vs. 55% “very often”). In addition, the leisure pursuits of media consumers far more
frequently include computer games and television, as reported in Chap. 5. Fifty-six
percent of them report playing computer games very often and 87% watch televi-
sion very often, whereas only 2% of all-rounders play computer games very often
and only 13% watch television very often. Whereas the leisure behavior of all-
rounders with its stronger focus on social contacts rather than media consumption
helps to build up a circle of friends, the passive activities of media consumers char-
acterized by a lack of direct interactions are inappropriate for making new contacts
and cultivating them. Nonetheless, we have to ask what is the cause and what is the
effect here. It would seem just as plausible to assume that children who find it hard
to make friends and do not manage to build up a stable circle of friends will tend to
withdraw and spend their leisure time with activities that they can carry out alone.
6.1.3 L
ower Class Children Are also Disadvantaged
Among Their Peers
To analyze which social and structural factors are decisive for the size of the circle
of friends in more detail, we summarized the responses into three categories and
computed a logistic regression. Zero to three friends represented a small circle; four
to nine, a normal or middle-sized circle; and 10 or more, a large circle of friends.
Then we compared the middle group named by 40–50% of the children in each age
group with the other two groups.
As Table 6.2 shows, children from the lowest social class significantly more fre-
quently have a small or no circle of friends and less frequently a large circle of
friends than middle-class children. Members of the lower middle class also tend to
have a smaller circle of friends, whereas upper class children cultivate markedly
more friendships. Whether the child is a native German or has a migration back-
ground proves to have no significant effect on the size of the circle of friends.
However, the way that leisure time is organized plays a decisive role. Children who
belong to no clubs or leisure-time groups name significantly fewer friends than
children who are organized into at least one group. Regularly meeting up in clubs
and leisure-time groups gives children an opportunity to build up stable contacts
outside of school as well. Moreover, the shared interests in the group activities
already provide a basis for possible friendships. Hence, members of the lower social
classes simultaneously have a less varied leisure time and a particularly small circle
of friends. Instead of being able to compensate for the constraints on leisure activi-
ties through numerous positive social contacts, these children also have to come to
terms with constraints on their friendships. In contrast, however, the type of school
has no influence on the size of the circle of friends. Children attending all-day
schools do not have more contacts with friends through the longer daily interactions
140 A. Jänsch and M. Pupeter
Table 6.2 Significant relations between size of friend circles and personal and social variablesa
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
No/Small circle of Medium-sized circle of Large circle of
% (per cell) friends friends friends
All children 15 51 35
Age
6–7 years 19 57 24b
8–9 years 15 51 34
10–11 years 12 45 44b
Social origin
Lower class 30b 53 17b
Lower middle class 20b 51 29
Middle class 14 54 32
Upper middle class 11 51 38
Upper class 8 45 47b
Club membership
Yes 10 51 39
No 28b 52 20b
Parental care and attention
No deficit 13 50 37
Deficit in one parent 21 54 25b
Care deficit 19 55 26b
a
The relationship was tested with a multivariate nominal regression on the variable “size of circle
of friends
Variables included: age, social origin, club membership, migration background, type of school
(half-day/all-day), and care and attention deficit
b
For ease of presentation, bivariate values are presented as percentages in the table. Notable devia-
tions for variables that were also significant within the multivariate statistical analysis are printed
in bold (p < .05)
at school than children attending half-day schools who possibly spend more time
alone at home. Time spent together at school or in afternoon daycare is accordingly
not enough to form friendships. Hence, further bonding elements such as shared
hobbies are just as relevant. For children who receive insufficient care and attention
from one or both parents, friendships seem to be a particularly important social
anchor and to some extent a possible way of compensating for the lack of parental
attention and appreciation.1 However, it is particularly in this group that a large
circle of friends is most unusual. Children who complain about not receiving enough
attention from either one or two parents significantly less frequently report having
10 friends or more than children who do not lack attention from their parents.
1
“Having friends is more important for children whose family relationships are less satisfying,
supportive, and positive than it is for children in higher-functioning families, and families contrib-
ute more heavily to children’s adjustment when they do not have close friends” (Gauze et al. 1996,
cited in Gifford-Smith and Brownell 2002).
6 Friendships Among Peers 141
Children who report meeting their friends very often during their leisure time
have a larger circle of friends than children who only sometimes or hardly ever meet
their peers. Whereas 41% of the former report having a large circle of friends with
10 or more peers, this is only the case for 28% (several times a week) or 19%
(hardly ever) of the remaining children. We assume that a bidirectional effect is to
be found here, because those who have a larger number of friends will generally
also meet up with these more often in their free time, whereas, at the same time,
regular contacts are necessary to maintain and strengthen social relationships.
We took those children who reported having at least one friend when questioned
about the size of their circle of friends, and we asked them how many really close
friends they have in order to determine the size of the core peer group. Roughly one-
quarter report having two and a further one-quarter having three really close friends,
and 10–15% report having either only one or four to five very close friends. This
leaves 8% who report having six to nine very close friends and 4% who have 10 or
more. As to be expected, a correlation analysis2 reveals a significantly positive rela-
tion between the size of the circle of friends in total and the number of very close
friends. This correlation is slightly lower for children in the youngest group than for
the 8-to 9-year-olds and the 10- to 11- year-olds. This indicates that younger chil-
dren still do not have such a differentiated concept of social relations at their dis-
posal and are less able to distinguish between friends and acquaintances.
When we ask the children whether they find it easy or difficult to make new
friends, the majority (66%) report having no difficulty; 27% find it more difficult;
and 7% are not certain—with the largest proportion of “uncertains” in the group of
6- to 7-year-olds (9%). This may be because social contacts are still organized and
determined more strongly by their parents as pointed out at the beginning of this
chapter, and they may still have little experience in making friends by themselves.
On the whole, children find it increasingly easy to make friends the older they
become. At 6–7 years, 63% report that they find it easy to forge new contacts; in the
group of 8-to 9-year-olds, this already rises to 66%; and in the 10- to 11-year-olds,
it is 69%.
Not very surprisingly, children who find it easy to forge social contacts generally
also have a larger circle of friends than children who find it hard to make friends.
Forty percent of those who report no difficulties have 10 or more friends, whereas
only 26% of those who find it more difficult to make friends have a comparably
large circle.
However, social competencies do not just influence the forging of all friendships,
however superficial these may be. We can also see an effect on the number of very
close ones. Whereas approximately one-half of the children who report that they
find it more difficult to forge friendships, have one to two very close friends, only
one-third of those who find it easy to make friends have so few close friends. Vice
versa, only 8% of children who find it more difficult to make friends have six or
more very close friends compared to 15% of those who find it easy. Nonetheless,
when interpreting such findings, it has to be noted that extraverted children, who
approach others more easily, may have a different understanding of what really
good friends are than introverted children: The former may more quickly consider
another child to be a good friend, whereas the latter may be more restrained in their
appraisals.
6.2.1 T
he Traditional Meeting Places: School, the Home
Neighborhood, and Clubs
A further question that links up closely with how children forge their relations to
peers and how they cultivate them is naturally where they meet their friends. Fig. 6.4
provides an overview of the locations involved.
The absolute frontrunner among regular opportunities for contact is the school.
A total of 78% of children see their friends here just about every day and 17% at
least several times a week. This holds for nearly all types of school. The proportion
is only slightly lower at 69% among children attending a special needs school.
Because schools for children with special needs are frequently not located where
children live and they have to travel some distance, this may result in a separation
between the school and the rest of daily life— including existing friendships.
Students attending basic secondary school (Hauptschule) in contrast particularly
frequently (86%) report meeting their friends every day at school. Hence, schools
are not just important locations of learning and development in the sense of aca-
demic education; they make a fundamental contribution to making and keeping
friends. The relevance of the school as a meeting place also continues into second-
ary school. This indicates that when children are confronted with new situations—
following the transfer to secondary school—they react flexibly and make new
friends, as is also revealed in the larger circles of friends they report.
A further important meeting place for many children is their own home and the
homes of their friends. About 60% meet their friends at home either almost daily or
several times a week, and an equally high proportion report being guests at friends’
homes almost daily or several times a week.
Closely linked to the own home is the immediate neighborhood with its sur-
rounding streets, parks, and playgrounds. Seventeen percent of the children meet
their peer group here almost every day and a further 50% see their friends here
several times a week. We can find no mentionable trends indicating a shift in meet-
ing friends from the parental home to public spaces with increasing age. In all three
6 Friendships Among Peers 143
School 78 17 4
Midday or after-
school care
62 25 12
Outside 17 50 32
Your home 6 55 38
Friend’s home 4 56 39
At a club
2 41 54
Online 6 14 79
age groups, children almost equally frequently use the own home, that of their
friends, or the neighborhood streets and playgrounds for their meetings.
The neighborhood in which children live can facilitate or impede contacts with
friends, and this can influence the frequency of meeting them. In 2007 and 2013, we
asked the children whether there are enough friends to play with living in their
neighborhood. In 2013, the neighborhood seems to be somewhat less suitable for
meeting friends in leisure time than it was in the first Child Study. Whereas in the
current Child Study, 63% of 8- to 11-year-olds report having enough friends to play
with in their immediate neighborhood, in 2007, this was 66%. Hence, the children
see less opportunity to meet other children in their neighborhood than they did 6
years ago. Nonetheless, the difference is too slight to be able to assume that it indi-
cates any real trend.
As reported above, clubs assume an important function in the social life of 6- to
11-year-olds. This can also be seen in the meeting places they name. Forty-three
percent of the children report meeting their friends in clubs just about every day or
several times a week.
For children attending some kind of daycare center after school in the afternoon,
these institutions offer an almost equally important opportunity to see their friends
as the school itself. Nearly two-thirds of these children meet their friends just about
every day here; and one-quarter, at least several times a week.
144 A. Jänsch and M. Pupeter
Approximately one-quarter of the children with access to the Internet report being
very frequent users of Facebook or other social networks (see Sect. 5.4). As shown
in Fig. 6.4, every fifth child who has access to the Internet and is regularly online
reports interacting with friends there just about every day or several times a week.
Based on all children, the proportion who meet their friends online is 3% just about
every day and 6% at least several times a week. Answers to the question where
meetings with friends occur also reveal the age effect reported in Sect. 5.4 indicat-
ing that younger children hardly ever use social networks and chat lines, whereas
10- to 11-year-olds use them particularly widely. Children in the youngest group
hardly ever report the Internet as a possible place to meet, 10% of 8- to 9- year-old
Internet users meet their friends there, and almost 30% of 10- to 11-year-olds use
this option.
It remains to be seen how the use of social networks by children in this age range
develops, because this is measured for the first time in the 2013 Child Study.
However, we can already see a trend toward children meeting up personally with
their friends less often. Looking at the group of 8- to 11-year-olds surveyed since
2007, we can observe an almost drastic decline in the frequency of personal contacts
to friends during leisure time. In both the first and second Child Studies, 68%
reported meeting their friends very often. In 2013, there has been a clear drop to
53%.
This raises the question how far this trend should be viewed in relation to the
Internet and the shifting of social contacts to virtual space. Are today’s children
already interacting with their peers in chat lines and forums rather than meeting
them in reality? Is the digital exchange replacing direct contact with friends?
For the age group surveyed in the Child Study, the answer is “generally no.”
When we compare this with the reports of 8- to 11-year-olds since 2007 on how
frequently they use the Internet for chat lines, we can even see that these have
become less popular. In 2007, 47% of regular Internet users reported using chat
lines very often; in 2010, this was 30%; whereas in 2013, this is only 26%. This
decline certainly also has to be seen in the light of the growing importance of social
networks that are taking over from the classic chat line. Nonetheless, these data can-
not be used to conclude that children today prefer to cultivate their social contacts
on the Internet rather than in reality. Those who use Facebook very often also meet
their friends more often during their free time. A total of 59% of the frequent users
of social networks report meeting their friends very often. This contrasts with 55%
of those who hardly ever cultivate contacts over the Internet. The same applies to
those who use chat lines very often. They more frequently report meeting children
they are friends with (60%) compared to children with little experience of chat lines
(53%). Hence, the Internet seems to be more of a supplement that offers the children
further possibilities of keeping in touch with others in addition to personal contacts
rather than being something that competes with meeting friends in the real world.
6 Friendships Among Peers 145
6.3 F
airness, Being Appreciated, and Satisfaction
with the Circle of Friends
When children have friends, they want to feel good in their company, to be treated
fairly, and to be appreciated. Therefore, to gain a closer look at children’s friend-
ships, we also asked how fair they consider their circle of friends to be (see Chap. 2)
and whether their friends value their opinion, that is, the opinion of the child being
surveyed (Chap. 7).
A total of 36% of the children consider that the way they are treated in their clos-
est peer group is very fair and 54% that it is quite fair. Hence, the children have a
thoroughly positive outlook here. The circle of friends is an area of childhood expe-
rience that is generally perceived to be fair. This part of the social environment
offers the great advantage of being variable within certain limits so that each child
can contribute to shaping it alone. Children do not need to make friends with peers
whom they perceive to be unfair or dominant, and they can distance themselves
from friends when they do not like the way that they behave. The perception of fair-
ness also grows with increasing age and autonomy. Among 6- to 7-year-olds, 34%
see the interaction among their friends as being very fair and 51% as quite fair. In
the middle age group, this rises to 36% and 55%, whereas among the 10- to 11-year-
olds, 37% consider behavior to be very fair and 56% as quite fair.
When asked how far friends value their opinions, 51% are convinced that they
place “generally more” value on them. A further 35% consider that they are appreci-
ated at least to some extent here (“sometimes one, sometimes the other”). This
shows that the children’s perceptions continue to grow in a positive direction.
Whereas 43% reported “generally more” value in 2007, this was 42% in 2010 (45%
of 8- to 11-year-olds), whereas in 2013 it is now 51% who feel so appreciated (53%
of 8- to 11-year-olds). Here as well, we see an age effect in that older children more
frequently have the impression that their friends value their opinions. Whereas 47%
of 6- to 7-year-olds believe that their opinion is of generally more value, the figure
rises to 49% in the 8- to 9-year-olds and 57% in the oldest group.
Being appreciated and treating each other fairly are important preconditions for
satisfaction with social relations. According to the children’s reports, both are to be
found among the majority of the circles of friends in 6- to 11-year-olds. Are they
accordingly also satisfied with their closest, intimate peer group?
146 A. Jänsch and M. Pupeter
Table 6.3 Significant relations between satisfaction with circle of friends and personal and
social variablesa
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
% (per cell) Negative to neutral Positive Very positive
All children 6 32 62
Social origin
Lower class 11b 44b 45
Lower middle class 11 28 61
Middle class 7 28 65
Upper middle class 4 36b 60
Upper class 5 70
Gender
Girls 5b 33 62
Boys 8 30 62
Circle of friends
Small 11b 35 54
Medium 7 33 60
Large 4b 27b 69
Migration background
Yes 11b 30 59
No 8 32 60
This seems to be the case for most children with 62% rating their own satisfac-
tion very positively on the 5-point scale. Hence, as with the other appraisals of sat-
isfaction, the level here is very high in general. In the following, we shall take a look
at the factors influencing this satisfaction (Table 6.3).
The social class of origin impacts significantly on this aspect of childhood satis-
faction as well. Compared to middle-class children, lower class children signifi-
cantly more frequently rate their circle of friends as being negative to neutral (11%)
or positive (44%). At only 45% very positive ratings, they are well below the cor-
responding average of 62% in all the children surveyed. Hence, having a low social
status and the general constraints accompanying this status also influences the qual-
ity of social contacts with peers. Instead of peers serving a compensatory function
in which the support of other children offsets deficits in one’s own sphere, what we
can see here is the emergence of another area in which socially disadvantaged chil-
dren are worse off than their peers. This also has to be viewed together with the
aforementioned finding that children from the lower social classes have fewer
friends than their better situated peers. As Table 6.3 shows, the size of the circle of
friends also impacts on satisfaction. It is interesting to see that a particularly large
proportion of upper class children also rate their circle of friends only as “positive”
and not as “very positive.” It is not possible to derive any unequivocal reason for this
from the data. Gender also proves to have a significant effect on satisfaction with the
closest peer group. Girls less frequently rate this as being negative to neutral than
their male peers. In contrast, children with a migration background significantly
more frequently rate their circle of friends as being negative to neutral compared to
6 Friendships Among Peers 147
their native German peers. However, age and leisure type have no impact on the
children’s ratings.
References
Gauze, C., Bukowski, W. M., Aquan Assee, J., & Sippola, L. K. (1996). Interactions between fam-
ily environment and friendship and associations with self perceived well-being during adoles-
cence. Child Development, 67, 2201–2216.
Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2002). Childhood peer relationships: Social acceptance,
friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41, 235–284.
Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional development in a relational context;
Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. In Berndt, T. J. & Ladd, G. W.
(Hrsg.), Peer relationships in child development. Zitiert nach: Oerter, R., & Montada, L. (1998)
Entwicklungspsychologie. Ein Lehrbuch. Weinheim: Beltz.
Prazen, A., Wolfinger, N. H., Cahill, C., & Kowaleski-Jones, L. (2011). Joint physical custody and
neighborhood friendships in middle childhood. Sociological Inquiry, 81(2), 247–259.
Pupeter, M., & Schneekloth, U. (2010). Die Gleichaltrigen: Gemeinsame – getrennte Welten?. In
World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie
(S. 141 – 160). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Salvas, M.-C., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Lacourse, E., Boivin, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2011).
Interplay between friends’ aggression and friendship quality in the development of child
aggression during the early school years. Social Development, 20(4), 645–663.
Chapter 7
Participation and the Children’s Own
Opinion
Monika Pupeter and Ulrich Schneekloth
This chapter looks at how children think they are perceived in their own frame of
reference; that is, in their own society. In which parts of their lives can children
shape things themselves, how far do they consider themselves and their opinions to
be valued, and which limits to their freedom do they perceive?
Theoretically, we draw once again on the Capability Approach presented in
detail in the Second World Vision Child Study (Andresen et al. 2010). Basically, this
approach aims to describe the conditions for living a good life and to present these
coherently. An important benchmark is the question of justice. One key demand is
that individuals should be enabled to make their own decisions about how they wish
to shape their lives. This requires not only the necessary resources but also the indi-
vidual competencies that enable individuals to act in a self-determined way and to
do what they consider to be appropriate. As we also showed in the last Child Study,
the foundations for this are already laid in childhood. One central aspect is “having
one’s own opinion valued and taken seriously” in the way that children perceive this
in the areas of life that are relevant to them, that is, in their family, at school, in their
circle of friends, and during leisure time (Schneekloth and Pupeter 2010). In which
areas do children experience being taken seriously? When are they asked for their
opinion? Where are they allowed to participate in decision making? How do they
perceive freedoms? These are the questions that we asked the children directly. We
shall take a closer look at their answers here.
In the first section, we want to report on the children’s possibilities of participa-
tion in everyday life. Although we shall focus on how far they can exert an influence
in the home, we shall supplement this by referring to the findings on participation at
school presented in detail in Chap. 4. In the second section, we shall deal with how
children perceive the value placed on their own opinions; in other words, how far
they think that their opinion counts in everyday life. Finally, in the third section, we
shall look at how the children themselves judge the freedoms granted to them in
their everyday lives.
7.1 W
hich Aspects of Their Everyday Lives Can Children
Determine Themselves?
For all people, and therefore for children as well, it is important not only to have but
also to perceive that one has action scopes and possibilities of shaping one’s world.
Participating in decision making as an independent individual promotes expecta-
tions of self-efficacy in children and helps them to practice negotiation processes
and learn how to deal with differences in opinion. Being able to make one’s own
contribution to decision-making processes and to say what one thinks, that is, to be
involved in what is happening, is something we described in Chap. 2 when consid-
ering the desire for “procedural justice” or “interaction justice.” Children want to be
treated as equals and thereby to be treated fairly—and not just by adults but also in
comparison to adults. This is particularly the case when this concerns matters that
relate to them personally and on which they then demand recognition of their own
rights.
When aged 6–11 years, children are in middle childhood and continually expand-
ing their range of action as they mature. They are beginning to separate themselves
from the intimate family circle. Six-year-olds are still looked after closely by their
parents, but they already go to school, even when many of them are still accompa-
nied by adults on their way there. Older children, in contrast, travel to school by
themselves, sometimes even by bus and by train, and they not only may but also
increasingly have to deal with many things by themselves. The image of the way to
school provides a fitting illustration of the process of separation from the parental
home. Ideally, the growth in the demands placed on children is accompanied by an
increasing trust in their own abilities to cope with what is new in their world.
7.1.1 P
articipation and Making Their Own Decisions
in Everyday Life: In the First Instance, a Question
of Age
what they do. It was important for us to address things in everyday lives that should
affect as far as possible all children regardless of their individual dispositions, pref-
erences, or interests. Therefore, we selected the following eight aspects1:
Can you decide for yourself:
• Which friends do you meet?
• What clothes do you wear?
• What do you spend your pocket money on?
• What to eat at home?
• How many friends can you bring home with you?
• What do you do in your free time?
• When do you do your homework?
• What does your family do in its free time (e.g., at the weekend)?
A large majority of children can decide for themselves which friends they meet
(88%) and what they do in their free time (85%). Hence, even at home, more than
four-fifths of children see themselves as relevant “decision makers”. Likewise, the
majority report that they themselves can generally say what clothes they want to
wear (79%), that they codetermine what the family does in its free time (75%), and
that they themselves also generally decide what to spend their pocket money on
(72%). Only roughly every second child, and thus a much smaller proportion,
reports that they can decide what is to eat at home (53%) or also how many friends
they can bring home with them (48%). No more than 35% of the children can gener-
ally decide when they want to do their homework (Fig. 7.1).
As to be expected, relevant differences depend on the children’s age (Table 7.1).
The largest—relatively speaking—possibilities of codetermination are found in the
10- to 11-year-olds. Almost every child (more than 90%) in this age group can
determine what to do in her free time and can largely decide what to wear. Eighty
percent can decide what to do with their pocket money. Interestingly, at 81%, sig-
nificantly more children in this age group report possibilities of codetermining what
the family does in its free time. Significantly more older children report, though on
a lower level at 54%, that they can generally decide for themselves how many chil-
dren they bring home with them, and 46% can decide for themselves when to do
their homework.
Results are similar though on a somewhat lower level for the 8- to 9-year-olds.
They also report more possibilities of codetermination than the 6- to 7-year-olds.
This holds true for the circle of friends and for their own leisure-time choices.
Although there are slight differences between 8- to 9-year-olds and 10- to 11- year-
olds here, they do not attain statistical significance. The same applies to what they
do with their pocket money. Here as well, the differences between 8- to 9-year-olds
1
Specifically, we asked children: “If you think about your everyday life, which are the situations in
which your opinion counts. Can you decide for yourself: . . . which friends you meet? Generally
yes, Generally no, Don’t know/No answer.” Compared to 2010, four more domains were included,
so that the question in 2013 contained seven items. A further question on possibilities of codeter-
mining family activities during leisure time was added to the 2010 and 2013 surveys.
152 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
When to do my homework 35
Fig. 7.1 Where can children make their own decisions in everyday life
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
Table 7.1 Which aspects of everyday life can children determine for themselves?
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
Generally yes, columns in % Girls Boys 6–7 years 8–9 years 10–11 years
I can decide for myself or help decide on
Which friends to meet 88 87 81a 89 92
What to do in my free time 85 85 77a 86 91
What clothes to wear 82 75a 63a 79a 90a
What to do as a family on a free time 77 72a 66a 75a 81a
What to spend my pocket money on 74 70 60a 73 80
What to have to eat at home 54 53 51 53 55
How many friends can I bring home with me 47 48 46 43 54a
When to do my homework 36 34 25 31 46a
a
Significant differences between means (p > .05) are printed in bold
and 10- to 11- year-olds fail to attain significance. At 79%, significantly more 8- to
9-year-olds can decide for themselves what they want to wear than 6- to 7-year-olds,
but significantly fewer 8- to 9-year-olds can decide this compared to 10- to 11- year-
olds. The same applies for codetermining what the family does in its free time.
Here, 75% of 8- to 9-year-olds can have their say—significantly more than among
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 153
the 6- to 7-year-olds, but significantly less than among the 10- to 11- year-olds.
However, there are no significant differences compared to the 6- to 7-year-olds
regarding whether they can decide for themselves how many friends they bring
home with them (43%) or when they do their homework (31%).
In comparison, 6- to 7-year-olds markedly less frequently report being able to
decide for themselves. The majority at this age can determine whom they are friends
with and what they do in their free time. However, only two out of three reported
that they can decide what clothes they want to wear or codetermine what the family
does in its free time. Sixty percent, and thereby significantly fewer compared to the
other age groups, report being able to do largely what they want with their pocket
money. However, as reported above, they do not differ from the 8- to 9-year-olds in
terms of being able to decide how many friends they bring home with them and
when they do their homework.
No significant age effects can be found regarding whether they can also decide
for themselves what is to eat at home. Participation in decision-making over what is
cooked at home seems to be less a question of the children’s age in the sense of a
continuous increase in autonomy, but primarily a question of what is the customary
practice in a particular family and how they organize themselves. Family framing
conditions in the form of the available domestic resources are naturally effective
elsewhere as well, for example, regarding how many friends they can bring home
with them, what the family does in its leisure time, or whether they can decide what
to spend their pocket money on. Nonetheless, it is interesting to see where the
child’s age actually is and is not a major determining factor.
Comparing genders on the possibilities of participation and codetermination that
children report in their everyday lives reveals only two significant differences: At
82%, girls report significantly more frequently being able to determine what clothes
they wear compared to 75% of boys. In addition, girls feel more integrated into plan-
ning family activities at 77% compared to 72% of boys. Otherwise, the possibilities
of codetermination in everyday life are fairly comparable in both genders. Regarding
clothing, it can be assumed that “external appearances” continue to play a greater
role for girls due to their socialization, and the importance of their self-determination
in this domain will be encouraged more strongly. Regarding the codetermination of
family leisure-time activities, no such clear explanation suggests itself.
Results do not reveal any clear trend over time. The replies to the questions on
the choice of clothing and on pocket money reveal no significant differences com-
pared to the last Child Study. For the item on the number of friends children can
bring home with them, 48% report that they can decide this in 2013 compared to
42% in 2010. Currently, 75% report that they can codetermine what the family does
in its leisure time compared to 80% in the last Child Study.
154 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
7.1.2 L
ower Class Children Have the Lowest Possibilities
of Self-Determination
In contrast, there is once again a clear effect of social origin. Children from the
lower class have the least decision-making scope in everyday life whereas children
from the upper class in contrast have the most (Table 7.2).
The largest difference is found on the item addressing participation in planning
family leisure time. Only 49% of lower class children consider that they can code-
termine this compared to 72% from the lower middle class, 77% from the middle
class, 79% from the upper middle class, and 81% from the upper class. Certainly, as
with the item on pocket money, a lack of possibilities of codetermination will be
particularly marked among lower class children when they do not even have the
“basis” for them; in other words, when there is a general lack of family leisure-time
activities and no pocket money.2 Things are probably similar for the items on how
many children they can bring home with them or whether they also decide on what
the family eats. Lack of space and economic constraints on housekeeping certainly
regulate the facts here.
It is symptomatic that at 92%, nearly all upper class children can decide for
themselves how they spend their leisure time compared to only 69% of lower class
children. Lower middle-class children also differ from upper class children at 82%,
whereas the differences compared to the middle class (86%) and the upper middle
class (87%) are not significant. At 76% in the lower and 83% in the lower middle,
significantly fewer children in these classes are able to choose which friends they
meet compared to the upper middle and upper class children. This notable finding is
2
Results of the First World Vision Child Study indicated that lower class children less frequently
get pocket money (Schneekloth and Leven 2007).
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 155
probably due predominantly to parents in the lower social classes seeing far higher
risks in their children’s circle of friends than parents in higher social milieus
(Andresen and Galic 2015).
No relevant social class effects can be ascertained for the item on whether chil-
dren can decide for themselves what clothes they will wear. Possible parental guide-
lines seem to be irrelevant here for children between 6 and 11 years. At 43%, upper
class children most frequently perceive that they can also codetermine when to do
their homework. Members of all other social classes, and therefore also explicitly
the lower class, significantly less frequently report participation in decision making
on this issue (between 32% and 38%).
7.1.3 P
ossibilities of Self-Determination in Everyday Life:
A Question Not Only of Age But Also of Social Origin
and Family Practices
3
The institutional space, in this case, the decisive “school living space” with its own binding rules
and framing conditions, is excluded here. This is addressed separately in our study with special
questions on codetermination and participation from the children’s perspective (see Chap. 4).
4
The computed index can take a range of values from 0 (codetermination in no domain) to 8 (code-
termination in all eight domains). Referring to the mean and the standard deviation, we could dis-
tinguish three groups of children: those with 4–6 namings (mean plus or minus 1 standard
deviation), 0–3 namings (lower than 1 standard deviation), and 7–8 namings (higher than 1 stan-
dard deviation).
156 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
6 domains 25
Average possibilities of self-
determination
5 domains 20
58%
4 domains 13
3 domains 9
Few possibilities of self-
determination
2 domains 4 15%
1 domain 2
all, is yet again when they do their homework. However, more than two out of three
children in this group can codetermine this.
Finally, 15% of the children are in the group with “few possibilities of self-
determination” in everyday life. Here, only just above one out of two children can
determine which friends they meet, whereas in all other domains, including what
they do in their leisure time, the majority report a lack of self-determination.
As reported above, the possibilities of self-determination increase as children
grow older. Nonetheless, the different scopes cannot be explained by age alone. We
also find variations within age groups. This becomes visible when we study which
children more frequently, that is, with a greater probability, belong to one of our
three groups. Analyzing the statistical relations between these features5 confirms the
effects of not only age but also other social and familial background factors
(Table 7.3).
As can be expected, younger children more frequently (and older children, in
contrast, less frequently) belong to the group with few possibilities of
self-determination in everyday life. The older the children are, the more frequently
they report being generally able to determine their everyday lives themselves.
Whereas 27% of 6- to 7-year-olds report having only a few of the decision-making
5
We tested relationships with both a multifactorial analysis of variance across our specially formed
total index and an ordinal regression across the three levels “possibilities of codetermination in
daily life: few, more frequent, consistent.” Both approaches produced comparable findings.
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 157
Table 7.3 Significant relations between possibilities of self-determination in everyday life and
personal and social variablesa
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
Per cell in % Few Average Just about all
All children 15 58 27
Age
6–7 years 27 55b 18b
8–9 years 13 64b 23b
10–11 years 8 54b 38b
Social origin
Lower class 37 50b 13b
Lower middle class 19 56 25
Middle class 12 63b 25
Upper middle class 14 57 29
Upper class 8 56b 36b
Migration background
Native German children 11 59 30b
Children with a migration background 24 55b 21b
Type of family
One-child family 15 55 30
Two-child family 15 55 30
Three-or-more-child family 17 64b 19b
Single parent 14 60 26
Parental care and attention
No deficit 14 57 29
Deficit in one parent 19 61 20
Care deficit 21 60 19b
Gender
Girls 15 55 30b
Boys 15 61b 24
a
The relationship was tested with a multivariate ordinal regression on the variable “possibilities of
self-determination in everyday life”
Variables included: Age, gender, social origin, migration background, type of family, care deficit,
and east vs. West Germany
b
For ease of presentation, bivariate values are presented as percentages in the table. Notable devia-
tions for variables that were also significant within the multivariate statistical analysis are printed
in bold (p < .05)
scopes we surveyed in their everyday lives, this is the case for only 8% of 10- to
11-year-olds. On the other hand, there are also 55% of 6- to 7-year-olds and 54% of
10- to 11-year-olds who report more frequent possibilities of self-determination,
and 18% of 6- to 7-year-olds as well as 38% of 10- to 11-year-olds who report just
about all possibilities of self-determination in everyday life.
We also find a slight gender effect: At 30%, girls belong slightly more frequently
than boys (at 24%) to the group with just about all possibilities of self-determination.
As mentioned above, this difference can be ascertained in only two of the eight
158 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
domains we survey. In these domains, we cannot rule out a possible effect of differ-
ent perceptions. If we were to look at other aspects of everyday life as well, this
difference might well level out. On the other hand, there are no indications that boys
in this age range feel that they are granted more autonomy in everyday life and girls
correspondingly less.
Independent of age, statistics once again reveal the effect of social origins: the
lower the social class, the higher the proportion of children with only few possibili-
ties of self-determination in their everyday lives. Lower class children at 37% and
lower middle-class children at 19% disproportionately frequently report having few
possibilities of self-determination. In contrast, 36% of upper class children dispro-
portionately frequently report having possibilities of codetermination in just about
all domains. On the one hand, the low possibilities of self-determination are, as
mentioned above, due to the less favorable framing conditions typical for lower
class children. If there is little space at home, then it is naturally more difficult to
bring an unlimited number of friends home with you. If money is limited, then free-
doms and the regulation of leisure time differ from those experienced by children in
the higher social classes (see also Sect. 3.4 on how children experience poverty). On
the other hand, different family practices and childrearing styles also play a role
here.
Another independent factor that is not mediated by, for example, class member-
ship is the effect of a migration background. At 30%, native German children mark-
edly more frequently have possibilities of codetermination in just about all domains
compared to 24% of children with a migration background. This result is in line
with findings on different childrearing styles in native German families and families
with a migration background. Not only families with roots in Islamic cultures but
also Spätaussiedler (ethnic German immigrants from the countries of the former
Soviet Union) particularly reveal a higher frequency of traditional and authoritarian
childrearing styles. Especially in children in this age range, these are often com-
bined with the demand to comply more strongly with the cultural traditions of the
country of origin (Fuhrer and Uslucan 2005; Uslucan 2008).
Children with deficits in care and attention (“My mother and father do not give
me enough of their time”) are also more likely to have low possibilities of self-
determination independent of age and social origins. They are also less likely to
have possibilities of self-determination in just about all domains. It can be seen here
that children who complain about a lack of parental attention are disadvantaged in
many ways. Parents whose children feel that they do not look after them enough
also grant their children fewer possibilities of self-determination and codetermina-
tion. This can be viewed as a restriction of the potential opportunities in these chil-
dren’s everyday lives.
Finally, there is an interesting relationship with the type of family. Independent
from the aforementioned factors, children from families with three or more children
report having somewhat fewer possibilities of self-determination in their everyday
lives than children in families with only one or no siblings. Combining this with the
finding in Chap. 2 that children from families with three or more children also com-
plain somewhat more frequently about being disadvantaged in their everyday lives,
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 159
there are many indications at this stage that this is not due primarily to the parental
childrearing style. It is at least just as much due to the constraints imposed on
families with lots of children by the lack of support for large families in everyday
life in Germany.
7.1.4 C
hildren with Low Possibilities of Self-Determination
in Everyday Life Also Experience Less Codetermination
at School
6
Means would have been higher and the correlation stronger if we had added together the “often”
and “sometimes” responses and thereby weighted possibilities of codetermination at school
differently.
160 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
6-7 years
“In our school, we are often allowed to help decide…“ 8-9 years
10-11 years
Number of domain sat school in which children
often are able to help decide
Mean from 0 to 7 (*significant at p < .05) *
2.2
*
1.7
1.2 1.3 1.2*
0.9 * 0.8
*
0.6 0.7
Fig. 7.3 Children with greater possibilities of self-determination in everyday life also experience
a bigger decision-making scope at school
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years
life and 1.7 (thereby not significantly more) among children with possibilities of
self-determination in just about all fields of everyday life. Among 6- to 7-year-olds,
in contrast, with less than one reported area of codetermination at school, all differ-
ences between groups fail to attain significance.
The finding that children who have more possibilities of self-determination in
everyday life also report more opportunities to codetermine at school is another
decisive effect of social origins. As pointed out above, upper class children have
more possibilities of self-determination not only in everyday life but also when they
attend a Gymnasium for their secondary education. Both in the family and at school,
they generally experience more possibilities of codetermination and they gain more
decision-making competencies. Being able to choose and decide for oneself is an
aspect of everyday life that these children take for granted, unlike children from the
lower classes who markedly more frequently have no access to these opportunities
for developing social competencies. In the last Child Study, we examined this aspect
in more detail from the perspective of perceived self-efficacy (Schneekloth and
Pupeter 2010). It is interesting to see this relationship emerge here as well.
It is also interesting to see that this correlation is already found in elementary
school children. Even though somewhat weaker, it can still be confirmed statisti-
cally. We attribute this to what we call the school district principle. In Germany,
elementary school children are assigned to specific elementary schools in their resi-
dential areas. Elementary schools in “posh” areas accordingly offer more possibili-
ties of codetermination than schools in less posh areas. Viewing possibilities of
codetermination as a feature of educational quality, children with a lower socioeco-
nomic family background have less access to high-quality institutions in this sphere
as well. This has also been noted critically in the German Parliament’s 14th Report
on Children and Youth (Deutscher Bundestag 2013). In addition, personality char-
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 161
Our study also asked children whether they feel that their opinions are taken seri-
ously: “Which of the following do you think care more about what you think and
which care less?” (Table 7.4).
Six- to 11-year-olds most frequently consider that their own opinions are taken
generally more seriously by their mothers (60%). Here, 27% report “sometimes
one, sometimes the other”; 9% are explicitly negative with “generally less”; and 4%
do not reply. Just under one-half feel they are taken seriously by their fathers (49%
“generally more,” 27% “sometimes one, sometimes the other,” 13% “generally
less,” 11% no reply), and results are similar for the circle of friends (51% “generally
more,” 35% “sometimes one, sometimes the other”, 11% “generally less”, and 3%
no reply).
Ratings of class teachers and, when applicable, of caregivers in institutions (day-
care centers, etc.) are lower. Not even one-third of children report that their class
teacher respects their opinion quite a lot (29% “generally more”, 33% “sometimes
one, sometimes the other”, 30% “generally less”, 8% no reply), and, likewise, only
one-third give positive reports on the persons providing them with institutional care
in the afternoons (33% “generally more”, 35% “sometimes one, sometimes the
other”, 25% “generally less”, 7% no reply).
Generally, children’s evaluations of how well their opinion is valued increase as
they grow older. Among the 10- to 11-year-olds, 65% report that their mothers and
52% that their fathers place generally more value on their personal opinion. Among
the 8- to 9-year-olds, this applies to 59% and 49% respectively; and among the 6- to
7-year-olds, 56% and 46%. In contrast, 7% of 10- to 11-year-olds report generally
less regard from their mothers and 12% (according to Table 7.4) generally less from
their fathers. Among the 8- to 9-year-olds, this applies to 8% and 12% respectively;
and among the 6- to 7-year-olds, 12% and 16%. Results are similar for how well
their opinion is valued by their circle of friends and by their class teachers and day-
care staff.
In contrast, differences in the ratings of boys and girls are less homogeneous. For
example, girls somewhat more frequently give no reply when estimating the regard
of their fathers whereas boy tend to rate their fathers more positively. The “some-
times one, sometimes the other” and negative answers reveal no differences. In con-
trast, girls rate the regard of their class teacher more positively than boys (32% vs.
25%). Correspondingly fewer girls give negative ratings than boys (28% vs. 33%).
162 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
Findings seem to be similar for institutional daycare staff. Girls feel better regarded
by them than boys (36% vs. 30%). On the other hand, more girls than boys give
explicitly negative ratings here (26% vs. 23%).
Accordingly, the children are quite capable of taking a “critical” perspective
when rating how seriously their opinion is taken. As far as the family and circle of
friends are concerned, however, at 10%, only a smaller portion give explicitly nega-
tive ratings. The “sometimes one, sometimes the other” ratings that one-quarter
report regarding the regard of their mothers nonetheless emphasize how sensitive
children are in this age range. In comparison, answers on satisfaction with life or
ratings of parental care are far more positive, but perhaps these questions are also
answered without so much thought. More than 90% rate the care they receive from
their parents either positively or very positively, and only a minority give neutral or
even negative ratings (see Chaps. 2 und 3).
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 163
56
Mother 57
60
47
Father 48
49
43
Friends 42
51
26
Class teacher 32
29
2007
24 2010
(If child attends after-school care)
36 2013
The staff where I go after school
33
7.2.1 T
he Trend: Slightly More Regard in the Family
and Circle of Friends; Ups and Downs at School
and in Institutions
Viewed as a trend, the regard that children experience in the family and the circle of
friends has increased slightly since our first Child Study in 2007 (Fig. 7.4). At this
point, we are referring to the explicitly positive ratings (“generally more”). For
mothers, the positive assessment has increased in the last two Child Studies from
56% in 2007 over 57% in 2010 to 60% in 2013. For fathers, it has risen from 47%,
across 48%, to a current 49%.7 The explicitly negative ratings fluctuate for the
mothers but remain below the 10% level. In the fathers, they were 13% in 2007, 9%
in 2010, and therefore slightly below the latest value of 13%. When rating the circle
of friends, positive ratings have moved from 43%, across 42%, to a current 51% and
thereby show the strongest increase. Negative ratings, in contrast, have remained
constant at round about 10%.
In contrast, there has been a slight decline compared to the 2010 Child Study in
ratings of the regard in which they are held by teachers and daycare staff. From
2007 to 2010, positive ratings rose from 26% to 32% for class teachers and from
24% to 36% for daycare staff. Currently, 29% of children give explicitly positive
ratings to the esteem in which they are held by class teachers and 33% by daycare
7
Note that the increase in reported positive regard from fathers was not large enough to attain sta-
tistical significance.
164 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
staff, when appropriate. Negative ratings for class teachers have changed from 23%
in the first Child Study in 2007, across 19% in 2010, to 30% in 2013. In the daycare
institutes, explicitly negative ratings have changed from 25% in 2007, across 17%
in 2010, to 25% in 2013.
Regarding parents and the circle of friends, there is a clear trend toward children
perceiving that their own opinion is being taken more seriously. However, regarding
teachers and daycare staff, there is no recognizably stable trend, with findings fluc-
tuating across the three waves of assessment.
Viewed in context, the degree of respect for their own opinion that children
report perceiving in their mothers and fathers is determined decisively by how much
time mothers or fathers dedicate to them. If children consider that their mothers
spend enough time with them, then an above-average 67% give explicitly positive
ratings on how well their mothers respect their opinion. If fathers devote enough
time, then the proportion of children who give explicitly positive ratings on how
well their fathers respect their opinion even reaches 68% (compared to an average
of 49%).
More negative ratings of how seriously they are taken by their fathers or mothers
are once again found more frequently among children from the lower classes.
Sixteen percent of lower class children rate the regard of their mothers and even
31% that of their fathers as being explicitly negative. A notable finding is that 19%
of children with a migration background compared to 10% of native German
children rate the regard of their fathers negatively. There are no significant differ-
ences in ratings on the regard of their mothers. The differences in the regard of
mothers versus fathers on the one hand and children with versus without a migration
background on the other reveal the significance of parental childrearing styles. The
children’s statements can be evaluated as support for an appraisal that continues to
dominate research on childrearing styles to this day: Children from less educated or
socially more deprived classes tend to be reared in an authoritarian manner and this
leads to differences in the regard paid to their opinion. The ratings of children with
a migration background can be related to the dominance of a “traditional father
role” in their families. For the rating on the regard given to one’s own opinion by
class teachers, it is the age of the child and the type of school that play a central role.
Children attending a Gymnasium or also a Realschule somewhat more frequently
consider that the class teacher takes their opinions seriously. The aforementioned
gender effect with a slightly higher rating of the class teacher’s regard for girls’
opinions is also significant. This finding also indicates that it is not just social fac-
tors that play an important role but the individual character as well. How regard is
perceived also depends on the way teachers as individuals impart the educational
goals of the school.
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 165
Do politicians take the well-being of children into account when making their deci-
sions? Do children feel that politicians take them seriously? We posed these ques-
tions only to children who showed some understanding of the term “politics.” We
first asked whether they had any interest at all in politics: “Are you interested in
politics?” If necessary, we also explained “What I mean is what politicians (such as
Frau Merkel) or what political parties do.”
In the present Child Study, 30% did not answer this question (53% of 6- to
7-year-olds, 31% of 8- to 9-year-olds, and 11% of 10- to 11-year-olds). In 2010, this
was 19% (30% of 6- to 7-year-olds, 31% of 8- to 9-year-olds, and 6% of 10- to
11-year-olds). However, we attribute this increase predominantly to method effects.
In contrast to 2010, in 2013, we had already presented a series of questions on poli-
tics and social justice. The children who were unable to answer these questions (see
Chap. 2) also more frequently gave no answer here either. In 2010 in contrast, 47%
replied “not at all interested”—markedly more than the 37% in the 2013 Child
Study (Fig. 7.5).
Otherwise, there are hardly any changes. Twenty-two percent characterize them-
selves as not very interested (compared to 24% in the 2010 Child Study); 9%, as
interested (compared to 8%); and 2%, as very interested (same in 2010). For older
children aged 8 to 11 years, we also have findings from our first Child Study in
2007. Here, 9% of 8- to 9-year-olds were interested (or even very interested) in poli-
tics. In 2010, this was 10%; and in 2013, it is also 10%. Among the 10- to
11-year-olds, the numbers were 11% in 2007, 15% in 2010, and 14% in 2013.
Generally speaking, there are no relevant changes apart from the correction of the
unusually low result among the 10- to 11-year-olds in 2007.
The majority of children still describe themselves as not being interested in poli-
tics. However, when we ask them to give concrete evaluations of, for example, what
they think about the way certain groups are treated in Germany, most give thor-
oughly differentiated appraisals and also demand to be included themselves when
the interests of children are at stake (see Chap. 2). This result is in line with findings
on political interests in adolescents (Schneekloth 2010). Here as well, it seems to be
a contemporary phenomenon that children pick up on and articulate here.
7.2.3 S
kepticism Regarding Whether Politicians Bother
About the Interests of Children
We asked the children who had at least some basic grasp of “politics” to rate how
they felt that politicians treat children: “Do you think that politicians also pay much
attention to children, that is, do they consider what they need to do so that children
will have a good life?”
166 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
2
Very interested
2
8
Interested
9
24
Not very interested
22
47
Not interested at all
37
At 35%, the largest group of children who were given this question reply some-
what skeptically. A positive appraisal of whether politicians look after the interests
of children is given by 29%, 22% are undecided, and the remaining 14% do not
agree with any of the available response alternatives.
The reported appraisals depend strongly on age, even when, as here, we analyze
only the responses of those who have some grasp of politics (Fig. 7.6). Among the
6- to 7-year-olds, a small relative majority of 29% answer positively; 25% rate the
consideration given to them by politicians more negatively, 20% are undecided, and
26% are unable to answer the question. The opinions of 8- to 9-year-olds are bal-
anced: 34% tend to think that politicians do not value the interests of children; 33%
think they do, 21% are undecided, and 12% cannot answer the question. Among the
10- to 11-year-olds in contrast, negative appraisals dominate. The majority of 41%
do not think that politicians spend much time thinking about what they should do
for children, 26% appraise them positively, 23% are undecided, and 10% can give
no answer. Hence skepticism about what politicians do increases with age.
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 167
29
I think so 33
26
20
Can’t decide, can’t say 21
23
25
I don’t really think so 34
41
6-7 years
26 8-9 years
Don’t know 12 10-11 years
10
Fig. 7.6 My opinion on whether politicians also pay much attention to what they need to do so
that we children can have a good life
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
When we ask the children quite generally about how they rate the freedoms their
parents grant them in their everyday lives, a large majority give positive or very
positive answers. At 83%, the positive rating has even increased compared to the
78% in the last Child Study in 2010 (Fig. 7.7).
This even more positive evaluation can be found in all age groups and underlines
how strongly children in Germany approve of the way their parents are rearing and
caring for them. Warmth, affection, and reliability, but also the willingness to dis-
pute with the child and if necessary set limits (authoritative childrearing style, see
Baumrind 1991) are appreciated by the majority of children and reflected in their
answers here. Discontent is found where children gain the impression that they are
not able to act in a sufficiently self-determined way in their everyday lives, or where
parents lack either the time or the ability to deal with them. This particularly fre-
quently affects 29% of lower class and 19% of lower middle-class children who
give neutral to negative reports on the freedoms granted to them by their parents. In
contrast, 12% of middle-class, 15% of upper middle-class, and 11% of upper class
children give neutral to negative reports on this.
The importance of social origins for both satisfaction and well-being as well as
for the opportunities to participate at school and during leisure time dominates the
life worlds of children throughout the 2013 Child Study. We can see that children
from the lower classes are disadvantaged. However, they are also disadvantaged
because their families are disadvantaged and have fewer opportunities and less
168 M. Pupeter and U. Schneekloth
40
Very positive
43
38
Positive
40
16
Neutral
14
3
Negative
1 2010
2013
2
Very negative
1
Fig. 7.7 Assessment of the amount of freedom granted by parents in daily life
Children in Germany aged 6–11 years (%)
potential to offer their children comparably good conditions and starting chances in
life. It is precisely these children who also need strong families, who need to be
socially well-embedded in their circles of friends, and who need to be respected in
all areas of life. Children and parents need the support of “the whole village” as we
have tried to portray it since our first Child Study. They do not need stigmatization
and exclusion. Instead it is necessary to promote initiatives to provide concrete sup-
port in everyday life that will effectively meet the primary goal of giving children
the capabilities they need in order to participate in society. The final Chap. 9 of the
Child Study will go into more detail on the political challenges this embodies.
References
Andresen, S., Hurrelmann, K., & Fegter, S. (2010). Wie geht es unseren Kindern? Wohlbefinden
und Lebensbedingungen der Kinder in Deutschland. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.)
Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 35 – 59). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Andresen, S., & Galic, D. (2015). Kinder. Armut. Familie. Alltagsbewältigung und Wege zu
wirksamer Unterstützung. Verlag Bertelsmann Stiftung, Gütersloh
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance
use. Journal of early adolescence. Band 11, Heft 1, S. 56 – 95.
Deutscher Bundestag. (2013). Der 14. Kinder- und Jugendbericht. Bericht über die Lebenssituation
junger Menschen und die Leistungen und Bestrebungen der Kinder- und Jugendhilfe Iin
Deutschland. Deutscher Bundestag, Drucksache 17 / 12200. http://dip21.bundestag.de/dip21/
btd/17 / 122 / 1712200.pdf (Stand: 24.06.2013).
7 Participation and the Children’s Own Opinion 169
Fuhrer, U., & Uslucan, H.-H. (Hrsg.). (2005). Familie, Akkulturation und Erziehung: Migration
zwischen Eigen- und Fremdkultur. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
Schneekloth, U. (2010). Jugend und Politik: Aktuelle Entwicklungstrends und Perspektiven. In
Shell Deutschland Holding. (Hrsg.), Jugend 2010. Eine pragmatische Generation behauptet
sich (16. Shell Jugendstudie). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer, S. 129 – 164.
Schneekloth, U., & Leven, I. (2007). Familie als Zentrum: nicht für alle gleich verlässlich. In
World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie
(S. 65 – 109). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Schneekloth, U., & Pupeter, M. (2010). Wohlbefinden, Wertschätzung, Selbstwirksamkeit:
Was Kinder für ein gutes Leben brauchen. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in
Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 187 – 221). Frankfurt a. M: Fischer.
Uslucan, H. H. (2008). Männlichkeitsbilder, Familie und Erziehung in den Communities von
Zuwanderern. Berliner Forum Gewaltprävention (BFG Nr. 34).
Chapter 8
Challenges Facing a “Policy for Children”
Now that we have presented and discussed the results of this, our Third World
Vision Child Study, we want to name the challenges facing a “policy for children”
that we, as the research team, derive from our findings. Here, we can link up with
the ideas presented in the corresponding chapter in the Second World Vision Child
Study, and some of these ideas will be repeated here. However, having introduced
the children’s idea of justice for the first time in the present World Vision Child
Study, we shall also ask how justice can serve as a benchmark for child and family
policy, and what steps have to be taken to implement it. In our first step, we shall
concentrate on more rights for children and present proposals regarding how to
enable children to participate in those areas of life that are relevant to them. Then
we shall present proposals for linking child policy together with policies for the
family, education, and the common good.
S. Andresen (*)
Department of Education, Institute of Social Pedagogy and Adult Education,
Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
K. Hurrelmann
Hertie School of Governance, Berlin, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
U. Schneekloth
Kantar Public, München, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
The United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child distinguishes the child’s
rights of protection, development, and participation. These rights all belong together
and should ensure that children can grow up into healthy adults. In Chap. 1, we
presented our justice theory approach showing how far justice depends on proce-
dures being transparent and open to the possibility of codetermination. Nonetheless,
such procedures have to be built on a legal basis that is valid and binding for all
persons. This is why the formulation and ratification of basic rights are such an
important step forward—precisely for those, such as children compared to adults,
who possess less power because of their elementary dependence on protection and
care.
8.1.1 S
trictly Applying the United Nations Convention
on the Rights of the Child
Germany ratified the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child back in
1992. The intervening years have seen discussions going far beyond Germany
regarding whether and, if yes, in which areas there is a need to act and introduce
reforms and which measures are needed to further support the rights of the child.
One important discussion in Germany addresses whether or not the rights of the
child should be formally entered into the national constitution. A frequent criticism
of this is that children’s rights would then have to be given primacy over parents’
rights and that the two cannot be reconciled. We do not share this concern. The
United Nations Convention also treats parents’ rights in a sensitive way. In German
law, parents’ rights focus on the interests and well-being of the child and therefore
expressly do not run counter to the child’s rights.
Regardless of one’s opinion on whether or not the rights of the child should be
laid down in the constitution, it is necessary to clarify exactly what is understood by
these rights, because, to some extent, we can see that different goals are being pur-
sued in different fields of action. Precisely in light of the interests revealed by the
children we surveyed, we consider this clarification to be the responsibility of politi-
cians. Working together not only with experts but also with children, politicians
need to clarify what exactly are the goals of the rights of the child, what duties they
impose on which groups, and which conceivable procedures are appropriate for dif-
ferent age groups.
Currently, one can see different accents. These have been analyzed recently in
the German parliament’s 14th Report on Children and Youth (Deutscher Bundestag
2013, p. 264): The rights of the child are associated with the recognition of the
legitimate interests of children and youths in light of their dependence on protection
and care. Hence, the need is to establish procedures that guarantee that even the
youngest children can make their voices heard and have their wishes taken into
8 Challenges Facing a “Policy for Children” 173
account. However, procedures designed to take the child’s wishes into account have
a somewhat different meaning within the family compared to public institutions.
Therefore, it is necessary to clarify exactly who is required to do what in order to
establish the rights of the child: be it the state, individual institutions, and/or the
family. Finally, it is necessary to reach agreement on who is responsible for the
rights of the child when the child herself is not in a position to stand up for them.
These few comments already show that particularly from a justice perspective, there
is a need to clarify how the rights of the child can be ensured and translated into
sensitive but effective procedures.
Just like its two predecessors, the Third World Vision Child Study reveals how
parental homes already make a major contribution to the success of participation. In
their own homes, children experience being held in high regard, having the way they
see things taken into consideration, and holding joint discussions on everyday
issues. This is shown by our results reported in Chap. 7: Children aged 6 to 11 years
decide for themselves on many aspects of everyday life such as what to spend their
pocket money on, what clothes they want to put on in the morning, and which
friends they have. At the same time, however, they have the backing of their par-
ents—who protect them but finally also supervise their decisions. This provides
children in many families with a protected free space in which they can develop
their self-esteem and self-efficacy—a free space in which they are also able to make
wrong decisions that can be corrected later.
This finding reveals how parents today make an important contribution to putting
the rights of the child into practice: They transform the family into a living open
discussion household. The willingness and ability of these parents should not be
misunderstood as “not having the courage to rear their children properly.” Involving
children and getting them to participate in making decisions on topics that they can
grasp does not mean giving in to them or avoiding conflicts. It is important to take
one’s own stance and justify it in order to take the child and the child’s own outlook
seriously. From the child’s perspective, things are fair when everybody has to stick
to the rules and receives equal treatment. What is needed is reliability and account-
ability rather than giving in as far as possible or ignoring problems.
Even when our study shows that a very large majority of children are very con-
tented with the way things are in their families, from the perspective of the rights of
the child, we must not forget that the family is not always a home in which people
treat each other with loving respect. In all social classes, there are families in which
the child’s right to physical and mental integrity and the right to participation and
respect have not been achieved. Recent years have seen far more public discussion
and acknowledgement of neglect and the sexual abuse of children than ever before.
For a long time, talking about sexual abuse was taboo—much to the detriment of the
children exposed to it. Even when the dark figure of undetected crime continues to
be high for these offences and we do not know precisely how widespread the prob-
lem is, one thing does seem to be clear: Most cases of sexual abuse of children take
place in the proximal environment, that is, within the family. When children are
faced with such abuse, they need to know that they can turn to attentive and respect-
ful adults in their daycare centers, schools, clubs, and neighborhood; and they need
to experience a climate in which they can have the courage to trust in others. A study
of the sexual abuse of girls and boys in institutions carried out by the German Youth
Institute (Deutsches Jugendinstitut e.V., DJI, 2011) has shown how frequently child
victims turn to educators they can trust and take them into their confidence. This
potential needs to be strengthened.
8 Challenges Facing a “Policy for Children” 175
8.1.4 E
xtending the Rights of the Child in Educational
Institutions
As pointed out above, the right to counseling is an important aspect of child partici-
pation wherever children are to be found. This means that those responsible in insti-
tutions, in daycare centers, in schools, boarding schools, children’s homes, or sports
clubs also need to think about which options and procedures they can establish to
grant children comprehensive participation rights. We still see a major need for
action here, because ever since our first Child Study in 2007, only about 30% of the
children we survey report feeling that their opinion is respected in school and in
daycare. This percentage has not changed over the years, and it is also relatively
independent of a child’s age. Whereas parents involve their children in decisions
more and more frequently as they grow older, there are only some signs of such an
approach among professionals.
From our perspective, this is a task to be tackled in professional training and
further training: Respect for what children think and how they think about things is
a basic precondition for participation and thus an important aspect of a professional
approach to working with children. Here as well, we consider it to be important to
emphasize that this attitude should not be derided as an easy way of avoiding con-
flict when dealing with children. Taking children seriously also means arguing with
them about which is the correct path for them to take. On this basis, individual
institutions can develop procedures in which the rights of the child are applied com-
prehensively. The Bundesarbeitsgemeinschaft der Landesjugendämter (2013)
[National Association of Länder youth welfare offices] has recently announced that
the establishment of the rights of the child is a quality feature of child daycare cen-
ters and a guideline for working with children. As well as being a feature of educa-
tional quality, it is something that institutions are obliged to implement by law.
Those responsible understand the rights of the child as a central strategy for imple-
menting child protection. This is based on the assumption of a close link between
active rights such as participation and the right of protection.
176 S. Andresen et al.
This call to take the dissatisfaction of children seriously, to teach them how to com-
plain, to encourage them to do so, and to view this as a task of education represents
an important step in establishing justice from the children’s perspective. It is pre-
cisely the nontransparent, at times seemingly unjust behavior of adults that offends
the child’s idea of justice. Providing a space for these feelings and helping children
to express them is an indispensable element of a progressive child policy. This has
been aided by an important legal reform since 2012, because two paragraphs (§ 8b
and 45) of Book Eight of the Social Code (SGB VIII) specify that institutions have
to provide complaint procedures through which children and youths can express
their personal concerns. Children can find themselves in situations in which they
have to refer to an independent complaints office or ombudsman. Here as well, it is
necessary to study how schools, in which all children spend a great deal of their
time, intend to put these provisions into practice and whether they also engage in
implementing children’s rights more strongly than before. In the context of the
school, this could certainly be linked to the theory of good teaching (Klieme et al.
2010), because this promotes three elements that are highly compatible with the
rights of the child: the greatest possible own initiative in children including their
cognitive activation, a good classroom climate, and a responsible and proactive
class management. In Chap. 4, we have shown how important the school is for chil-
dren’s daily lives. Therefore, it is precisely teachers whom we wish to encourage to
invest more energy than before in exploiting the potential of the school as a space in
which children can experience participation and strengthen their self-efficacy
(Hurrelmann and Timm 2011).
8 Challenges Facing a “Policy for Children” 177
References
AGJ – Arbeitsgemeinschaft für Kinder- und Jugendhilfe. (Hrsg.). (2012). Sozialgesetzbuch VIII
auf dem Stand des Bundeskinderschutzgesetzes. Berlin: Gesamttext und Begründungen.
Andresen, S., & Heitmeyer, W. (Hrsg.). (2012). Zerstörerische Vorgänge. Missachtung und sexuelle
Gewalt gegen Kinder und Jugendliche in Institutionen. Weinheim und Basel: Beltz Juventa.
Bundesarbeitsgemeinschaft der Landesjugendämter. (2013). Sicherung der Rechte von Kindern
als Qualitätsmerkmal von Kindertageseinrichtungen. http://www.bagljae.de/downloads/114_
sicherung-der-rechte-von-kindern-in-kitas.pdf (Stand: 24.06.2013).
Cremer, H. (2012). Kinderrechte und der Vorrang des Kindeswohls. Die UN-Kinderrechtskonvention
bietet ein weites Anwendungsfeld. Anwaltspraxis, 4, S. 327 – 329.
Deutscher Bundestag. (2013). Der 14. Kinder- und Jugendbericht. Bericht über die Lebenssituation
junger Menschen und die Leistungen und Bestrebungen der Kinder- und Jugendhilfe Iin
Deutschland. Deutscher Bundestag, Drucksache 17 / 12200. http://dip21.bundestag.de/dip21/
btd/17 / 122 / 1712200.pdf (Stand: 24.06.2013).
DJI (Deutsches Jugendinstitut e.V.). (Hrsg.). (2011). Sexuelle Gewalt gegen Kinder. Missbrauch
in Institutionen, Forschungsergebnisse und Empfehlungen für einen besseren Kinderschutz.
München: DJI.
Hurrelmann, K., & Timm, A. (2011). Kinder, Bildung, Zukunft Drei Wege aus der Krise. Stuttgart:
Klett.
Klieme, E., Artelt, C., Hartig, J., Jude, N., Köller, O., Prenze, M., Schneider, W., & Stanat, P.
(Hrsg.). (2010) PISA 2009. Bilanz nach einem Jahrzehnt. Münster/ New York/ München/
Berlin: Waxmann.
Appendices
The design of the Third World Vision Child Study 2013 and the methods applied are
oriented toward the two earlier Child Studies of 2007 and 2010. Once again, we
carried out a quantitative survey of a representative sample of 6- to 11-year-old
children (in 2007, only 8- to 11-year-old children). We supplemented this standard-
ized survey with a qualitative assessment resulting in 12 portraits of children who
had been selected systematically. Within the study design, our quantitative survey
served to present the living conditions and life worlds of children in all their breadth.
The qualitative survey portrays selected children, thereby delivering an impression
of children in their life worlds characterized by more depth and closeness to daily
life.
The underlying population for the quantitative survey is children in Germany
between the ages of 6 and 11 years. We surveyed 2535 children. To gather enough
cases for analyses in the new German states in the east of the country, we used a
disproportional sample distribution: A total of 1729 interviews were carried out in
the old German states (including Berlin) and 806 in the new German states. Of
course, we took this deliberate overrepresentation of children from the new German
states into account when analyzing the data. We did this by using official statistics
to construct a sample weighting framework and projecting the sample on to the resi-
dential population aged 6–11 years in the actual East–West relationship differenti-
ated according to gender. We also took account of the fact that not all 6-year-olds
are already attending school. Weighted in this way, the structure of the sample cor-
responds to the true distribution of 6- to 11-year-old school children found in offi-
cial statistics.
Survey Instruments
The quantitative survey is based on two different instruments: First, the interview
with the children was a personal oral survey conducted by well-trained interviewers
on the basis of a set standardized assessment instrument. The survey itself was
computer-assisted: The respondent’s answers were entered directly into the inter-
viewer’s laptop computer. The second instrument was a short written questionnaire
that one parent was asked to complete preferably while the child was being inter-
viewed. This parent questionnaire was used to gather basic sociodemographic infor-
mation on the child’s family background.
When developing the child questionnaire, we tried not only to ensure that the
instruments were comparable with those used in the 2007 and 2010 surveys, but
also to focus on one special topic in each Child Study—in this case, the idea of
justice. When rating their idea of justice, the children were given visual support in
the form of a 4-point smiley scale. This visual support enabled them to quickly ori-
ent themselves and perform a ranking on the given dimension. The items on satis-
faction or on well-being were assessed with the 5-point smiley scale used in previous
Child Studies. The children also received visual support with their qualitative rat-
ings in other response formats (see the questionnaire in Appendix 4).
While preparing the study, we tested the suitability of our instruments as part of
a cognitive pretest of children and then revised the instruments as required. In addi-
tion, when formulating items on the idea of justice, we took account of results
obtained from the first qualitative interviews that had already been carried out by
this time. In the subsequent pilot test in the field, several interviewers carried out 95
advance interviews with children in the target group under the same conditions as
those to be used in the subsequent main survey. This also included asking the chil-
dren to reply to some open unstructured questions on what they understand by the
term justice.
According to the interviewers, the children responded to the Third World Vision
Child Study just as willingly as they had done to the previous ones. In 87% of cases,
the interviewers rated the children’s willingness to answer their questions as good;
and in 12%, as moderate or “sometimes one, sometimes the other.” They rated the
willingness to answer their questions as bad in only 21 children (< 1%). However,
even in these cases, individual analyses revealed no reason to drop them from the
sample. The child questionnaire is documented in Appendix 4 and the parent ques-
tionnaire in Appendix 5.
Sample
As in the first two Child Studies, the survey was based on quota sampling.
Interviewers were instructed to survey a precisely defined number of school chil-
dren aged 6 and above who could be classified to specific subgroups.
Appendices 181
Field Work
Data for the Third World Vision Child Study were collected between the beginning
of January and the middle of February 2013. Therefore, the Third World Vision
Child Study was carried out in the middle of the school year in contrast to the
Second World Vision Child Study, which had been carried out at the beginning of
the school year in the fall of 2009. There were 400 well-trained Infratest interview-
ers who each performed an average of six surveys. For 76% of the surveys, at least
one further person was present. In 61% of all cases, this was the mother; in 15%, the
father.
The interviewers themselves were allowed to select the specific target persons
based on the given criteria in the quota sample. They could recruit these persons
within their personal circle of friends and acquaintances, institutions, or typical
locations in which children congregate. After gaining the parents’ permission in
advance (obligatory in this age range), they interviewed them in their parental
homes.
The average interview lasted 37 minutes. Surveys of 6- to 7-year-old and 8- to
9-year-old target persons took only slightly longer at 37 minutes than those of 10- to
11-year-old target persons at 36 minutes. When looking at the duration in the young-
est age group, it has to be considered that questions on more general topics in soci-
ety were dropped if the child was unable to answer the first leading question on that
topic.
As part of the Infratest controls, interviewers had to document the addresses of
respondents. Infratest controlled roughly 10% of all surveyed target persons at ran-
dom by asking the children’s parents either directly by telephone or by post whether
the interviewer had actually carried out an interview of the given length addressing
the given topic. Further quality controls were applied to the completed question-
naires by checking the internal consistency of the responses. This resulted in only
minor corrections. Reports that were obviously incorrect were analyzed as “no
reply.”
After completion of the field work and the quality controls, we were left with a
valid dataset that could be subjected to a differentiated analysis with the help of the
statistical software packet SPSS 19. This book presents the detailed results of these
analyses.
Appendices 183
Population Children living and attending school in Germany aged 6–11 years (2007:
8–11 years)
Sample Household survey. Quota sample controlled for:
Age and gender
Type of school (elementary, Hauptschule, Realschule, Gymnasium,
other schools, special needs school)
Migration background
Federal State and type of settlement structure (BIK-Type),
Case numbers n = 2,535 child interviews (disproportionate allocation),
n = 1,729 in West German states (including Berlin) und n = 806 in East
German states
Assessment Individual oral child interview (CAPI: Computer Assisted Personal
method Interviews)
Written parent questionnaire (self-report). German- and Turkish-language
versions
Time period Beginning of January to mid-February 2013
Weighting Official statistics (Bevölkerungsfortschreibung 2011 andMikrozensus
2011)
The Social Origins Index applied in the World Vision Child Study is a combination
of the parents’ educational background and the material resources available to the
household. Information on both aspects is taken from the Parent Survey. This is
supplemented by the children’s estimates on the number of books in the home and
the parents’ reports on the housing type. Depending on the replies, each individual
variable is assigned a different number of points.
Social Origins Index: Component Variables and the Number of Points Assigned to Them
Housing type
Rented 1
Own property 2
How well do you manage your household with the amount of money available to you and
your family every month?
Very well 3
Well 2
Moderately 2
Poorly 1
Very poorly 0
The points awarded on the variables are added together to form a summary index.
If information on both parents is available for the variable “educational back-
ground,” the higher of the two qualifications is entered. The summary index can
range from 3 to 14 points.
The Social Origins Index is formed by dividing the summary index into five
groups. Each group then corresponds to one level on the Social Origins Index. This
takes the following ranking:
Appendix 2.1 Social class Index: Variables included and their scores
Thank you for agreeing to take part in our study. I am now going to ask you a few
questions that either tell me about you personally or let me know what you think
about things.
1b. Child is
Male
Female
3. How many people live in your home? Add everybody together and please
count yourself as well.
Interviewer: Make sure that the child counts her or himself as well
Household size ________
Does not apply, I live in a children’s home (wrong target group; end of survey)
Appendices 195
4. And who are all the people you live with in your home? Look at this list, and
tell me which of these persons live with you.
(Question text for 6-, 7-, and 8-year-olds) And who are they? I am going to read out
this list, and I want you to tell me if that person lives together with you in your
home.
Interviewer: Present List 4, read it out loud, and go through it together with the
child.
□ Myself
□ My father
□ My mother
□ My stepfather/My mother’s partner
□ My stepmother/My father’s partner
□ My brother or brothers (Interviewer: also stepsiblings)
□ My sister or sisters
□ My grandfather
□ My grandmother
□ Other relatives (uncle or aunt, cousins, …)
□ Other people my parents know
□ Yes
□ No, I do not have any brothers or sisters
7. Were your parents born in Germany, or does one or both of your parents
come from another country?
8. (If one or both parents were born in another country; otherwise proceed to
Question 9)
What language do you speak mostly at home?
□ Mostly German
□ Mostly the language of my parents (my non-German
parent)
9. Do you think that your (if the child reports living with a father who has a new
partner in Question 4, add “real”) mother gives you enough of her time?
□ Yes
□ Sometimes yes, sometimes no
□ No
□ Doesn’t apply, mother not present
10. (If the child reports living with a stepmother or the father’s new partner in
Question 4)
And do you think that your stepmother or your father’s new partner gives you
enough of her time?
□ Yes
□ Sometimes yes, sometimes no
□ No
11. How about your (if the child reports living with a mother who has a new partner
in Question 4, add “real”) father? Does he give you enough of his time?
□ Yes
□ Sometimes yes, sometimes no
□ No
□ Doesn’t apply, no father present
Appendices 197
12. (If the child reports living with a stepfather or the mother’s new partner in
Question 4)
And your stepfather or your mother’s new partner, does he give you enough of his
time?
□ Yes
□ Sometimes yes, sometimes no
□ No
13. Generally speaking, how happy are you about the way your parents look
after you? Look at the faces on this list and show me which one fits best.
You can just point with your finger or you can tell me which letter fits.
Interviewer: Show the Smiley scale (5-point smiley scale at end of booklet). Let
younger children point to the faces and then classify them.
A B C D E
□ No
□ Hardly ever
□ Sometimes
□ Very often
16. Roughly how many books are there in your home? We have prepared the
following answers for you.
(For 6-, 7-, and 8-year-olds in addition) I shall read them out loud. (For all chil-
dren) Just tell me which is the right amount or point it out to me with your
finger.
Interviewer: Present List 16, read the response categories out loud, and use your
finger to point out which answer you mean. If necessary, repeat the procedure twice.
Interviewer: This item assesses the number of books in the home and not just the
child’s own books.
198 Appendices
1 2 3 4 5
Only a few (up to 10 books)
About one shelf (11–24 books)
About one bookcase (25–100 books)
About two bookcases (101–200 books)
Three or more bookcases (more than 200 books)
17. Do you have a room of your own?
Interviewer: If the child shares her or his room with other children (e.g., brother,
sister, cousin), enter 2 “Yes, together with my brother(s) or sister(s)”
18. (If the child reports having her or his own room or a room with siblings in
Question 17)
In your room, do you have
(a) Yes (b) No
Only continue with further questions when child answers “More like things are” to
Question 19–2 or “Less like things are” to Question 19–4
_______Because there is not enough money in my family, I can hardly ever go to
movies or swimming pool.
_______Sometimes we can’t afford to buy things for school such as exercise books
or pens.
_______I can’t join a club or learn to play a musical instrument, because my family
cannot afford to pay for it.
_______We hardly ever have children’s birthday parties because they cost too
much.
_______I often have to wear clothes that are out of fashion.
_______From time to time, we get food for free, for example, from the “Tafel” (a
stall where people can get free food handouts).
_______Sometimes I am cold in winter because I do not have any warm clothes.
_______Because there is not enough money in our family, I sometimes earn some
extra money myself.
_______Within the last year, my parents have had to borrow money from my own
savings.
_______Every year we take at least a one-week vacation away from home.
20. How satisfied are you in general with the amount of freedom your parents
give you in your daily life; in other words, what they let you do and what
they don’t let you do? Please just tell me which face in the list fits best or
tell me which letter fits.
Interviewer: Show the Smiley scale at the end of the booklet. Let younger children
point to the faces and then protocol them.
A B C D E
Justice
21. I shall now read out some sentences. Please tell me whether you find them
to be very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair.
Interviewer: Give children the list for Question 21. Let younger children point to the
faces and then protocol them.
poor parents. Do you think that is very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very
unfair?
_______Mostly it’s adults—and not the children themselves—who decide where
and when to build a children’s playground. This is because they say that
they are the ones who know best about such things. Do you think that is
very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
_______What would you think if non-German children had to speak only German
not just in class but also in the school breaks. Do you think that is very fair,
quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
22. And how fair do you think things are in general?
Interviewer: Give children the list. Let younger children point to the smiley faces
and then protocol them.
Read out the response categories once, and then as needed.
A B C D E
_______In your family, are things very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
_______And in your school? Are things there very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or
very unfair?
_______And with your friends? (pause, and when no response) Are things very fair,
quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
_______And when you think about Germany as a whole? (pause, and when no
response) Are things very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
For 6- to 7-year-olds who reply to Question 22.4 with “Don’t know/No answer,”
skip Question 22.5
_______And what about the whole world? (pause, and when no response) Are
things very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
23. For 6- to 7-year-olds who reply to Question 22.4 or Question 22.5 with “Don’t
know/No, answer,” skip Question 23
What do you think: Is the way people treat the following groups in Germany
very fair, quite fair, rather unfair, or very unfair?
Interviewer: Give children the list. Let younger children point to the smiley faces
and then protocol them.
Read out the response categories once, and then as needed.
A B C D E
_______Children or adolescents
_______Old people
_______Foreigners
Appendices 201
_______The handicapped
_______Poor people
24. And how about you yourself? Do you feel treated unfairly for any of the
following reasons?
Interviewer: Give children the list for Question 24
(a) Often (b) Sometimes (c) Hardly ever (Does not apply)
_______Because of your age
_______Because you are a boy/girl (depending on answer to Question 1. b)
_______Because of your external appearance (that is, what you look like)
_______Because your parents don’t have much money
_______Because your mother or your father do not come from Germany (if child
reports that either one or both parents were not born in Germany in
Question 7)
School/Institutions
25. Now let’s turn to school. What grade are you in?
26. How do you like school? Please point to the smiley that fits or tell me which
letter fits.
Interviewer: Show the Smiley scale. Let younger children point to the faces and then
protocol them.
A B C D E
27. What would you say? Are you
□ Yes
□ No
□ No
□ Yes, once a week
□ Yes, more than once a week
□ Hardly ever
□ Not very often
□ Once a week
□ Several times a week
□ Every day
Appendices 205
39. (Skip question if child reports “hardly ever” watching television in Question
36)
And how long do you normally watch television or DVDs every day?
Interviewer: This question addresses regular television viewing on weekdays.
40. (Skip question if child reports “hardly ever” playing computer games in
Question 36)
And how about computer games, Gameboy, PlayStation, and the like? How
often do you play with them?
41. (Answers 3 or 4 on Question 40) And roughly how many hours do you play
with them each day (on Question 40 = 4)/on these days (on Question 40 = 3)?
Interviewer: Please ask for the exact amount of time: Half an hour is 0.5 h and a
quarter of an hour is 0.25 h.
□ Yes
□ No, I do not have my own mobile phone
206 Appendices
43. Do you have access to the Internet (that is, can you go online with a com-
puter)? (Filter: Proceed to Question 47 if child reports “No answer/Don’t
know”
44. (If Question 43 = yes) Do you use the Internet regularly every week? (Filter:
Proceed to Question 47 if child reports “No answer/Don’t know”).
□ Yes
□ No, only occasionally, or never (Proceed to Question 47)
45. (If Question 44 = yes) And for roughly how long do you use the Internet
each week?
46. (If child reports “Up to 1 hour” and above on Question 45)
What do you do most of the time on the Internet?
Interviewer: Present list. Name the response categories again for each question. For
younger children, read them out loud while using your finger to point out which
answer you mean.
(a) Hardly ever (b) Sometimes (c) Very often
_______Send e-mails
_______Search for something in particular
_______Use Facebook (or other social networks such as Local List)
_______Chat, that is, talk to others on the Internet
_______Just surf to see what I can find
_______Play computer games
_______Look at or download videos
_______Download music
_______Twitter
Appendices 207
47. Now, make a rough guess: Round about how many friends do you have?
□ One
□ 2–3
□ 4–5
□ 6–9
□ 10 or more
□ None at all (Proceed to Question 49)
48. Skip if child reports “None at all” in Question 47; still pose question if child
reports “No answer/Don’t know”
And how many really good friends do you have?
□ 1
□ 2
□ 3
□ 4
□ 5
□ 6–9
□ 10 or more
□ None at all
Check: Number reported in Question 48 should not be higher than that reported in
Question 47
49. Do you find it easier or more difficult to make friends?
□ Easier
□ More difficult
50. If child reports between “1” and “10 or more” in Question 47 or between “1”
and “10 or more” in Question 48. Skip if child reports “None at all” in
Question 48
How frequently do you meet your friends/friend?
Interviewer: Present the list and read out the response categories
(a) Just about every day (b) Several times a week (c) Hardly ever/
Never
_______At school?
208 Appendices
□ Generally yes
□ Generally no
56. Which of the following people do you think care more about what you
think and which care less?
Interviewer: Please do not read “Sometimes one, sometimes the other” out loud.
Only offer it as a response category when the child is hesitant.
(a) Generally more (b) Generally less (c) Sometimes, one, sometimes
the other
_______Your mother (does she tend to care more about what you think or less?)
_______Your father.
_______Your friends.
_______Your class teacher.
_______ (If child reports attending some sort of after-school care in Question 31)
The staff at the place you go to after school.
57. Do the following things worry you?
Interviewer: Give the child the list. Name the response categories again for each
question. For younger children, read them out loud while using your finger to point
out which answer you mean.
(a) Hardly ever (b) Sometimes (c) Very often
_______Poor grades and that I won’t be able to keep up with the others at school
_______That my parents will become unemployed or not be able to find any work
_______That somebody might bully or hit me
_______That environmental pollution is increasing
_______That there are more and more people who are poor in Germany
_______That a war might suddenly break out in Germany
_______That more and more foreigners are coming to Germany all the time
210 Appendices
58. Are you interested in politics? Would you say that you are
Interviewer: If the child does not understand what is meant by politics, then please
explain as follows:
“What I mean is what politicians (such as Frau Merkel) or what political parties
do.”
□ I think so
□ I don’t really think so
□ Can’t decide, can’t say
60. And, finally, how happy are you with your life in general?
Interviewer: Show the Smiley scale (5-point smiley scale at end of booklet). Let
younger children point to the faces and then classify them.
A B C D E
Thank you for taking part and answering my questions!
To be completed by the interviewer.
61. How willing was the child to answer the questions?
□ Very willing
□ Moderately willing
□ Unwilling
□ Sometimes one, sometimes the other
□ No
□ Yes
Appendices 211
□ The mother
□ The father
□ Grandparents
□ Siblings
□ Other relatives
□ Other children
□ Other adults
64. (If Question 62 = yes) Did anybody intervene during the course of the inter-
view (e.g., by reformulating questions, giving hints, or influencing the
direction of answers)?
Dear Parents,
Thank you very much for granting permission for your child to participate in the
Third World Vision Child Study and allowing us to ask our questions. In the follow-
ing questionnaire, we would like to obtain some background information on you
and your family.
Naturally, your answers will be dealt with completely anonymously and they will
not be passed on or made available to any third persons.
P 02 I am
Married
A single parent
P 03a My present age is
years.
P 03b If you are married or living with your partner
The present age of my partner is
years.
P 04a My (highest) secondary school education is
Hauptschule (8th–9th grade)
Realschule/Mittlere Reife (10th grade)
Fachhochschulreife
Abitur
I have no secondary school qualifications
I am still attending secondary school
P 04b If you are married or living with your partner
My partner’s (highest) secondary school education is
Hauptschule (8th–9th grade)
Realschule/mittlere Reife (10th grade)
Fachhochschulreife
Abitur
My partner has no secondary school qualifications
My partner is still attending secondary school
P 05a I am currently
Employed full-time (35 h a week or more)
Employed part-time (between 15 and 35 h a week)
Marginally employed (less than 15 h a week)
Unemployed
Taking parental leave or some other form of leave
In training
Studying or still attending secondary school
A housewife or househusband or for some other reason not employed
P 05b If you are married or living with your partner
My partner is currently
Employed full-time (35 h a week or more)
Employed part-time (between 15 and 35 h a week)
Appendices 213
Turkey
Greece
Italy
Former Yugoslavia
Former Soviet Union
Another West European country
Another East European country
An Arabic country
An African country
Another country
P 08b If you are married or living with your partner
My partner was born in
Germany
Turkey
Greece
Italy
Former Yugoslavia
Former Soviet Union
Another West European country
Another East European country
An Arabic country
An African country
Another country
P 09a My current nationality is
German
Turkish
Greek
Italian
Former Yugoslavian
Former Soviet Union
Another West European country
Another East European country
An Arabic country
An African country
Another country
P 09b If you are married or living with your partner
My partner’s current nationality is
German
Turkish
Greek
Italian
Appendices 215
Former Yugoslavian
Former Soviet Union
Another West European country
Another East European country
An Arabic country.
African
Another country
P 10a My religion
Catholic
Protestant
Other Christian religion
Islam
Other non-Christian religion
No religious affiliation
P 10b If you are married or living with your partner
My partner’s religion
Catholic
Protestant
Other Christian religion
Islam
Other non-Christian religion
No religious affiliation
P 11 What is your child’s nationality?
German
Non-German
My child has dual nationality
P 12 What type of school is your child currently attending?
Elementary school
Hauptschule
Realschule
Gymnasium
School with several tracks (orientation stage [Orientierungsstufe], comprehensive,
…)
Special needs school
P 13 Does your child spend some time outside the family or school in, for exam-
ple, a club such as a sports club, a social group, a music group, a church
group, a nature or animal protection group, or the like?
Sports club (soccer, swimming, horse riding, tennis, …)
Music group/Music school
Dance club/Ballet
Painting/drawing group
Theater or movie group
216 Appendices
Church group
Girl guides/Boy scouts
Nature or animal protection society
Organized group in a child or youth club
Trachtenverein [society for traditional costumes]/Brauchtumspflege [cultural folk-
lore group]
Other group or club (please specify) ______________________
No, my child does not belong to any organized group or a club
P 14 And when your child was younger, did she or he attend a day nursery?
No, my child did not attend a day nursery (Proceed to Question P 15)
Yes, my child did attend a day nursery (Proceed to Question P 14a)
P 14a How old was your child when she or he first attended a day nursery?
Age when my child first attended a day nursery:
Less than one year old
One year old
Two years old
More than two years old
P 15 Did your child attend a Kindergarten or preschool?
No, my child did not attend a Kindergarten or preschool (Proceed to Question P 16)
Yes, my child did attend a Kindergarten or preschool (Proceed to Question P 15a)
P 15a How old was your child when she or he first attended a Kindergarten or
preschool?
Age when my child first attended a Kindergarten or preschool
Less than three years old
Three years old
Four years old
Five years old
More than five years old
P 16 We live in a
Single family house (terraced house or detached)
In a smaller building containing several apartments (up to 12 apartments)
In a larger building or block (more than 12 apartments)
P 17 Type of housing
Rented
Own property
P 18 In your own household, how well do you get by on the amount of money
available to you and your family each month?
Very well
Quite well
Just about okay
Less well
Appendices 217
Circus Story
Next month, a famous circus will be coming to town. The children in a school class
would really like to go to the circus and see the show. Although the class has its own
savings account with money to pay for excursions, there is not enough money to pay
for tickets for everybody to go to the circus. The class is discussing what they can
do. The children come up with the idea of holding a garage sale. All the children
should bring things they no longer need from home so that they can be put on sale.
On the day of the garage sale, all the children bring things to be sold. Only Jana
and Emil come with empty hands. Anton and Luise, in contrast, have brought a
particularly large number of things. What do you think: Should all the children be
able to go to the circus?
After the question has been discussed in detail, the next question probes deeper:
At the circus, the class has reserved a complete block of seats. One-half of the chil-
dren can sit in the front row; the other one-half have to sit behind them. How
should the children decide who gets to sit in the front row? Why?
If the children fail to grasp the dilemma themselves, the questioner poses follow-
u p questions and asks whether the children would judge the following two versions
differently:
Version A: Jana didn’t manage to bring anything because she was ill and Emil sim-
ply didn’t have anything at home that he could bring.
Version B: Jana and Emil couldn’t be bothered/had something else they wanted to
do on the day they should have spent some time looking for things to bring.
218 Appendices
Flute Story
Three children—Anne, Bob, and Carla—are quarrelling over who should get a flute
to play with. Imagine you have to decide which child should get the flute. Anne
claims the flute because she is the only one of the three who knows how to play it.
Bob points out that he is the only one of the three who has no other toys to play with
because he is poor. Carla, in contrast, claims the flute because she is the one who
spent several months working hard to make it. Which child should get the flute and
why should that child get it?
Lukas and Philipp are twins. Now that the school marks the children’s work and
gives them grades, their parents have decided to give them a reward of two Euros for
every Grade 1 (Excellent) and one Euro for every Grade 2 (Good). They get no
money for a Grade 3 (Fair). Philipp is happy, because even when he doesn’t bother
to do any revision on the day before the test, he still always manages to get a 1 or a
2. Things are different for Lukas. He also wants to get good grades and he tries very
hard. Sometimes, he even starts learning and revising hard a couple of days before
the test. Nonetheless, he often manages to get only a 3 and sometimes occasionally
a 2.
Lukas goes to his parents and tells them that he finds the arrangement unfair
because he hardly ever gets a reward—even though he works much harder than
Philipp! What do you think?
Provocation and Punishment
Jakob and Carsten don’t like each other very much, so they mostly just avoid each
other. During a break in the schoolyard, Carsten suddenly starts to call Jakob names.
Jakob tries not to listen, but Carsten doesn’t stop. He provokes Jakob more and
more. Eventually, Jakob gets very angry and gives Carsten a hard push. Carsten falls
down and grazes his knee. The teacher sees this and steps in immediately. Jakob is
kept in after school and given extra work to do as a punishment. What do you think
of that?
Appendices 219
A class goes on regular school trips. The children get to vote where the class will go
on each trip. Every time there is a vote, 12 children vote for the swimming pool and
8 children vote for the zoo. The class has already gone swimming twice. On the
third time, the children who want to go to the zoo protest. They think that it is not
fair when they have to go the swimming pool again on their school trip. What do
you think?
After the question has been discussed in detail, the next question follows:
Udo is the only child who has always wanted to go to the museum. Hardly any of
the other children want this. Should the class nonetheless take one trip to the
museum?
Participation
A small town has a playground where the younger children like to go and play. The
town mayor believes that there are still not enough places in the town where youths
can meet each other. He wants to organize a meeting with the citizens of the town
and discuss whether the playground should be replaced by a youth center. All the
adult residents of the town are invited to an evening meeting at the town hall. They
should vote whether to keep the playground or to build a youth center instead. The
children are not invited to attend the meeting. What do you think of that?
References
AGJ – Arbeitsgemeinschaft für Kinder- und Jugendhilfe. (Hrsg.). (2012). Sozialgesetzbuch VIII
auf dem Stand des Bundeskinderschutzgesetzes. Berlin: Gesamttext und Begründungen.
Allbus: Datenreport. (2011). Ein Sozialbericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Bonn:
Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung.
Allensbacher Institut für Demoskopie. (2013). Was ist gerecht? Gerechtigkeitsbegriff und -wah-
rnehmung der Bürger. Allensbacher Archiv, IfD-Umfrage 11001, Allensbach.
Andresen, S., & Heitmeyer, W. (Hrsg.). (2012). Zerstörerische Vorgänge. Missachtung und sexuelle
Gewalt gegen Kinder und Jugendliche in Institutionen. Weinheim und Basel: Beltz Juventa.
Andresen, S., & Hurrelmann, K. (2007). Was bedeutet es, heute ein Kind zu sein? Die World Vision
Kinderstudie als Beitrag zur Kinder- und Kindheitsforschung. In World Vision Deutschland.
(Hrsg.) Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 35 – 64). Frankfurt a. M.:
Fischer.
Andresen, S., Hurrelmann, K., & Fegter, S. (2010). Wie geht es unseren Kindern? Wohlbefinden
und Lebensbedingungen der Kinder in Deutschland. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.)
Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 35 – 59). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Andresen, S., Hurrelmann, K., & Schneekloth, U. (2012). Care and freedom. Theoretical and
empirical aspects of children’s well-being. Child Indicators Research, 5, 437–448.
Andresen, S., Mailand, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013). Erfahrungen und Erleben von Armut
aus der Sicht von Kindern: »Ich würde meiner Familie was schenken und dafür sorgen, dass sie
nicht so viel in Schwierigkeiten sind.« Unsere Jugend 65. Jg. (S. 123 – 129).
Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung. (2012). Bildung in Deutschland 2012. Ein indika-
torengestützter Bericht mit einer Analyse zu Perspektiven des Bildungswesens im demograf-
ischen Wandel. Bielefeld: W. Bertelsmann Verlag.
AWO-ISS. (2012). Lebenssituation und Zukunftschancen von (armen) Kindern und Jugendlichen.
AWO-Verlag.
Bacher, J., Winkelhofer, U., & Teubner, M. (2007). Partizipation von Kindern in der Grundschule.
In Alt, C. (Hrsg.), Kinderleben – Start in die Grundschule (Band 3: Ergebnisse aus der zweiten
Welle, S. 137 – 163). Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
Baumert, J., Stanat, P., & Watermann, R. (Hrsg.). (2006). Herkunftsbedingte Disparitäten im
Bildungswesen. Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance
use. Journal of early adolescence. Band 11, Heft 1, S. 56 – 95.
Bertelsmann Ländermonitor Frühkindliche Bildungssysteme (o. J.) (2013) Tabelle 6, Tabelle 7,
Tabelle 8 (Stand ab 2007). http://www.laendermonitor.de/downloads-presse/index.nc.html?no_
cache=1 (Stand: 19.06.2013).
Hadjar, A., & Becker, R. (Hrsg.). (2006). Die Bildungsexpansion. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encour-
ages equal sharing in children but not chimpanzees. Nature. doi: 10.1038/nature10278. http://
wkprc.eva.mpg.de/pdf/2011/Hamann_Warneken_Greenberg_Tomasello_2011.pdf (Stand:
20.08.2013).
HBSC-Team Deutschland. (2011). Studie Health Behaviour in School-aged Children – Faktenblatt
»Lebenszufriedenheit von Kindern und Jugendlichen”. Bielefeld: WHO Collaborating Centre
for Child and Adolescent Health Promotion.
Hernandez, D. J., & Marotz, K. G. (2012). Disparities in child well-being across income groups:
Trends in the U.S. from 1985 to 2008. Child Indicators Research, 5(1) S. 93 – 121.
Hinsch, W. (2002). Gerechtfertigte Ungleichheiten: Grundsätze sozialer Gerechtigkeit. Berlin:
Walter de Gruyter.
Holst, E., & Seifert, H. (2012). Arbeitszeitpolitische Kontroversen im Spiegel der
Arbeitszeitwünsche. WSI-Mitteilungen, 2 / 2012, S. 141 – 149.
Hopf, W. (2010). Freiheit-Leistung-Ungleichheit. Bildung und soziale Herkunft in Deutschland.
Weinheim: Beltz.
Hunner-Kreisel, C., & Stephan, M. (Hrsg.). (2013). Neue Zeiten, neue Räume: Kindheit und
Familie im Kontext von (Trans-)Migration und sozialem Wandel. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Hurrelmann, K., & Schultz, T. (Hrsg.). (2012). Jungen als Bildungsverlierer. Weinheim: Beltz
Juventa.
Hurrelmann, K., & Timm, A. (2011). Kinder, Bildung, Zukunft Drei Wege aus der Krise. Stuttgart:
Klett.
Hurrelmann, K., Quenzel, G., & Rathmann, K. (2011). Bildungspolitik als Bestandteil moderner
Wohlfahrtspolitik. Zeitschrift für Soziologie der Erziehung und Sozialisation, 31, S. 313 – 328.
Ittel, A., & Scheithauer, H. (2007). Geschlecht als »Stärke« oder »Risiko«? Überlegungen zur
geschlechterspezifischen Resilienz. In Opp, G., & Fingerle, M. (Hrsg.). Was Kinder stärkt.
Erziehung zwischen Risiko und Resilienz (S. 98 – 115). München/Basel: Reinhardt.
Jampert, K., Zehnbauer, A., Sens, A., Leuckefeld, K., & Laier, M. (Hrsg.). (2009). Kinder-Sprache
stärken. Aufwachsen mit mehreren Sprachen. Praxismaterial. Weimar/Berlin: Das Netz.
Klieme, E., Artelt, C., Hartig, J., Jude, N., Köller, O., Prenze, M., Schneider, W., & Stanat, P.
(Hrsg.). (2010) PISA 2009. Bilanz nach einem Jahrzehnt. Münster/ New York/ München/
Berlin: Waxmann.
KMK Sekretariat. (2012). Allgemein bildende Schulen in Ganztagsform in Ländern der
Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Bonn und Berlin: KMK.
Kohlberg, L. (1964). The development of moral character and ideology. In M. L. Hoffmann (Ed.),
Review of child development research. New York: Wiley.
Kohlberg, L. (1974). Zur kognitiven Entwicklung des Kindes. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp.
LBS-Kinderbarometer. (2011). Stimmungen, Trends und Meinungen von Kindern aus Deutschland.
Recklinghausen: RDN Verlag.
Leven, I., & Schneekloth, U. (2007). Die Freizeit. Anregen lassen oder fernsehen. In World Vision
Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 165 – 200).
Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Leven, I., & Schneekloth, U. (2010). Die Schule: Frühe Vergabe von Lebenschancen. In World
Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2010 (S. 161–185). Frankfurt a. M.:
Fischer.
Liebig, S., & Lengfeld, H. (Hrsg.). (2002). Interdisziplinäre Gerechtigkeitsforschung. Zur
Verknüpfung empirischer und normativer Perspektiven. Hamburg: Campus.
Liebig, S., & May, M. (2009). Dimensionen sozialer Gerechtigkeit. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte,
47 / 2009, S. 3 – 8.
Luthar, S. S. (1999). Poverty and children‘s adjustment. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
224 References
Maaz, K., Baumert, J., Gresch, C., & McElvany, N. (Hrsg.). (2010) Der Übergang von der
Grundschule in die weiterführenden Schulen. Berlin und Bonn: BMBF.
Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest. (Hrsg.). (2012). FIM-Studie (2011): Familie,
Interaktion & Medien. Untersuchung zur Kommunikation und Mediennutzung in Familien.
Stuttgart: Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest: LFK.
Meiland, S., Andresen, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013a) Denkst du, die Kinder sind hier arm?
Sichtweisen und Erfahrungen von Kindern in prekären Lebenslagen. In Fisher, D., Gathen, J.,
Höhmann, K., Klaffke, T., Rademacker, H. (Hrsg.). Friedrich Jahresheft XXXI 2013, Schule
und Armut (S. 16 – 18). Oldenburg: Friedrich Verlag.
Meiland, S., Andresen, S., Milanovic, D., & Blume, J. (2013b). Erfahrungen und Erleben von
Armut aus der Sicht von Kindern: »Ich würde meiner Familie was schenken und dafür sorgen,
dass sie nicht so viel in Schwierigkeiten sind.« Unsere Jugend 65. Jg. (S. 123 – 129).
Minkkinen, J. (2013). The structural model of child well-being. Child Indicators Research, 6(3),
547–558.
Morschitzky, H. (2009). Angststörungen. Diagnostik, Konzepte, Theorie, Selbsthilfe (4. Auflage).
Heidelberg: Springer.
Oerter, R., & Montada, L. (Hrsg.). (2002). Entwicklungspsychologie (5. vollständig überarbeitete
Auflage). Weinheim: Beltz.
O’Hare, W. P., & Gutierrez, F. (2012). The use of domains in constructing a comprehensive com-
posite index of child well-being. Child Indicators Research, 5, 609–629.
Olk, T., & Roth, R. (2007). Mehr Partizipation wagen. Gütersloh: Bertelsmann Stiftung.
Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional development in a relational context;
Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. In Berndt, T. J. & Ladd, G. W.
(Hrsg.), Peer relationships in child development. Zitiert nach: Oerter, R., & Montada, L. (1998)
Entwicklungspsychologie. Ein Lehrbuch. Weinheim: Beltz.
Piaget, J. (1932/ 1983). Das moralische Urteil beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett.
Piaget, J. (1937 /1975). Der Aufbau der Wirklichkeit beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett.
Piaget, J. (1999). Über Pädagogik. Beltz: Weinheim.
Prazen, A., Wolfinger, N. H., Cahill, C., & Kowaleski-Jones, L. (2011). Joint physical custody and
neighborhood friendships in middle childhood. Sociological Inquiry, 81(2), 247–259.
Pupeter, M., & Schneekloth, U. (2010). Die Gleichaltrigen: Gemeinsame – getrennte Welten?. In
World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie
(S. 141 – 160). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Quenzel, G., & Hurrelmann, K. (Hrsg.). (2010). Bildungsverlierer. Wiesbaden: Verlag für
Sozialwissenschaften.
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Harvard University Press.
Robert Bosch Stiftung. (2012). Starke Kinder – starke Familie. Wohlbefinden von Kindern in
Städten und Gemeinden. Stuttgart: Robert Bosch Stiftung.
Salvas, M.-C., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Lacourse, E., Boivin, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2011).
Interplay between friends’ aggression and friendship quality in the development of child
aggression during the early school years. Social Development, 20(4), 645–663.
Schneekloth, U. (2010). Jugend und Politik: Aktuelle Entwicklungstrends und Perspektiven. In
Shell Deutschland Holding. (Hrsg.), Jugend 2010. Eine pragmatische Generation behauptet
sich (16. Shell Jugendstudie). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer, S. 129 – 164.
Schneekloth, U., & Leven, I. (2007). Familie als Zentrum: nicht für alle gleich verlässlich. In
World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2007. 1. World Vision Kinderstudie
(S. 65 – 109). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Schneekloth, U., & Pupeter, M. (2010a). Familie als Zentrum: Bunt und vielfältig, aber nicht für
alle Kinder gleich verlässlich. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland
2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 61 – 94). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Schneekloth, U., & Pupeter, M. (2010b). Wohlbefinden, Wertschätzung, Selbstwirksamkeit:
Was Kinder für ein gutes Leben brauchen. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in
Deutschland 2010. 2. World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 187 – 221). Frankfurt a. M: Fischer.
References 225
Schneewind, K. A. (2008). Sozialisation in der Familie. In Hurrelmann, K., Grundmann, M., &
Walper, S. (Hrsg.), Handbuch Sozialisationsforschung (7. vollst. überarbeitete Auflage, S.
256 – 273). Weinheim: Beltz.
Schroeder, D., Picot, S., & Andresen, S. (2010). Die qualitative Studie: 12 Porträts von
Kinderpersönlichkeiten. In World Vision Deutschland. (Hrsg.), Kinder in Deutschland 2010. 2.
World Vision Kinderstudie (S. 223 – 240). Frankfurt a. M.: Fischer.
Sen, A. (2010). Die Idee der Gerechtigkeit. München: C. H. Beck Verlag.
Sikorski, U., & Kuchler, B. (2011). Wer muss worauf verzichten? Einschätzungen zur Wohn- und
Lebenssituation der privaten Haushalte. Wirtschaft und Statistik, 5 / 2011, S. 484 – 492.
Stange, W., Meinhold-Henschel, S., & Schack, S. (2009). Mitwirkung (er)leben. Handbuch
zur Durchführung von Beteiligungsprojekten mit Kindern und Jugendlichen. Gütersloh:
Bertelsmann Stiftung.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2009). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit-Einbürgerung 2008 (Fachserie
1 Reihe 2.1). Wiesbaden.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2011a). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit. Bevölkerung mit
Migrationshintergrund-Ergebnisse des Mikrozensus 2010 (Fachserie 1 Reihe 2.2). Wiesbaden.
Statistisches Bundesamt. (2011b). Pressemitteilung Nr. 345 vom 20. September 2011: Knapp die
Hälfte der Großstadtkinder aus Familien mit Migrationshintergrund. Wiesbaden.
Stecher, L., Krüger, H. H., & Rauschenbach, T. (Hrsg.). (2011). Ganztagsschule – neue Schule?
Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
Stubbe, T. (2009). Bildungsentscheidungen und sekundäre Herkunftseffekte. Münster: Waxmann.
Tracy, R. (2007). Wie Kinder Sprachen lernen. Und wie wir sie dabei unterstützen können.
Tübingen: Francke.
Trautner, H. M. (1991). Lehrbuch der Entwicklungspsychologie (Bd. 2: Theorien und Befunde).
Göttingen: Hogrefe.
UNICEF. (2012). Measuring child poverty. New league tables of child poverty in the world’s rich
countries. Report Card 10, Innocenti Research Centre, online unter. www.unicef-irc.org/publi-
cations/pdf/rc10_eng.pdf (Stand: 14.07.2013).
UNICEF. (2013a). The state of the world’s children 2013: Children with disabilities. online:
http://www.unicef.org/sowc2013/files/SWCR2013_ENG_Lo_res_24_Apr_2013.pdf (Stand:
14.07.2013).
UNICEF. (2013b). Child well-being in rich countries. A comparative overview (Innocenti Report
Card 11 (UNICEF)).
Uslucan, H. H. (2008). Männlichkeitsbilder, Familie und Erziehung in den Communities von
Zuwanderern. Berliner Forum Gewaltprävention (BFG Nr. 34).
Woodward, L. J., & Fergusson, D. M. (2001). Life course of young people with anxiety disorders
in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40,
1086–1093.
Zander, M. (2013). Resilienz – Gender – Prekäre Lebenslagen. Wie kann eine gendersensible
Resilienzförderung aussehen? Betrifft Mädchen, 3 / 2013.
Index
F
Family, 2, 8, 10, 11, 14, 15, 20, 23, 24, 26, 31, M
40, 41, 45, 47, 49, 53–57, 60, 65–69, Migration / migration background, 2, 9,
71, 72, 74, 76–81, 88, 92, 101, 12–14, 37, 42, 44–49, 53, 61–67, 75,
107–109, 115–117, 120–125, 130, 131, 92, 117, 118, 126, 127, 133, 134, 139,
135, 140, 149–160, 162–164, 171, 140, 146, 157, 158, 164
173–176 Mother, 54, 57–60, 63, 74, 76, 77, 79,
Family types, 47, 54, 131, 157 158, 162
R
Respect (of what they think), 7, 102, 161, U
162, 164, 175 Unemployment of parents, 13, 30, 49
S W
Satisfaction, 3, 4, 6, 7, 25–28, 42, 46, 49, Well-being, 2–8, 10, 12, 14, 18–20, 24, 25,
103–104, 107, 132–134, 136, 145–147, 30, 32, 33, 41, 81, 95, 96, 135, 165,
162, 167 167, 172, 173