(Education in a Competitive and Globalizing World Series) Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas (Editors) - Recent Trends in Education (Education in a Competitive and Globalizing World Series)-Nova S
(Education in a Competitive and Globalizing World Series) Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas (Editors) - Recent Trends in Education (Education in a Competitive and Globalizing World Series)-Nova S
No part of this digital document may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or
by any means. The publisher has taken reasonable care in the preparation of this digital document, but makes no
expressed or implied warranty of any kind and assumes no responsibility for any errors or omissions. No
liability is assumed for incidental or consequential damages in connection with or arising out of information
contained herein. This digital document is sold with the clear understanding that the publisher is not engaged in
rendering legal, medical or any other professional services.
EDUCATION IN A COMPETITIVE
AND GLOBALIZING WORLD SERIES
Motivation in Education
Desmond H. Elsworth (Editor)
2009. ISBN: 978-1-60692-234-7
BORISLAV KUZMANOVIĆ
AND
ADELINA CUEVAS
EDITORS
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or
transmitted in any form or by any means: electronic, electrostatic, magnetic, tape, mechanical
photocopying, recording or otherwise without the written permission of the Publisher.
For permission to use material from this book please contact us:
Telephone 631-231-7269; Fax 631-231-8175
Web Site: http://www.novapublishers.com
Independent verification should be sought for any data, advice or recommendations contained in
this book. In addition, no responsibility is assumed by the publisher for any injury and/or damage
to persons or property arising from any methods, products, instructions, ideas or otherwise
contained in this publication.
This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information with regard to the
subject matter covered herein. It is sold with the clear understanding that the Publisher is not
engaged in rendering legal or any other professional services. If legal or any other expert
assistance is required, the services of a competent person should be sought. FROM A
DECLARATION OF PARTICIPANTS JOINTLY ADOPTED BY A COMMITTEE OF THE
AMERICAN BAR ASSOCIATION AND A COMMITTEE OF PUBLISHERS.
CONTENTS
Preface ix
Chapter 1 Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role
in the Lives of People with Disabilities 1
William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein, Shelley Kaplan,
María Verónica Reina and Peter Blanck
Chapter 2 Language-Cognition Interactions During Bilingual Language
Development in Children 39
Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
Chapter 3 Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High
School Level 71
Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Chapter 4 Teleology and Evolution Education 107
Kostas Kampourakis and Vasiliki Zogza
Chapter 5 The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing
Instruction 131
Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundín, Jesús Nicasio García
and Mark Torrance
Chapter 6 Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 155
C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
Chapter 7 Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 175
Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
Chapter 8 Putting PBL Into Practice: Powers and Limitations of Different
Types of Scenarios 195
Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
Chapter 9 Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music
Instruction 215
Bernard W. Andrews
viii Contents
Chapter 10 Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 239
Leslie Wasserman
Chapter 11 Tailoring and Webcasting for Patient and Student Health Education 253
Ray Jones and Inocencio Maramba
Index 263
PREFACE
This important book presents new research from around the world in the field of
education. Technological advancement has broadened educational, employment and training
opportunities for students and adults with disabilities via distance education. This book
reviews the breadth of distance learning initiatives and their benefits and challenges for
learners with disabilities. Furthermore, it is generally known that continuous learning and
upgrading of knowledge in any area may result in a relatively permanent change of behavior.
This book examines how providing health care workers with knowledge in the area of
nosocomial infections may result in a more consistent implementation of hygienic and other
reasonable measures for the prevention of nosocomial infections. Patients and the general
public are now better educated and have access to vast amounts of medical knowledge. This
book also reviews the current status of medical decision-making, highlights the challenges in
trying to ensure that patients are empowered to participant in their decisions. Other chapters
in this book discuss how a child's bilingualism may affect his/her linguistic and cognitive
skills, the benefits of traditional e-learning for students and a discussion on Problem Based
Learning (PBL) model, in which problems appear at the beginning of the learning sequence,
being introduced by the teacher and solved by the students.
Chapter 1 - Jobs for the Future—with its partners Eduventures and FutureWorks—was
asked by the U.S. Department of Labor to synthesize the research literature on the challenges
facing adult learners in higher education today and emerging strategies for increasing the
number of adults over 24 who earn college credentials and degrees. This synthesis is meant to
provide perspectives on key issues facing adults as more and more of them see the need for
higher education credentials, not just for shortterm training. The project has two phases: first,
this document, which is a broad, synthetic overview of the issues; and second, a more indepth
exploration of particular highvalue topics that will be agreed upon by the partners and
department personnel.
Powerful economic, demographic, and market trends are reshaping the landscape of
higher education, particularly for adults. Moreover, it is wise to ask how these trends might
affect its key constituencies: employers who depend on increasingly highly skilled employees
for their competitive success and growth; job seekers who need more than high school
credentials to succeed in the economy; and workers who may have to, or want to, transition to
new careers.
Chapter 2 - Technological advancement has broadened educational, employment and
training opportunities for students and adults with disabilities via distance education. Distance
x Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas
education is a prized tool of K-12 and higher education, and for vocational training and
research. Advancing knowledge of accessible technology and universal design concepts have
coincided with the distance education movement, and with a federal mandate for accessible
technology under Section 508 of the amended Rehabilitation Act. However, studies of
website accessibility and universal applications designed to deliver online learning question
whether these tools permit equal and effective participation by people with varying
disabilities. Other studies of accessible virtual knowledge communities suggest needed
improvements to advance the inclusion of people with disabilities in online collaborative
research and training initiatives. This chapter explores the implications of the growing
distance education movement for people with disabilities. First, a review the breadth of
distance learning initiatives and their benefits and challenges for learners with disabilities.
Second, a discussion on applicable learning theory and practice, and the relevant mandates of
U.S. disability laws. Third, evaluate likely compliance on distance learning activities with
disability law and propose best practices to support distance education programs for equal
access and opportunity in employment, education, and other areas by the widest number and
variety of people.
Chapter 3 - A growing body of research suggests a close relationship between children‘s
development of linguistic and cognitive skills, with language development and cognitive
development mutually influencing each other. This chapter considers childhood bilingualism
as a special case of language acquisition, with implications for the relationship between
linguistic and cognitive processes. Specifically, during bilingual language development,
language input is phonologically more complex and spans two languages instead of one.
Given language input that includes phonetic inventories and vocabulary from two languages
(consisting of two labels for most concepts), linguistic and cognitive implications for
bilingual development are discussed. In addition, the chapter considers the consequences of
simultaneous activation of two language systems and the juggling of two language codes in
children. It is suggested that bilingual children may have a higher cognitive processing load
during language use and learning, resulting in both linguistic and cognitive differences
compared with monolingual peers. Recent research on linguistic and cognitive differences
between monolingual and bilingual children is placed in the context of current theoretical
models of language learning, development, and processing. In particular,consider recent
findings from monolingual and bilingual children in the context of learnability theory (with
an emphasis on the influence of input complexity on linguistic / cognitive development), and
in light of usage-based accounts of language acquisition (with an emphasis on the
development of potentially different cognitive skills in monolinguals and bilinguals). The
conclusion is that childhood bilingualism provides a unique context for examining the
interaction between linguistic and cognitive mechanisms during development.
Chapter 4 - This paper discusses some topics that stem from recent contributions made by
the history, the philosophy, and the didactics of science and are considered relevant to the
introduction of the special relativity theory in secondary education, adopting a contextualized
approach from a historical, epistemological and pedagogical point of view.
The results of several investigations into the teaching and learning of the Special
Relativity Theory are presented. Also, a discussion of the pedagogical approach that is
considered appropriate to introduce this theory at high school level as well as how the
treatment given to the theory by the textbooks more commonly used by teachers can influence
their pedagogical decisions and the students‘ understanding of this topic.
Preface xi
The mental images of relevant concepts of Mechanics that Argentine secondary students
have and which are necessary in order to understand the Special Relativity Theory are
analyzed. The results seem to indicate that the examined group of students has not developed
appropriate schemata to solve problems in which these concepts are involved. Based on these
results, some objectives-obstacles that should be overcome by students in a classroom context
adopting a specific teaching proposal that will provide for the significant learning of the
special relativity theory.
An outline is offered of a teaching proposal to deal with the Special Relativity Theory in
Argentina at high school level. This proposal was designed within a theoretical framework
that takes into account epistemological, historical, psychological and pedagogical aspects
which are compatible among themselves.
Chapter 5 - According to the teleological view of nature before Darwin the function of an
organ determined its form, in other words organisms were considered to possess those organs
that would enable them to perform all necessary functions properly. Darwin‘s evolutionary
theory advanced the priority of form over function, in the sense that the existing variation and
the available forms within a population posed constraints to the possible performed functions.
Considering these, it is interesting that studies in conceptual development research have
shown that young children exhibit an intuitive teleological reasoning. Two different
explanations for its origin have been proposed which agree on that it is characteristic of
students‘ explanations for biological properties from very early in childhood. Since
teleological explanations both in pre-Darwinian times and in students‘ conceptual
development are based on preconceived plans or on the intention to achieve goals, in this
chapter the major types of teleology that Darwin‘s theory had to confront are described and
studies in conceptual development research focusing on intuitive teleological reasoning are
reviewed. Then particular implications for evolution education are discussed and it is
concluded that evolution instruction should focus on conceptual change from explanations
that presuppose design etiologies, which were popular in pre-Darwinian times and which also
predominate in students‘ intuitive reasoning, to explanations that presuppose consequence
etiologies which are compatible with Darwin‘s theory and current evolutionary biology.
Chapter 6 - Writing is a complex and cognitively demanding activity. It cannot be
performed as a sequence of discrete steps; it requires the simultaneous combination of several
strategies and the application of various mental resources. Writing is, therefore, both a
recursive and a dynamic process. To be successful, writers need an understanding of the
components of a quality text as well as knowledge of writing strategies that can be used to
shape and organize the writing process. In particular, writing competence requires appropriate
and self-regulated knowledge of strategies for planning what to write, and then revising what
has been written.
In this chapter, a review is presented of the recent research on the planning and revision
processes in writing in order to show the importance that these have in the development of
writing competence. Then, a description of the existing research is discussed, evaluating
strategy-focused intervention studies, to provide an overview of the nature of the
interventions programs and an indication of which have been most successful. In the second
part of the chapter, a description and summarized findings from (Torrance, Fidalgo, and
García, 2007; and Fidalgo, Torrance, and García, 2008). These studies move beyond existing
research by (a) exploring the effects of strategy focused instruction on students‘ writing
processes as well as on their written products and (b) demonstrating the long-term effects of
xii Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas
this kind of intervention. In a final section, the practical implications of this body of research
(both ours and others) are presented and suggestions for how lessons learned from this
research might be applied in the classroom.
Chapter 7 - Problem based learning (PBL) has been adopted widely in the education of
health professionals and is used in many disciplines worldwide including medicine, nursing,
midwifery and other medical allied professions. The rationale for a PBL based programme in
midwifery lies in the theoretical claims that it promotes the development of communication
skills, teamwork, sharing information, problem solving and developing independent
responsibility for learning (Wood 2003) and that it may increase retention of knowledge,
transfer of concepts and enhance intrinsic interest (Norman and Schmidt 1992).
PBL was initially developed in the 1980s in medical education in North America as it
was felt that classroom learning did not always transfer to the clinical setting. Early
developers of the approach felt that learning in small groups through the use of clinical
problems would make medical education more interesting and relevant to practice. In the
1990‘s an EBL based approach was adopted by schools of nursing and midwifery in the UK.
Since the main focus of midwifery practice is on normal, healthy pregnancy and birth, the
term enquiry based learning (EBL) has been used by many midwifery programmes, but PBL
is the most commonly used term in the literature and will be used in this chapter.
Chapter 8 - The information age has permeated all aspects of our everyday lives,
including health care. Patients and the general public are now better educated and have access
to vast amounts of medical knowledge, previously only available to health care professionals.
Concurrently, many patients want to play a larger role in their personal health care decisions.
Shared decision making (SDM) is a term used to describe the collaborative process by which
patients and their health care providers make medical decisions, and it is generally considered
to be the most preferred approach. In this chapter, a review of the current status of medical
decision making, highlight the challenges in trying to ensure that patients are empowered to
participant in their decisions, and discuss issues related to training health care providers to
assist and engage their patients in decision making. Also explored, is the opportunities that
SDM provides for improving health care in the future through better education,
communication, and exchange.
Chapter 9 - The most common Problem Based Learning (PBL) model is the one in which
problems appear at the beginning of the learning sequence, being introduced by the teacher
and solved by the students. This paper acknowledges a conception of PBL organized around
sets of problems formulated by the students from scenarios that may focus on a broad theme.
In such a PBL environment the teachers‘ key role is to select or develop scenarios that
can originate relevant problems from an educational point of view. Scenarios can be of
different types, ranging from the verbal to the image-based ones, and may induce different
problems, depending on the information they offer and the intriguing power they convey.
Bearing in mind the role of problems in a PBL sequence, the relationship between
problems and scenarios, and the fact that in traditional school settings students are hardly
given the opportunity to ask questions, two issues can be raised: are students able to formulate
relevant questions to be used for PBL purposes? How do different types of scenarios (texts,
comics and images) compare in terms of their potential to originate such questions? Are
teachers able to anticipate students‘ questions? What are the characteristics of the social
environment that better foster the formulation of high-level questions?
Preface xiii
Results from research carried out with teachers and lower and upper secondary school
students suggest that students can formulate high-level questions from diverse types of
scenarios and that teachers can anticipate them. As far as the social environment is concerned,
results indicate that the older students are, the less valuable group work is in terms of high
level questions induction, whatever the type of scenario. However, results are not conclusive
with regard to the comparative effectiveness of diverse types of scenarios in what concerns
their power to induce relevant questions for PBL purposes.
Chapter 10 - This study assessed the effect of advance organizers in music instruction.
Four intact beginning instrumental grade nine classes participated in the study. Nine musical
concepts were used as the dependent variables: instrumentation, counting, note values,
slurring, key signature, meter, dynamics, harmony, and dotted note. The treatment group and
the control group were pre-tested and post-tested on their knowledge of the musical concepts
before and after the advance organizer instruction was implemented. For performance and
listening skill acquisition, post-test only was administered as the participants were beginners
without prior instrumental experience. Findings indicate that the learning of three concepts -
instrumentation, dynamics and harmony – was significantly affected by the independent
variable. None of the performance or listening skills based on the nine concepts were
significantly affected by the advance organizer instruction.
Chapter 11 - The field of Cognitive Science is dispelling some of the classical myths or
misunderstandings about how the brain learns, how brain development may be impacting
language acquisition and cognition. This is an exciting time to be an educator and to see the
incredible possibilities for the latest theories of how the brain develops and evolves. No
longer are people settled with the idea that the brain is ‗fixed‘ by the time a person reaches the
age of three, that you must throw our hands up in despair if a child comes to school not ready
to learn. Through the most recent research,it is learned that our mind can be enriched,
expanded and developed toward several levels of sophistication. This article draws upon the
marriage of the latest brain based research and technologies as they provide implications for
early detection of cognition and learning difficulties. This new direction in knowledge about
the development of the brain has great potential for the education of our children and the
stability of our nation as a global competitor.
In: Recent Trends in Education ISBN 978-1-60741-795-8
Editor: Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas © 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Chapter 1
ABSTRACT
Technological advancement has broadened educational, employment and training
opportunities for students and adults with disabilities via distance education. Distance
education is a prized tool of K-12 and higher education, and for vocational training and
research. Advancing knowledge of accessible technology and universal design concepts
have coincided with the distance education movement, and with a federal mandate for
accessible technology under Section 508 of the amended Rehabilitation Act. However,
studies of website accessibility and universal applications designed to deliver online
learning question whether these tools permit equal and effective participation by people
with varying disabilities. Other studies of accessible virtual knowledge communities
William N. Myhill, Senior Research Associate, BBI, Adjunct Professor of Law, Syracuse University; Deepti
Samant, Research Associate, BBI; Shelley Kaplan, Director, BBI Southeast DBTAC; María Verónica Reina,
Senior Research Associate, BBI; Peter Blanck, Chair, BBI, University Professor, Syracuse University.
http://bbi.syr.edu/
David Klein, Director of Technology, Law, Health Policy & Disability Center
This research was funded, in part, by grants to Dr. Blanck from the U.S. Department of Education, National
Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research (NIDRR) for i) ―Rehabilitation Research and Training
Center (RRTC) on Workforce Investment and Employment Policy for Persons with Disabilities,‖ Grant No.
H133B980042-99; ii) ―IT Works,‖ Grant No. H133A011803; iii) ―Demand Side Employment Placement
Models,‖ Grant No. H133A060033; iv) ―Technology for Independence: A Community-Based Resource
Center,‖ Grant No. H133A021801; and v) ―Southeast Disability & Business Technical Assistance Center,‖
Grant No. H133A060094.
2 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
INTRODUCTION
This chapter explores the implications of the growing distance education movement for
people with disabilities. Technological advancement has broadened educational, employment
and training opportunities for students and adults with disabilities and nontraditional students
via distance education (Ex-Coach, 2006; Keller, 2006; Southeast DBTAC, 2005; Watson and
Ryan, 2006). Distance education rapidly is becoming a prized tool of K-12 and higher
education (Austin, 2007; Murray, 2006; Ryman, 2005; TSCandU, 2007a), and for vocational
training (Keller, 2006; TSCandU, 2007b). There is growing popularity in strictly distance
learning degree and certification programs in large part for their convenience (Mehta, 2007;
TSCandU, 2007c). Enactment of the No Child Left Behind Act (2001) spurred K-12
educators to find innovative and alternative means of improving student learning and
opportunities for the least successful children and those in rural or under-resourced areas
(Boseman Public Schools, n.d.; Cognos, 2004; GAO, 2004; Hasten, 2004; Myhill, 2004).
Distance education has become a popular approach (Watson and Ryan, 2006). Moreover,
distance education is proving to be a money maker for many educational programs and the
information and communications technology firms that serve them (Mintz, 2004).
Advancing knowledge of accessible technology and universal design concepts has
coincided with the distance education movement, and with a federal mandate for accessible
technology under Section 508 of the amended Rehabilitation Act (Blanck, Hill, Siegel, and
Waterstone, 2004). However, studies of website accessibility and applications designed to
deliver online learning question whether these tools permit equal and effective participation
by people with varying disabilities (NCD, 2006). A 2006-2007 study by Myhill, Cogburn,
Samant, Addom, and Blanck (in press) of accessible cyberinfrastructure-enabled knowledge
communities suggests needed improvements to advance the inclusion of people with
disabilities in online collaborative research and training initiatives.
In this chapter, first, the authors review the varieties and uses of distance education
programs and their implications for learners with disabilities. Second, we explore emerging
learning theory and practice for delivering distance education, best practices for instructing
learners with disabilities, and their civil rights to K-12 and higher education. Third, we
evaluate current distance education compliance with disability law and propose policy
initiatives and best practices for distance education programs that ensure equal access and
opportunity for learners with and without disabilities.
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 3
Distance education has entered into nearly every imaginable realm of formal and
informal education, training, and certification. Traditional (i.e., brick and mortar) and virtual
(i.e., online only) colleges and universities offer both matriculated courses and classes for
professional development (Jung, Galyon-Keramidas, Collins, and Ludlow, 2006; NCES,
2003). Public and private companies and non-profits that specialize in a particular field, such
as law, teacher education, business and research skills, offer workshops and courses to meet
professional development (e.g., continuing legal education, Java enterprise development
certification, human subjects training), advancement, and other criteria (Lawline.com, 2006;
SkillSoft, 2007; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, n.d.). For instance, software
application skill sets required for jobs in administrative support, publishing, accounting,
payroll, software design, web design, human resources, and innumerable other positions
frequently require certification in the use of specific applications (SkillSoft, 2007).
State and local education agencies, including private and charter schools offering K-12
programs, provide standard curriculum and advanced placement credit via distance learning
(Watson and Ryan, 2006). While most K-12 distance programs arise from brick and mortar
schools, new programs are beginning to offer only a virtual experience. Trade and technical
schools that prepare students to be a dental assistant, paralegal, medical transcriptionist, home
inspector, or real estate salesperson among numerous other careers, offer online study and
examination leading to professional certification and licensure (TSCandU, 2007b). These
programs, occasionally free, serve nearly any educational or training purpose that a student,
school, employer, or public agency may have.
In this section we review the technologies involved in distance learning, possible
instructional arrangements, and the purposes or goals of varying distance learning initiatives.
4 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
We find these three factors are intertwined significantly, with important implications for
students with disabilities.
a. Web-based Learning
Web-based learning tools can be distinguished, in part, on the basis of their time of
occurrence, that is, synchronously or asynchronously (Bafoutsou and Mentzas, 2002).
Asynchronous learning opportunities are those where the human facilitator of learning does
not interact with the learner in real time, and commonly include websites and email (Codone,
2004). Designing web pages that host course content is a simpler means of providing
asynchronous learning opportunities on the World Wide Web (―Web‖). Such ―static web-
based educational‖ methodology may include linked HTML pages, presentations, and
documents, among others, in a variety of formats (Codone, 2004; Poindexter and Heck,
1999).
Web-based courses offer a range of asynchronous services such as email, electronic
bulletin boards, discussion forums, content management systems, mailing groups and
Listservs (Hiltz and Wellman, 1997; Maher, 1999; Watson and Ryan, 2006; Zhang, Zhao,
Zhou and Nunamaker, 2004). Several commercially available web-based course management
tools, such as the Blackboard suite of products, WebCT,1 and Lotus Notes, facilitate the
delivery of course content and student submissions in multiple formats (Lewis, MacEntee and
1 Blackboard and WebCT, previously supplied by independent companies, have merged under the Blackboard
(2005) banner.
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 5
Youns-Maher, 2002; Storey, Phillips, Maczewski and Wang, 2002). These tools include a
range of features allowing instructors to make content materials available, such as posting
linked HTML documents, and uploading documents and presentations in multiple formats
(e.g., Word, PDF, PowerPoint) (Lewis, et al.). In addition, they facilitate student assessment
activities through the capability of developing online quizzes and tests, conducting student
surveys, supporting assignment and paper submissions, and providing comprehensive
gradebook tools (Blackboard, Inc., 2004; Storey, et al.). Most course management tools, such
as Blackboard, allow instructors to set up online and face-to-face class discussions promoting
interaction between students, and include multi-media capabilities that offer an instructor
broad flexibility in the organization of a virtual classroom (Babbitt, 2003).
Open source applications increasingly provide content management systems where
students can collaborate asynchronously. For instance, two IT Works2 research projects at the
Burton Blatt Institute—which address the i) design of accessible open source business
applications and ii) effective accommodations for employees with disabilities in media
industries—use Google Docs and Spreadsheets (2007) to share and edit common evolving
documents (e.g., methods, findings) among the eight graduate student researchers and the
Project Director. This enhances regular faculty supervision, feedback, and direction of student
work.
2
IT Works is funded by the National Institute for Disability and Rehabilitation Research (U.S. Department of
Education), Grant No. H133A011803.
6 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
Similarly, instant messaging programs are embedded into course management tools such as
Blackboard (2004) and web conferencing tools such as Elluminate Live! (Elluminate, 2006).
Recently academic libraries have started offering instant messaging reference services in
addition to email and web-based assistance (Foley, 2002).
c. Virtual Reality
Virtual reality simulations, traditionally delivered through desktop applications with the
help of special devices such as goggles and gloves, immerse the user in a 3-d virtual world
(Samant, Myhill, and Blanck, 2006). Virtual reality simulations also can be delivered
remotely to students who cannot be in the physical location of the equipment (Park, et al.,
2001). Tele-immersion applications seek to merge audio and video conferencing with virtual
reality environments (Leigh, 1999), to provide a collaborative space for individuals in remote
locations to interact and work with each other in virtual worlds (Mortensen, et al., 2002).
Motion capture avatars and annotations are capable of recording each user‘s head and hand
gestures, full body motion and voice, and other users can view these avatars using their own
immersive technology (Lee, Ghyme, Park, and Wohn, 1998; Mortensen et al. 2002). This
allows multiple users, in remote locations, to interact and work with each other through their
avatars.
These tools are being used in several distance learning programs. For instance, the
University of Illinois at Chicago and Central Missouri State University held a collaborative
class in which students were introduced to the culture and people of Harlem through a virtual
reality application called ―Virtual Harlem‖ (Park, et al., 2001). Students recorded their
opinions and actions in virtual reality through annotations that were saved and could be
retrieved later. Different exercises such as allowing a group of students to explore Virtual
Harlem together and allowing others to observe their actions through their avatars, permitted
students at different locations to explore this recreation of Harlem in a collaborative manner.
Disability and Technical Assistance Center (2007) funded by the National Institute on
Disability and Rehabilitation Research (U.S. Department of Education) offers a variety of
online educational tools largely targeting employers and business owners and providing
continuing education credits in matters regarding rights and responsibilities under the
Americans with Disabilities Act.
Notably, in 2004 the U.S. Department of Education published its policy
recommendations for use of the Internet and advancing technologies to improve public
education. Among the recommendations was ―Support E-Learning and Virtual Schools,‖
wherein the Department acknowledged:
In the past five years there has been significant growth in organized online instruction (e-
learning) and ―virtual‖ schools, making it possible for students at all levels to receive high
quality supplemental or full courses of instruction personalized to their needs. Traditional
schools are turning to these services to expand opportunities and choices for students and
professional development for teachers. Recommendations for states, districts and schools
include:
Watson and Ryan‘s (2006) comprehensive review of K-12 distance education initiatives
found 38 states have adopted ―state-led online learning programs, significant policies
regulating online education, or both.‖ (p. 6). These programs take many forms, including 1) a
unit of the state education agency or board of education (e.g., Idaho Digital Learning
Academy, Illinois Virtual High School), 2) an independent entity created by the state (e.g.,
Colorado Online Learning), 3) a separate school district (e.g., Florida Virtual School), 4) a
unit of a state university (e.g., University of California College Prep, 5) cooperatives that
operate in multiple states (and countries) with membership in the hundreds of schools,3 6)
state led yet privately funded schools (e.g., Louisiana Virtual School), 7) and multiple charter
schools (such as in Minnesota, Kansas, and Pennsylvania), among others (Watson and Ryan,
2006). They also range significantly in size, such as from 600 students (Hawaii E-School) to
68,000 students (Florida Virtual School) (Watson and Ryan).
Business skills training courses, such as the more than 2,000 offered by SkillSoft (2007),
range comprehensively across the IT, business, desktop, legal compliance, environmental and
occupational safety, and financial skill areas, and are among the more well-established and
lucrative distance education initiatives. Rapidly growing is the number of providers offering
college degree programs solely via the web (TSCandU, 2007a), however, on occasion raising
questions of quality. The University of Phoenix, a network of small brick and mortar
campuses in 39 states, though doing the majority of its teaching online, is in federal court
3
Virtual High School, Inc. (2007) has a membership of 457 schools, including one-third of Massachusetts‘ high
schools, and serves over 9,000 students worldwide (Watson & Ryan, 2006).
8 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
The Internet is the most vital tool to emerge in the last 50 years for enhancing the lives of
people with disabilities. It offers unprecedented access to information and services,
overcoming many of the obstacles that people with disabilities previously experienced. It
should be easier to shop online than choose clothes from a retail outlet that you can‘t see. It
ought to be easier to bank online than manoeuvre a wheelchair up the steps to the bank
building. It must be possible for people with disabilities to get online, because otherwise
society will suffer (United Nations, 2006a, pp. 19-20).
For the more than 20 million working age adults with disabilities, 15 million children
with disabilities, and the millions of students attending any one of the 10,793 public schools
(11.9% of all public schools) failing to make ―adequate yearly progress‖ for two consecutive
years,4 distance education may offer hope that an affordable choice or alternative is available
to facilitate their academic, technical, or professional achievement necessary to live
independent and self-determined lives (Blanck and Myhill, in press; NEA, 2006; RRTC,
2005). In 2001, 48% of U.S. two- and four-year colleges and universities (both public and
private) received requests for accommodations by persons with disabilities seeking to take
their distance education courses. Larger and public institutions received more requests than
medium/smaller and private institutions, respectively (NCES, 2003).
Individuals with disabilities may have the most to gain from effective distance learning
opportunities. Research consistently finds fewer successful outcomes for children with
disabilities as they age through K-12 education and transition into the adult world. For
instance, just 50% of all students with disabilities served under the Individuals with
Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) graduate from high school (U.S. Department of Education,
2006). Transition planning, mandated by the IDEA for students ages 16 and older who
receive special education services (20 U.S.C.A. § 1414(d)(1)(A)(i)(VIII), 2005), frequently
lacks relevance, is poorly implemented or is ineffective (NCD, 2000a; 2000b). During the two
years following high school graduation, 5.7% of students with disabilities attend a four-year
college compared to 28.3% of their peers without disabilities (Wagner, Newman, Cameto,
Garza, and Levine, 2005). Educational achievement and rates of employment for people with
disabilities remain low compared to people without disabilities. For instance, in 2004 people
with disabilities were less than half as likely to earn a bachelors degree (12.7% vs. 29.8%)
and to be employed (38.3% vs. 78.6%) (RRTC, 2005). Moreover, following secondary
4
Adequate Yearly Progress is determined by applying state-defined ―high standards of academic achievement‖ that
are ―statistically valid and reliable‖ to evaluate whether all students and specific sub-groups of students (e.g.,
economically disadvantaged students, major racial and ethnic groups, students with disabilities, and English
language learners) are making ―continuous and substantial improvement.‖ (20 U.S.C.A § 6311(b)(2)(C),
2005).
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 9
education, services for people with disabilities become fragmented, significantly diminish in
scope and availability, overly target low paying jobs, and pose social and physical barriers
(Gill, 2005; NCD, 2003; Paul, 2000; Rao, 2004; Zaslow, 2005).
Much like school choice and voucher programs (i.e., other alternative edicational tools
with rising popularity), distance education is susceptible to significant criticisms and
drawbacks (Myhill, 2004). Both present concern with 1) inconsistent or unproven
effectiveness and accountability, 2) intentional or inadvertent ―creaming‖ (i.e., selection of
students), 3) the absence of teacher preparation and professional development standards for
the unique environments of distance education, and 4) the national shortage of highly trained
special education teachers (Myhill; Watson and Ryan, 2006).
Barriers to the accessibility of technologies are caused largely by three categories of
problems: technical, design, and intrapersonal barriers (Wimberly, Reed, and Morris, 2004).
Technical barriers occur because of either limitations in hardware (e.g., lack of computer
memory) or because of a user‘s lack of knowledge about a technology's usage (e.g., no
alternative strategies when an application does not work as expected). Design barriers occur
when applications lack design characteristics that will make information accessible. For
example, the most common barrier to university web pages is the lack of alternative text for
images (Schmetzke, 2001). Intrapersonal barriers occur when the learning environment does
not meet the needs of individual learner characteristics. For example, a two-hour web
conference may tax the stamina of a person with chronic fatigue syndrome.
Barriers inherent in the design of distance learning technologies specifically may exclude
individuals with varying impairments from success in these programs. We have found that
―persons with vision, hearing, fine motor, or cognitive impairments, and learning or attention
difficulties … experience the greatest barriers to effective communication when technologies
demand multi-sensory interaction (e.g., unimpaired hearing, vision, attention, and fine motor
skills), or permit limited forms of input/interaction (e.g., speech without closed captioning, or
mouse without keyboard access) (Myhill, et al., in press). Similarly, a 2006 report sponsored
by the United Nations (2006a) concluded there is global failure to provide the most basic
level of web accessibility to people with disabilities. Of the 100 websites used in the study,
selected from travel, finance, media, politics, and retail websites in 20 countries, most did not
meet the World Wide Web Consortium (W3C) ―Web Content Accessibility Guidelines 1.0‖
(United Nations; W3C, 1999), widely regarded as the premier international standard (W3C,
2006).
Another study by investigators with the Southeast Disability and Business Technical
Assistance Center (2006) identified four factors impacting the distance learning experiences
of college students with disabilities: instructor characteristics, learner characteristics, design
barriers, and factors affecting systems change (e.g., key stakeholder buy-in). One project
found that many students with disabilities, specifically students with learning disabilities, had
limited computer skills (Southeast DBTAC). Similarly, instructors differ significantly in their
level of computer literacy. Investigators at four universities discovered that many faculty
members had limited experience using a computer, let alone with designing accessible web
pages or online courses (Southeast DBTAC; University of Florida, 2007).
Faculty and staff instructors may have little or no familiarity with the access needs of
students with disabilities. At Blue Ridge Community College, a majority of instructors
attending a workshop on web accessibility did not grasp even the most basic concepts of web
accessibility (Southeast DBTAC, 2006). Similarly, when 98 individuals representing higher
10 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
education institutions in eight Southeast states attended two one-day conferences on web
accessibility, the conferences provided 90% of them with their first exposure to the
information access and technology needs of students with disabilities—despite the fact that
almost 90% of participants were disability services coordinators, specialists, or directors
(Southeast DBTAC). Another project discovered that many instructors had no knowledge or
experience with students who had disabilities, nor did they have any idea how to provide
reasonable accommodations within the classroom, in online courses, or in testing (Blue Ridge
Community College, 2004).
Direct one-for-one transfer of course materials into online content may create significant
access barriers or fail to use the Internet in ways to enhance access to resources otherwise
unavailable to students with disabilities (Southeast DBTAC, 2006). Investigators at East
Tennessee Technology Access Center found online textbooks difficult to locate or different
from course content, and observed that students with physical disabilities encountered online
communication barriers with instructors. Obtaining buy-in from key educational
administrators and other key stakeholders is a significant challenge to the successful adoption
and implementation of policies and practices to ensure the accessibility of distance learning
opportunities (Southeast DBTAC).
Social learning theories have had a profound effect on online learning. In these
theoretical frameworks, learning is not perceived as the acquisition of a static repository of a
domain of information. Instead, it is perceived as the product of individual experience as
constructed from immersion in a culture (Schenker and Scadden, 2002). The ability to create
a knowledge structure, to define problems, and to locate needed information to address those
problems are more valuable skills for students than learning a static database of information.
The teacher is no longer the repository and source of information. A teacher's responsibility,
then, is less as a lecturer and more as a coach, who supports and encourages students
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 11
throughout their learning process (Bruner, 1996). As Brown and Gray (1995) state, ―Learning
is less about absorbing information than it is about becoming a part of a community.‖ (p. 78).
2. Computer-Mediated Communication
According to Tu (2005), computer-mediated communication (CMC) provides a platform
upon which a community‘s knowledge structure can exist. To achieve successful
communities of practice, three critical areas of collaborative technologies should be
considered: personalization, digitization, and interactivity. Personalization describes how
individuals selectively present themselves in an online environment. CMC technologies allow
12 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
varying levels of participation and types of technologies. Learners can connect briefly and
frequently using mobile technologies or for long durations using a standard wired Internet
connection. Unique CMC factors that affect personalization include 1) synchronicity versus
asynchronicity, where learners can communicate in real time or according to their own
schedules, and 2) communication channels, which include text, audio, and video (Tu).
Personalization allows people with disabilities to present themselves in ways in which
they feel comfortable (Tu, 2005). For instance, people with sensory impairments, who may
take longer than others to access, process, and respond to information, can use asynchronous
communication to allow themselves time to collaborate with others comfortably. Learners
with visual impairments can use a screen reader to access asynchronous, text-based
information from a bulletin board. Learners with learning disabilities who may not be able to
respond quickly in writing to others in a learning community can take the time to edit their
writing, use a spell checker, and submit their response asynchronously, or they may choose
the use of a live video stream synchronously to speak their ideas, rather than rely on the
written word.
Digitization of content allows higher quality information to be available at increasingly
faster transmission speeds. Digitization continually increases access to information in new
ways (Tu, 2005). Higher quality audio and video are becoming more available in their
creation and distribution, and access to this information is now possible via wireless and
handheld devices, making quality information ubiquitously available to more individuals. For
people with disabilities, improvements in digital technologies help the conversion of quality
information to different sensory modalities and provide easy ways to create and access this
information (Tu). Voice recognition systems increasingly are more robust, allowing people to
convert their words to digital text accurately and with less effort. Realtime captioning is more
easily provided to users of video chat or conference rooms to enable individuals with hearing
impairments to participate actively in synchronous classrooms.
Interactivity provides learners the ability to engage in real collaboration in learning
environments through two-way communication. A learning environment affords four types of
interaction: learner-instructor, learner-content, learner-learner, and learner-interface (Tu,
2005). This interactivity allows learners to provide input into the knowledge base of the
learning environment, which is stored and made available to others for further interactivity
and for reference. The interactive processes of sharing, retrieving, validating, and managing
both new and archived knowledge (Tu) create an environment where the community
synergistically helps its members acquire useful and meaningful information and skills.
always accessible and can be used universally without the use of assistive technologies
(Mace, 2007; Seale, 2007).
Thus, universally designed technologies provide a much higher likelihood that they can
be used efficiently and effectively by a diverse population. For building a community, where
all individuals feel included and are full participants, adapted and accessible designs are based
on the assumption that at least two different populations are present, those with disabilities
(the abnormal) and those without (the normal), which can create a stigmatizing effect. On the
other hand, a universally designed technology assumes a single, continuous, but diverse
community, representing different characteristics and abilities (Iwarsson and Stahl, 2003).
The equalizing effect of this assumption promotes equal participation among participants in a
community of practice.
Landmark disability civil rights laws in the United States ushered in new eras of rights
for people with disabilities, and the recognition that ―[d]isability is a natural part of the human
experience [that] in no way diminishes the right of individuals to participate in or contribute
to society.‖ (20 U.S.C.A. §1400(c)(1), 2005). Moreover, people with disabilities have the
right to enjoy independent and self-determined lives, ―pursue meaningful careers, [and] enjoy
full inclusion and integration in the economic, political, social, cultural, and educational
mainstream of American society ….‖ (29 U.S.C. § 701(3), 2000).
Nearly three decades after the earliest of these laws (i.e., the Rehabilitation Act of 1973),
in passing the Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 (ADA), Congress found that
individuals with disabilities are a discrete and insular minority who have been faced with
restrictions and limitations, subjected to a history of purposeful unequal treatment, and
relegated to a position of political powerlessness in our society, based on characteristics that
are beyond [their] control … and resulting from stereotypic assumptions not truly indicative
of the individual ability … to participate in, and contribute to, society. (42 U.S.C. §
12101(a)(7), 2000).
These laws, however, did not foresee the technological advances of the Internet, or the
dramatic increase in distance education initiatives via the Internet. Not until the 1998
amendments to the Rehabilitation Act did laws seriously contemplate Internet accessibility. In
this section, we review applicable U.S. federal and state laws, and the United Nations 2006
Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities.
Barriers take many forms and impede not merely physical access (e.g., a hotel room or
public restroom), but access to meaningful communication (e.g., telephone, television, email,
or lecture), participation (e.g., in a classroom, board room, or parent-teacher or community
association meeting), and benefit of programs and services (e.g., enrolling for social security
benefits, healthcare coverage, or university courses) (Myhill, et al., in press; 42 U.S.C. §
12101, 2000).
ADA Title II requires state and local governments to ensure they do not exclude qualified
persons with disabilities from their programs, services, and benefits by reason of disability
(42 U.S.C. § 12132, 2000). For instance, to ensure effective communication, Title II was
found to require the accessibility of state and local government web sites. In Martin v.
Metropolitan Atlanta Rapid Transit Authority (2002), a federal district court in Georgia
concluded that bus and transit service scheduling information on the MARTA website must
be accessible to persons who are blind. When a student with a disability ―meets the essential
eligibility requirements‖ for participation in or receipt of the services of state- or locally-
operated universities, colleges and trade schools, the school cannot turn the student away (§§
12131(2)-12132). Moreover, in a letter from Adriana Cardenas in the Office for Civil Rights
(OCR), to the President of California State University at Los Angeles, Cardenas indicated that
Title II:
requires a public college to take appropriate steps to ensure that communications with
persons with disabilities ‗are as effective as communications with others‘ …. OCR has
repeatedly held that the term ‗communication‘ in this context means the transfer of
information, including (but not limited to) the verbal presentation of a lecture, the printed text
of a book, and the resources of the Internet. (U.S. Department of Education, 1997; see also 28
C.F.R. § 35.160(a), 2006).
ADA (Arizona Revised Statutes, § 41-1492.06, 2006; Virginia Code Annotated, § 51.5-46,
2006). The majority of states have not incorporated the ADA within their statutes.
The mandate of § 504 of the Rehabilitation Act, which prohibits the same discriminatory
conduct by ―any program or activity receiving Federal financial assistance,‖ (29 U.S.C. § 794,
2000) also may be invoked as Title II institutions of higher education commonly receive
federal funds for student aid, and in support of Pell and research grants (Miller v. Abilene
Christian University of Dallas, 1981; see also Tyndall v. National Education Center of
California, 1993). Likewise, Title II and § 504 are applicable to the programs and services of
public schools (K-12), that is, Title II as a local government agency and § 504 as a recipient
of Federal IDEA funds (Sandison v. Michigan High School Athletic Association, Inc., 1994).
Qualified students with disabilities may not be denied equal access to and the benefit of these
programs and services.
Private colleges, universities, trade schools, and businesses may not deny a student with a
disability the full and equal enjoyment of their services and facilities on the basis of disability
(42 U.S.C. § 12182(b)(1)(A)(i), 2000). ADA Title III requires that ―public accommodations‖
(e.g., cinemas, department stores, restaurants, and other entities with operations that affect
commerce) ensure that students with disabilities have ―full and equal enjoyment of the goods,
services, facilities, privileges, advantages, or accommodations.‖ (§ 12182(a)). Title III entities
specifically include private nursery schools, elementary and secondary schools, colleges and
―other place[s] of education.‖ (§ 12181(7)(J); Guckenberger v. Boston University, 1997).
Title III discrimination includes the ―failure to make reasonable modifications in policies,
practices, or procedures‖ to accommodate a student with a disability, unless the school
demonstrates that modifications would ―fundamentally alter‖ the nature of their services (§
12182(b)(2)(A)(iii)).
Although it is not settled U.S. law whether public accommodations offering services via
the Web are subject to ADA requirements, the evolving majority position indicates persons
with disabilities cannot be excluded from their web-based services if the Title III entity has a
nexus to a permanent physical location (Blanck, et al., 2004). The Eleventh Circuit Court of
Appeals, for instance, found that Congress intended ADA Title III to apply to public
accommodations with a ―physical concrete‖ presence and not solely a virtual presence
(Access Now, Inc. v. Southwest Airlines, 2002, p. 1319).
legislation directing the Indiana Office of Technology (IOT) to develop standards that comply
with § 508, and which apply to all branches of state and local government (Indiana Code, § 4-
13.1-3(1)(a), (d), 2006). The standards developed require the accessibility of ―IT equipment,
software and services‖ including ―all web pages hosted by or for the state.‖ (IOT, 2005, p.1).
Similarly, the State of North Carolina enacted the Persons with Disabilities Protection
Act, which prohibits state and local government from denying ―the full and equal enjoyment
of the goods, services, facilities, privileges, advantages, and accommodations … on the basis
of a disabling condition.‖ (North Carolina General Statutes, § 168A-6, 2006). Statute further
directs the Office of the State Chief Information Officer (OSCIO) to develop and implement
standards for web portals that ―allow persons to access State government services on a
24-hour basis.‖ (North Carolina General Statutes, § 66-58.20(a)). The OSCIO (2005), in turn,
produced Standard 2.2.1 requiring state government full compliance with Priority 1 of the
World Wide Web Consortium‘s Web Content Accessibility Guidelines, and Practice 2.1.9
recommending that EandIT is accessible ―to the broadest possible range of users and
compatible with a wide range of assistive technologies.‖
5
Formerly the Education for All Handicapped Children Act (1975), the Act was renamed the Individuals with
Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) in 1990 (Katsiyannis, Yell, & Bradley, 2001). The Individuals with
Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004 is the latest reauthorization of the IDEA.
6
Health impairments are ―chronic or acute health problems‖ causing ―limited strength, vitality or alertness‖ and
which ―adversely affect … educational performance.‖ (34 C.F.R § 300.8(c)(9), 2006).
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 17
needs; 2) articulates measurable goals and short-term objectives to remediate the needs; 3)
designates specialized services, modifications, supports, and supplementary aids to implement
the goals and objectives; and 4) determines the least restrictive environment(s) (LRE) in
which services will be provided (Myhill). Online distance learning services provided by state
or local education agencies to students receiving special education services must conform to
the individual child‘s IEP. Yet distance learning may pose significant accessibility barriers.
Moreover, such services run afoul of the FAPE mandate if inaccessibility impedes
educational benefit or imposes costs on the family such as having to purchase assistive
technologies, or if the services remove the student from the least restrictive or impose a more
restrictive environment.
7
One hundred nations have signed the Convention, however, only Jamaica has ratified the Convention. United
Nations (2007).
18 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
recreation, leisure and sports, outlines the obligation to ensure intellectual property laws do
not constitute an unreasonable or discriminatory barrier for people with disabilities when
accessing cultural materials (Art. 30, § 1(c)). Removing such barriers often involves the use
of ICT devices. For instance, digitalizing printed materials is an effective way to provide
access to copyrighted texts for people with visual disabilities (Art. 21(b)). Finally, Article 32,
§ 1(d), which focuses on international cooperation, advocates the provision of technical and
economic assistance where appropriate, such as encouraging the sharing of technologies to
connect persons with disabilities with accessible and assistive technologies.
The implementation of the Convention in the realm of ICT will face numerous
challenges, as indicated by a large gap between today‘s accessibility difficulties and the goals
established by the Convention. The majority of the potential State Parties to the Convention
have developed programs addressing a variety of disability issues contained in its provisions
with relative success, but only a few have adopted and enforced accessible design standards
for ICT (UNESCO, 2005).
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 19
Students with disabilities are a diverse population with varying abilities, interests,
impairments and skills such as information and computer literacy. As such, students with
disabilities have significantly differing needs and require individualized methods for equal
and accessible distance learning opportunities. Investigators at the Southeast DBTAC (2005)
concluded that effective distance learning opportunities for students with disabilities is less
about their abilities, however, and more about the accessibility of the course itself.
Facilitating successful academic achievements for children with disabilities, and
transition, post-secondary education, training, and employment for adults with disabilities
requires specialized instructional methods, strategies, materials, modifications and
accommodations, highly qualified special education teachers for K-12, effective transition
planning, and instructors/faculty with the will and way to meet individual learner needs. We
review the range of needs, and best practices for meeting the needs, in general terms,
beginning with children, followed by transition, and adults with disabilities.
1. Children
The IEP, and the goals, strategies, arrangements, and modifications necessary to
implement the IEP, are essential for meeting the unique individual needs of each child with a
disability. Here is a glimpse at a fragment of the considerations necessary in preparing the
IEP, in light of the diversity of children served under the IDEA:
Participating in a small group discussion for the child who is deaf; hearing
instructions from the physical education teacher above the din of the gymnasium for
a child using a hearing aid;
Comprehending grade level reading assignments for the child with a learning
disability in reading; preparing written reports for the child with a learning disability
in written expression;
Calculating large numbers or measuring angles for the child who is blind; following
the band conductor‘s directing for a child with low vision;
Managing impulsive outbursts for the child with traumatic brain injury or a serious
emotional disturbance;
Measuring distances and volumes for the child with dramatic Tourette‘s based tics;
Maintaining focus for instructions, reading, test taking, and other tasks, amid typical
classroom distractions, for the child with Attention Deficit Disorder;
Making a class presentation for a child with a speech impairment;
Going on a class fieldtrip to a unique and remote geological area for a child with an
orthopedic impairment caused by Cerebral Palsy;
Partnering on projects with other students for a child with Asperger‘s Syndrome.8
Research demonstrates that the least restrictive environment (LRE) and applicable best
practice for the vast majority of children with disabilities is to provide their educational
8
The lead author, formerly a special education teacher (1989–2001), regularly addressed these considerations by
facilitating the development and implementation of IEPs for hundreds of children with diverse disabilities.
20 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
services in inclusive classrooms (Frattura and Capper, 2006; Jenkins, Antil, Wayne, and
Vadasy, 2003; McLeskey and Waldron, 2007; Mortweet, Utley, Walker, Dawson, Delquadri,
Reddy, et al., 1999; NCD, 1994; Rea, McLaughlin, and Walther-Thomas, 2002; Waldron and
McLeskey, 1998). Inclusion refers to the provision of specialized educational services to a
child with a disability in the general education classroom (Waldron and McLeskey; Rea, et
al.). Some children require a more individualized service for a part of the school day, such as
physical therapy, counseling, or direct intensive reading instruction in a one-to-one (more
restrictive) setting (NCD, 1996; Vaughn and Linan-Thompson, 2003).
Additional evidence-based practices include 1) instructional strategies, such as small
group instruction, modeling and strategy training, teaching metacognitive strategies, peer
tutoring, cooperative learning, direct instruction, and functional instruction; 2) progress
monitoring and curriculum-based measurement; and 3) behavioral techniques, such as
precision requests, response costs, behavior momentum, self monitoring, and applied
behavior analysis (Browder and Cooper-Duffy, 2003; Cook and Schirmer, 2003; Deno, 2003;
Jenkins, et al., 2003; Landrum, Tankersley and Kauffman, 2003; Mortweet, et al., 1999;
Vaughn and Linan-Thompson, 2003). We discuss a few of these practices as applicable in
Part III (infra).
The highly qualified special education teacher is an essential member of the team that
develops the IEP, oversees its implementation, documents the child‘s progress, and reports
back to the team (Browder and Cooper-Duffy, 2003; Myhill, 2004; Vaughn and Linan-
Thompson, 2003). Since passage of the NCLB, this is one who 1) has full state certification
or licensure as a special education teacher; 2) has at minimum a bachelor‘s degree (20
U.S.C.A. § 1401(10)(B)(iii), 2005); and 3) meets applicable state requirements or
demonstrates competence in the core subject matters s/he teaches to children with special
needs (§ 1401(10)(B),(D)). Teachers new to the profession further must demonstrate subject
matter and teaching skill competence via passing a ―rigorous‖ state assessment, typically
leading to certification or licensure (20 U.S.C.A. § 7801(23)(B), 2005).
2. Transition
Transition services are ―a coordinated set of activities for a [student] with a disability‖
that are ―focused on improving [her/his] academic and functional achievement … to facilitate
… movement from school to post-school activities, including post-secondary education,
vocational education, integrated employment … , continuing and adult education, adult
services, independent living, or community participation.‖ (20 U.S.C.A. § 1401(34)(A),
2005). The services must be based on the student‘s needs, ―taking into account … strengths,
preferences, and interests; and includ[ing] instruction, related services, community
experiences, the development of employment and other post-school adult living objectives,
and, when appropriate, acquisition of daily living skills and functional vocational evaluation.‖
(§ 1401(34)(B)-(C)). Transition services must begin no later than age 16 and are developed as
part of the IEP (§ 1414(d)(1)(A)(i)(VIII)).
Effective transition services are essential for developing the skill base and confidence
necessary to live a productive, successful, and independent life (Center for Workers with
Disabilities, 2006). Student-centered planning, vocational assessment, parental involvement,
knowledge of legal rights, responsibilities and community resources, developing social and
self-advocacy skills, and a personal understanding of strengths, weaknesses and needs are
important components of effective services (Center for Workers with Disabilities;
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 21
Connecticut Transition Task Force, 2001; Kohler and Field, 2003). Among evidence-based
and highly promising practices for effective transition are
3. Adults
The primary aim of education and training for adults has become learning how to learn.
Research on adult education in the last two decades indicates career and vocational educators
place a over-emphasis on discrete job skills needed in the prevailing job market, which are
likely to be obsolete in a decade (Allsopp, Minskoff, and Bolt, 2005; Sizoo, Agrusa, and
Iskat, 2005). More importantly, adults must develop the skills to learn new skill sets, and be
lifelong learners, as employers and the job market will demand their willingness and ability to
adapt (Sizoo, et al.). The Learning and Study Strategies Inventory (LASSI) is one successful
tool for measuring strategic learning skills, described as 1) learning skills (i.e., information
processing, test-taking, identifying main ideas), 2) willingness to learn (i.e., motivation,
attitude, anxiety), and 3) self-regulation of learning (i.e., time management, self-testing,
concentration, study aids) (Sizoo, et al.).
Learning how to learn requires the willingness and self-regulation to identify weaknesses
and focus attention on improving those skills. Learning how to manage anxiety improves
performance significantly (Sizoo, et al., 2005). Adults with learning disabilities attending
college benefit from appropriate accommodations of materials and instruction, but may not be
prepared to learn new skills in the absence of essential learning strategies (Allsopp, et al.,
2005). Colleges and universities today cater to a broader range of student abilities, including
students with varying disabilities, and are involved in instructing learning strategies to assist
struggling students (Allsopp, et al.).
Internships, or experiential education, akin to paid work experiences for students with
disabilities transitioning out of high school, may be another important strategy. College
students with disabilities typically have less work experience and are less likely to complete
college than their peers without disabilities (Wagner, et al., 2005; Zafft, Sezun, and Jordan,
2004). Faculty and administrators in higher education, enhance academic and employment
outcomes for students with disabilities through developing experiential learning opportunities
in their community, and by offering course credit for these experiences (Zafft, et al.).
22 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
Students with disabilities are gaining opportunities via online distance learning and
Internet resources to have individualized learning experiences (e.g., assignments and
feedback), extra practice, automated progress tracking and reporting to invested partners (e.g.,
teachers, parents), active participation in cooperative learning activities, and to review
simulations and skill modeling (Arrigo, 2005; Smith and Meyen, 2003). As a tool the Internet
is more cost effective to school systems compared with assistive technologies, because of its
multiple applications for multiple learners rather than singular use for specific users (Smith
and Meyen). The Web provides vast, inexpensive or free resources for the instructors of
students with disabilities, including electronic journals and databases, synchronous and
asynchronous communication tools (e.g., email, chat, public assignment or event
calendaring), tools for organizing information, multimedia literacy instruction, and
multimedia portfolio assessment (Smith and Meyen).
Persons with vision, hearing, fine motor, or speech impairments, attention or seizure
disorders, learning and other disabilities nonetheless find that many ICTs pose barriers to
their full participation in online activities. Persons with visual impairments do not get a
structural overview of a web page when first encountering it as do persons without visual
impairments (Arrigo, 2005). Graphic images that convey imbedded information are not
accessible to persons with significant visual impairments (Burgstahler, Corrigan, and
McCarter, 2006). Audio conferencing may exclude persons with speech impairments, or
persons with hearing impairments in the absence of closed captioning (Burgstahler, Corrigan,
and McCarter, 2004; Klein, et al., 2003). Video presentations without closed captioning or a
live sign language interpretor (onsite or remotely) pose these same barriers to persons with
hearing impairments (Burgstahler, et al., 2006). Instant messaging poses barriers to persons
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 23
with fine motor impairments in their hands and persons with specific learning disabilities in
reading or writing (Arrigo, 2005). The cost of voice recognition software, alternative input
devices, screen readers, and other assistive technologies, which provide access for persons
with varying impairments, may be prohibitive (Arrigo, 2005).
Many online courses provided by postsecondary institutions are not accessible (Arrigo,
2005). Multiple studies have shown that university web pages, including institutions in the
UK and Ireland, generally are not accessible, using baseline metrics such as Bobby, an
automatic accessibility checker (Rowland, 2000; Jackson-Sanborn, Odess-Harnish and
Warren, 2002; Kelly, 2002; Schmetzke, 2001; Thompson, Burgstahler, Comden, 2003).
Though most postsecondary institutions in a 2003 study indicated they provide online
distance education, ―only 18% indicated that they followed established accessibility
guidelines to a major extent; 28% followed guidelines to a moderate extent, 18% followed
guidelines to a minor extent, 3% did not follow guidelines at all, and 33% did not know if the
Web sites adhered to accessibility guidelines.‖ (Burgstahler, et al., 2004, p. 237).
One of the difficulties in producing accessibility for online instruction comes about
because faculty members often are responsible for getting their content on the Web. Although
these faculty are experts in their fields, they usually do not have time or motivation to learn
evolving methods for creating accessible online content (Cook, and Gladhart, 2002;
Edmonds, 2004). Further, instructional technology leaders on college campuses regard user
support and budget as higher priorities (Cook, and Gladhart, 2002). Currently, faculty are able
to upload their content to content management systems (CMSs), such as WebCT/Blackboard.
Since these web applications are created and maintained by third parties, some web-access
decisions are removed from university personnel. Although generally these CMSs are
becoming more accessible, some barriers remain (e.g., WebCT) (Illinois Center for
Instructional Technology Accessibility, 2006).
Many accessibility barriers can be avoided by careful planning of course design. Ideal for
this purpose are use of universal design principles. For instance, universally-designed
learning opportunities may employ ―materials in varying and redundant media,‖ offer
―alternative means to demonstrate knowledge/skill acquisition (e.g., written, spoken, work
product, demonstration, … PowerPoint or SMART board),‖ or include geographically-
distributed learning via ―distance learning modules, web-conferencing, instant messaging,
chat classrooms, VoIP (Voice and Video over IP), Listservs, and email
distribution/submission of materials).‖ (Myhill, 2006, p. 4).
Additionally, accessible Web casts can be created through the ability to stream different
forms of media simultaneously. The Independent Living Research Utilization program
(ILRU) holds regular Web casts and provides live captioning with each Web cast to make the
session accessible to participants with hearing impairments. Archived Web casts are
supported by documents containing complete transcriptions of the real time sessions (ILRU,
2006). The Law, Health Policy and Disability Center (2005; 2006) at the University of Iowa
similarly conducts numerous Web casts and interactive trainings with real time captioning
and free archived transcripts, such as for the Disability Program Navigator Leadership Audio
Conference Series, and the Center‘s many other online trainings and discussions.
Using cascading style sheets as the basis for accessible Web page templates,
―standardizes and simplifies the formatting of each page,‖ reduces time to build a new
accessible page, and simplifies navigation for a person using a screen reader or a keyboard
24 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
without a mouse (Burgstahler, et al., 2006). Alternative text tags provide screen readers
access to information imbedded in graphics (Klein, et al., 2003).
Accessibility features increasingly are integrated into online course management tools.
Blackboard (2004) software solutions are designed to comply with Section 508 regulations as
well as the W3C Web Accessibility Initiative Standards. Inbuilt features allow users to make
their course content and presentations more accessible using alternative text tags, multiple
presentation formats, client-side server maps, and accurate markup features such as header
elements. Moreover, the tools are compatible with assistive technologies, including screen
readers.
Researchers are developing adaptive course management tools tailored to individual
learners (Cirillo, Cozzolino, De Santo, Marsella, and Salerno, 2000), such as the Agent Based
Intelligent Tutoring System (ABITS), which acts as a remote private tutor (Capuano,
Marsella, and Salerno, 2000). These intelligent tutoring systems are configured to deliver
appropriate content using virtual training assistants that provide ad-hoc modifications based
on learner preferences (Cirillo, et al, 2000).
The enormous variety of purposes, uses, and applications for distance learning that reach
students with and without disabilities of all ages provides more opportunities to evaluate legal
compliance than this chapter can address. We narrow our focus to address three scenarios
with present or emerging high incidence distance education programs, in light of common
disabilities and applicable law.
multidisciplinary team of experts,9 and which must be ―reasonably calculated‖ to meet the
student‘s specific educational needs (Myhill, 2004, p. 1057; Board of Education v. Rowley,
1982, pp. 206-207).
Violations of the FAPE occur when services a) are not free, b) are not appropriate, c) do
not meet state educational standards; d) do not comport with the student‘s IEP, e) are not
provided under public supervision, f) do not comply with IDEA procedural safeguards, g) do
not occur in the LRE, or h) do not provide educational content commensurate with that
provided to grade level peers without disabilities (Myhill, 2004). Common violations occur
when significant evidence indicates the design or implementation of the IEP was insufficient
(Myhill, 2004).
The online civics course is an excellent way for Melanie to experience advanced
curriculum appropriate to her individualized educational needs. Enrollment and participation
may not require fees from Melanie, though an exchange of funds may occur between her local
high school and the virtual (state) high school. If the online course typically is attended by
students away from school, and Melanie will need speech recognition technology to produce
and edit her writing along with an alternative input device to access and move through the
course materials, the IEP team must designate these needs in her IEP and provide them
without cost.
The IDEA procedural safeguards are due process rights provided to the student with a
disability and her family ensuring they have notice, the right to participate in all educational
decisions, access to applicable records, and meaningful opportunities to question and
challenge the appropriateness of the IEP and accuracy of records (20 U.S.C.A. § 1415, 2005).
They are intended to keep the family in the loop and the school accountable. A well designed
and implemented IEP prepared in collaboration among the members of the multidisciplinary
team is the best evidence that procedural safeguards have been followed.
Given the civics course follows and provides state approved curriculum and assessment,
and is facilitated and monitored by the state such as through a highly qualified teacher, the
course likely will comply with the IDEA. Though the course is provided to Melanie in
isolation from her peers without disabilities, if the IEP team agrees, this would not deny her
services in the least restrictive environment because it offers a highly normalizing learning
opportunity that is self-paced by preference and not segregated without choice.
9
The team commonly includes the student‘s parent or guardian, school administrator, special and general education
teachers, and providers of any anticipated or currently provided related services (such as occupational or
physical therapy, speech language services, counseling, and assistive technology) (Myhill, 2004).
26 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
In the first year of Raymond‘s studies he will take general credit courses required of all
City College students, such as English and History, as well as introductory courses in the
School of Library Science. The courses largely are self-paced and reached from any computer
with Internet access. They entail extensive reading and writing assignments using specified
print materials. Compared to the often large sections of most first year courses, the evening
and distance courses average 20 to 40 students. All of Raymond‘s first semester courses
require a class meeting once per week, either in person or via web conference, where the
instructor and students engage in discussion of course subject matter. This also is an
opportunity for students to meet, form study groups, and raise questions to the instructor.
Title II of the ADA and § 504 of the Rehabilitation Act require City College to ensure
their communications with Raymond ―are as effective as communications with other[]‖
students (28 C.F.R. § 35.160(a), 2006). The College must 1) ―make reasonable modifications
to policies, practices, and procedures,‖ 2) provide services in the ―most integrated setting
appropriate,‖ 3) remove communication barriers, including those posed by the design of the
web resources, and 4) provide necessary auxiliary aids and services to ensure Raymond has
―an equal opportunity to participate in, and enjoy the benefits of‖ City College (Blanck, et al.,
2005, p. 345; U.S. Department of Education, 1997; 28 C.F.R. § 35.160(b)(1)). The College,
however, does not have to make modifications that ―fundamentally alter the nature‖ of their
programs and services (U.S. Department of Justice, n.d., § II-3.6100).
The online college courses are a beneficial public service to students with and without
disabilities in the community. As a student who is deaf, class discussions (in-person or
online), communications with the instructor, media presentations (e.g., class video), and audio
web course materials, for instance, must be provided to Raymond in an alternate format, that
is as equally effective for him as the audible components of these activities for his peers.
Whether attending the weekly class in person or via web conference, the College must
provide real time closed captioning or ASL translation for all such communications.
Notably, both closed captioning and ASL translation are reasonable accommodations
and/or necessary auxiliary services with costs to be borne by the College, not Raymond. Web
conferencing applications must have the capacity to display live captioning provided by a
third party, or real time video ASL translation. Web based course materials, such as
audio/video media clips, must provide concurrent captioning or a complete transcript of the
audio content. The College cannot require that Raymond attend weekly classes in a separate
location, such as where the ASL Translator is located. He must have a meaningful
opportunity to join his peers in an integrated setting.
Additionally, City College must accommodate Raymond‘s need to communicate with his
class peers (again whether in person or via web conference), in a manner ensuring equality of
communication. This may take the form of his signing to a translator who then speaks or
writes/types out his communication in real time. It may be reasonable to permit Raymond
extra time to speak in person or in class more slowly. Similarly, it may be more effective for
Raymond to type out his own messages via instant messaging or for a speech synthesizer to
enunciate. Importantly, the instructor must not dissuade Raymond from meaningful
participation by virtue of his requiring extra time for communication, but may consider all
available possibilities for the mutually optimal method. Given the range of reasonable
accommodations, it is unlikely the College could be held accountable to purchase a speech
synthesizer.
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 27
These accommodations are not likely to ―fundamentally alter the nature‖ of Raymond‘s
courses, such as to preclude their availability to him, as they largely are technological
extensions of the College‘s already existing course web conferencing system. The availability
of ASL translators has become a common and widely accepted practice under ADA.
With increasing integration of the Internet and Web in education, several best practices
have emerged to ensure and facilitate accessibility in online courses. ―[U]niversally designed
technologies provide for input and interaction in multiple alternative and equally effective
ways (e.g., keyboard, mouse, or voice input; visual graphic or text output).‖ (Myhill, et al., in
press). The IDEA supports universally designed technology, ―as a vehicle for maximizing
curricular accessibility for all students, including those with disabilities‖ (Downing, 2006,
p.71). The Center for Applied Special Technology (CAST, 2006) suggests the following
Universal Design for Learning guidelines: a) multiple means of representation (i.e., both
technical and content presentation), b) multiple means of expression (i.e., ways for student to
represent knowledge), and c) multiple means of engagement (i.e., addressing affect and
motivation).
Developing a course to be accessible from the onset is less expensive and easier to design
than trying to modify or redesign existing inaccessible course materials (Burgstahler, et al.,
2006). Creating accessible web page templates and cascading style sheets, to be used as the
framework to create all distance learning courses by an institution, can facilitate compliance
with accessibility standards (Burgstahler, et al.). People with disabilities often know what
does and does not work for them. Meaningful involvement of students with disabilities is
essential for identifying specific barriers to their full participation in distance learning
activities (Blue Ridge Community College, 2004). Administrators and faculty can
demonstrate their respect for student expertise and time by providing course credit, tuition
reduction, or stipends commensurate with the time required to evaluate distance education
materials and to make recommendations for overcoming barriers (Blue Ridge Community
College, 2004; Southeast DBTAC, 2006).
Hands-on training and lab demonstrations are effective methods for developing staff
awareness of the access needs of students and faculty with disabilities, and developing skills
in creating accessible distance learning opportunities (Southeast DBTAC, 2006; University of
Florida, 2007). Blue Ridge Community College (2004) developed The Faculty Resource
Guide to Removing Information and Education Barriers to Students with Disabilities (2004),
to increase awareness about accessible ICT and the impact of different disabilities on a
student‘s ability to function successfully in an academic setting. When faculty and web
designers are trained to focus on the needs of the user, the accessibility of the end product is
much higher (University of Florida, 2007).
Designers must understand the range of barriers (e.g., fine motor, visual, stamina,
hearing, attention, memory, and others) for students with disabilities that arise with online
learning opportunities, and how these affect course content, presentation, and web page
formatting (Klein, et al., 2003; Samant, et al., 2006). For example, people with visual
disabilities need descriptions for graphics; adequate space around links might be necessary for
people who have fine motor difficulties using a mouse, and high rates of flickering or motion
on a web site may induce a seizure for persons with seizure disorders, or prove highly
distracting for persons with Attention Deficit Disorder (Klein, et al.; Wall and Sarver, 2003).
Placing questions at the end of a course segment, rather than at the end of a module (i.e.,
comprised of multiple segments), eases the strain on students with cognitive disabilities, such
as traumatic brain injury, to focus on and retrieve relevant information (Southeast DBTAC,
2006). Providing proactive technical assistance to instructors for making web sites and online
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 29
CONCLUSION
Vast technological advancement rapidly is broadening the reach and diversity of distance
learning opportunities for students of all ages, needs, and abilities. Federal and state initiatives
are redefining educational policy and practice, and provide generous support to these
programs with significant funds. Private distance education enterprises are among the most
sophisticated and profitable providers of the job skill trainings in greatest demand.
U.S. disability law and policy mandate people with disabilities have the same rights to
independent and self-determined lives, to pursue meaningful careers, and to enjoy full
participation and integration in the political, economic, educational, and social mainstream as
persons without disabilities. Universal design principles offer a framework for the design of
distance learning technologies and services that provide effective access and meaningful
benefit to the most diverse range of learners. Public and private educators and businesses
must ensure their programs and services provide equal benefit to diverse learners with
disabilities. In so doing, society on the path to tearing down the last of the physical and
attitudinal barriers that historically have isolated and discriminated against people with
disabilities.
REFERENCES
Access Board (Architectural and Transportation Barriers Compliance Board). (2005).
Electronic and information technology accessibility standards. 36 C.F.R. part 1194,
http://www.access-board.gov/sec508/standards.htm
Access Now, Inc. v. Southwest Airlines, Co., 227 F.Supp.2d 1312 (S.D. Ga. 2002).
Allsopp, D.H., Minskoff, E.H., and Bolt, L. (2005). Individualized course-specific Strategy
instruction for college students with learning disabilities and ADHD: Lessons learned
from a model demonstration project. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 20(2),
103-118.
Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 (―ADA‖). 42 U.S.C. §§ 12101 et seq. (2000).
30 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
Downing, J.A. (2006). Media centers and special education: Introduction to the special
issue. Intervention in School and Clinic, 42(2), 67-78.
Edmonds, C.D. (2004). Providing access to students with disabilities in online distance
education: Legal and technical concerns for higher education. American Journal of
Distance Education, 18(1), 51-62.
Education for All Handicapped Children Act. (1975). Pubic Law 94-142, Nov. 29, 1975, 89
Stat. 773.
Elluminate, Inc. (2006). Elluminate live! – lite edition. Retrieved March 18, 2007, from
http://elluminate.com/sales/datasheets/LiteEdition.pdf
Ex-coach – Tech a win for student athletes. (2006, December 7). eSchool News.
Foley, M. (2002). Instant messaging reference in an academic library: A case study. College
and Research Libraries, 63(1), 36-45.
Frattura, E. and Capper, C.A. (2006). Segregated programs versus integrated comprehensive
service delivery for all learners. Remedial and Special Education, 27(6), 355-364.
Gill, M. (2005). The myth of transition: Contractualizing disability in the sheltered workshop.
Disability and Society, 20(6), 613-623.
Google. (2007). Google docs and spreadsheets tour. Retrieved March 14, 2007 from
http://www.google.com/google-d-s/intl/en/tour1.html
Government Accountability Office (―GAO‖). (2004, September 23). No Child Left Behind
Act: Additional assistance and research on effective strategies would help small rural
districts. GAO-04-909. Washington, D.C., at http://www.gao.gov/new.items/d04909.pdf
Guckenberger v. Boston University, 974 F. Supp. 106 (D. Mass. 1997).
Hasten, M. (2004, September 20). Filling rural teacher posts no easy task: Small tax base
means smaller salaries. Lafayette Daily Advertiser.
Hiltz, S.R., and Wellman, B. (1997). Asynchronous learning networks as a virtual classroom.
Communications of the ACM, 40(9), 44-49.
Illinois Center for Instructional Technology Accessibility. (2006). WebCT Accessibility
Interest Group – List of accessibility issues. Retrieved March 20, 2007, from
http://www.disability.uiuc.edu/cita/collaborate/webct/problems.php
Independent Living Research Utilization Program. (2006). Webcasts—frequently asked
questions. Retrieved March 20, 2007, from http://www.ilru.org/html/ training/
webcasts/FAQ.html
Indiana Code. (West 2006). Accessibility standards, §§ 4-13.1-3 et seq.
Indiana Office of Technology (―IOT‖). (2005, July 1). Information technology policy (ITP)
02-1. Available at http://www.in.gov/iot/pdfs/policies/ITP_02-1_Assistive_Technology_
Standards.pdf
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (―IDEA‖). 20 U.S.C.A. §§ 1401 et seq. (2005).
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004. Public Law 108-446, Dec.
3, 2004, 118 Stat. 2647 (2004).
Institute for Food Laws and Regulations. (2006). Certificate program. Retrieved March 14,
2007, from: http://www.iflr.msu.edu/Certificate-program.htm
Information Technology Technical Assistance and Training Center (―ITTATC‖). (2003,
April). National assessment of state EandIT accessibility initiatives. Retrieved March 15,
2007, from http://www.ittatc.org/laws/report_2.php
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 33
ITTATC. (2006, January). Overview of state accessibility laws, policies, standards and other
resources available on-line. Retrieved March 15, 2007 from http://accessibility.
gtri.gatech.edu/sitid/stateLawAtGlance.php
Iwarsson, S. and Stahl, A. (2003). Accessibility, usability, and universal design—Positioning
and definition of concepts describing person-environment relationships. Disability and
Rehabilitation, 25(2), 57-66.
Jackson-Sanborn, E., Odess-Harnish, K., and Warren, N. (2002). Web site accessibility: A
study of six genres. Library Hi Tech, 20(3), 308-317.
Jenkins, J.R., Antil, L.R., Wayne, S.K., and Vadasy, P.F. (2003). How cooperative learning
works for special education and remedial students. Exceptional Children, 69(3), 279-292.
Jung, L.A., Galyon-Keramidas, C., Collins, B., and Ludlow, B. (2006). Distance education
strategies to support practica in rural settings. Rural Special Education Quarterly, 25(2),
18-24.
Katsiyannis, A., Yell, M.L., and Bradley, R. (2001). Reflections on the 25th anniversary of
the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. Remedial and Special Education, 22(6),
324-334.
Keller, B. (2006). NCATE accredits its first online teacher-training program. Education
Week, 26(11), at 12.
Kelly, B. (2002, September-October). Webwatch: An accessibility analysis of UK University
entry points. Ariadne, 33. Retrieved March 20, 2007, from http://www.ariadne.
ac.uk/issue33/web-watch/
Klein, D., Myhill, W.N., Hansen, L., Asby, G., Michaelson, S., and Blanck, P. (2003).
Electronic doors to education: Study of high school web accessibility in Iowa. Behavioral
Sciences and the Law, 21(1), 27-49.
Kock, N., and Nosek, J. (2005). Expanding the boundaries of E-Collaboration. IEEE
Transactions on Professional Communication, 48(1), 1-9.
Kohler, P.D. and Field, S. (2003). Transition-focused education: Foundation for the future.
Journal of Special Education, 37(3), 174-183.
Landrum, T.J., Tankersley, M., and Kauffman, J.M. (2003). What is special about special
education for students with emotional or behavioral disorders? Journal of Special
Education, 37(3), 148-156.
Law, Health Policy and Disability Center. (2006). 2006 disability program navigator
leadership audio conference series. Retrieved March 19, 2007, from
http://disability.law.uiowa.edu/dpn/audio/2006_dpn_leadership_ac.html
Law, Health Policy and Disability Center. (2005). Archives. Retrieved March 19, 2007, from
http://disability.law.uiowa.edu/lhpdc/archives/index.html
Lawline.com. (2006). Online continuing legal education. Retrieved March 16, 2007, from
http://www.lawline.com/index.html
Lee, C., Ghyme, S., Park, C., and Wohn, K. (1998). The control of avatar motion using hand
gesture. Proceedings of the ACM symposium on Virtual reality software and technology,
Taipei, Taiwan, November 2-5, 1998, 59-65.
Leigh, J. (1999). A review of tele-immersive applications in the CAVE research network.
Proceeding of Virtual Reality.
Lewis, B., MacEntee, V., and Youngs-Maher, P. (2002). Software compare and contrast:
Blackboard, WebCT and Lotus Notes, a Panel discussion. IS2002 Information Science +
IT Education Conference, Cork, Ireland.
34 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
Locatis, C., Fontelo, P., Sneiderman, C., Ackerman, M., Uijtdehaage, S., Candler, C. (2003).
Webcasting videoconferences over IP: A synchronous communication experiment.
Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 10(2), 150-153.
Mace, R. (2007). About UD: Universal design principles. Raleigh. NC: Center for Universal
Design. Retrieved March 13, 2007, from http://www.design.ncsu.edu/cud/about_ud/
udprinciples.htm
Martin v. Metropolitan Atlanta Rapid Transit Authority, 225 F.Supp.2d 1362 (N.D. Ga.
2002).
Matthews, D. (1999, September). The origins of distance education and its use in the United
States. The Journal. Retrieved March 16, 2007, from http://www.thejournal.com/
articles/14278
McLeskey, J. and Waldron, N.L. (2007). Making differences ordinary in inclusive
classrooms. Intervention in School and Clinic, 42(3), 162-168.
Mehta, S. (2007, February 4). New K-12 elective: Class online. Los Angeles Times, at A1.
Miller, S.M. and Miller, K.L. (1999). Using instructional theory to facilitate communication
in Web-based courses. Educational Technology and Society, 2(3). Retrieved February 5,
2007, from http://ifets.massey.ac.nz/periodical/vol_3_99/miller.html
Miller v. Abilene Christian University of Dallas, 517 F. Supp. 437 (N.D. Tex. 1981).
Mintz, J. (2004, August 17). Enterprise: Law school profits from classroom-web mix –
bricks-and-mortar studies blend with Internet use for California bar exam. Wall Street
Journal, at B3.
Moore, M.G. (1989). Three types of interaction. American Journal of Distance Education,
3(2). Retrieved February 6, 2007, from http://www.ajde.com/Contents/vol3_2.htm#
editorial
Mortensen, J., Vinayagamoorthy, V., Slater, M., Steed, A., Lok, B., and Whitton, M.C.
(2002). Collaboration in tele-immersive environments. Eighth Eurographics Workshop
on Virtual Environments.
Mortweet, S.L., Utley, C.A., Walker, D., Dawson, H.L., Delquadri, J.C., Reddy, S.S., et al.
(1999). Classwide peer tutoring: Teaching students with mild mental retardation in
inclusive classrooms. Exceptional Children, 65(4), 524-536.
Murray, C. (2006, November 29). FLVS debuts forum for virtual teachers: Monthly forum
encourages educators to share best practices, knowledge. eSchool News.
Myhill, W.N. (2004). No FAPE for children with disabilities in the Milwaukee Parental
Choice Program: Time to redefine a free appropriate public education. Iowa Law Review,
89(3), 1051-1091.
Myhill, W.N. (2006). The future for universal design in improving employment outcomes for
people with disabilities. Unpublished Issue Brief (National Council on Disability
Solicitation No. 05-01) (on file with author).
Myhill, W.N., Cogburn, D.L., Samant, D., Addom, B., and Blanck, P. (in press). Accessible
cyberinfrastructure-enabled knowledge communities in the disability community.
Assistive Technology Journal, 18.
National Center for Education Statistics (―NCES‖). (2003). Distance education at degree-
granting postsecondary institutions: 2000-2001 (NCES 2003-017). Available at
http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2003/2003017.pdf
Distance Education Initiatives and their Early 21st Century Role… 35
Thompson, T., Burgstahler, S., and Comden, D. (2003). Research on web accessibility in
higher education. Information Technology and Disabilities E-Journal, 9(2). Retrieved
March 20, 2007, from http://www.rit.edu/~easi/itd/itdv09n2/contents.htm
Tennessee v. Lane, 541 U.S. 509 (2004).
Trade Schools, Colleges and Universities (―TSCandU‖). (2007a). Distance learning schools
directory. Retrieved March 8, 2007, from http://www.distance-learning.ca/
TSCandU. (2007b). Trade schools directory. Retrieved March 8, 2007, from
http://www.trade-schools.net/
TSCandU. (2007c). Online degree Q and A. Retrieved March 8, 2007, from http://www.trade-
schools.net/articles/online-degree-article.asp
Tu, C.-H. (2005). From presentation to interaction: New goals for online learning
technologies. Educational Media International, 42(3), 189-206.
Tyndall v. National Education Center of California, 31 F.3d 209 (4th Cir. 1993).
Ubell, R. (2000). Engineers turn to e-learning. IEEE Spectrum, 37(10), 59-63.
U N E S C O. (2005, December). Workshop on ICT and persons with disabilities: Summary
report. Organized on the occasion of the Second Phase of the World Summit on the
Information Society (WSIS) (Tunis, Tunisia 16 November 2005). Available at
http://portal.unesco.org/ci/fr/files/21052/11382869031report_ict_pwd_en.pdf/report_ict_
pwd_en.pdf
United Nations. (2006a, November). United Nations global audit of Web accessibility.
London: Nomensa.
United Nations. (2006b, December 16). Depositary notification (C.N.1236.2006.TREATIES-
1, at http://untreaty.un.org/English/CNs/2006/1201_1300/1236E.pdf
United Nations. (2007). The Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities: Opening
for signature. Retrieved July 20, 2007, from http://www.un.org/disabilities/
convention/signature.shtml
U.S. Constitution, Amendment XI. Suits Against States.
U.S. Constitution, Amendment XIV. Citizenship; Privileges and Immunities; Due Process;
Equal Protection; Apportionment of Representation; Disqualification of Officers; Public
Debt; Enforcement.
U.S. Department of Education. (1997, April 7). Letter to Dr. James Rosser, President of
California State University at Los Angeles, from Adriana Cardenas, Team Leader, Office
for Civil Rights, U.S. Department of Education, http://www.rit.edu/~easi/law/csula.htm
U.S. Department of Education. (2004). Toward a new golden age in American Education—
how the Internet, the law and today‘s students are revolutionizing expectations: National
Education Technology Plan 2004. Washington, D.C. http://www.ed.gov/ about/offices/
list/os/technology/plan/2004/site/theplan/NETP_Final.pdf
U.S. Department of Education. (2006, April). 26th annual report to Congress on the
implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 2004, Vol. 2.
Washington, D.C.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (n.d.). Protecting human subjects training.
Retrieved March 14, 2007, from http://www.hrsa.gov/humansubjects/
U.S. Department of Justice. (n.d.). The Americans with Disabilities Act title II technical
assistance manual. Retrieved March 15, 2007, from http://www.ada.gov/taman2.html
University of Florida. (n.d.). Accessibility. Retrieved March 7, 2007, from www.at.ufl.edu/
accessibility/accessibility_cd/index.html
38 William N. Myhill, Deepti Samant, David Klein et al.
Vaughn, S. and Linan-Thompson, S. (2003). What is special about special education for
students with learning disabilities? Journal of Special Education, 37(3), 140-147.
Virginia Code Annotated. (West 2006). Title 51.5. Persons with disabilities.
Virtual High School, Inc. (2007). VHS member profile. Retrieved March 11, 2007, from
http://www.govhs.org/Pages/AboutUs-Home
W3C. (1999, May 5). Web Content Accessibility Guidelines 1.0. Retrieved March 7, 2007,
from http://www.w3.org/TR/WAI-WEBCONTENT/
W3C. (2006). WAI mission and organization. Retrieved October 14, 2006, from
http://www.w3.org/WAI/about.html
Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Garza, N., and Levine, P. (2005, April). After high
school: A first look at the postschool experiences of youth with disabilities (NLTS2).
Available at http://www.nlts2.org/pdfs/afterhighschool_report.pdf
Waldron, N.L. and McLeskey, J. (1998). The effects of an inclusive school program on
students with mild and severe learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 64(3), 395-405.
Wall, P.S. and Sarver, L. (2003). Disabled student access in an era of technology. Internet
and Higher Education, 6, 277-284.
Watson, J. and Ryan, J. (2006). Keeping pace with K-12 online learning. Evergreen
Consulting Associates. Available at http://www.nacol.org/docs/ Keeping%20Pace%2
0with%20K-12%20Online%20Learning%202006.pdf
Wenger, E., McDermott, R., and Snyder, W. M. (2002). Cultivating communities of practice.
Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Whitehead, B. (2005, November 12). What is web conferencing and the benefits it provides?
WebConferencingZone. Retrieved March 18, 2007, from http://www.web-conferencing-
zone.com/4049-web-based-conferencing.htm
Wimberly, L., Reed, R., and Morris, M. (2004). Postsecondary students with learning
disabilities: Barriers to accessing education-based information technology. Information
Technology and Disabilities, 9(1), 1-18. Retrieved March 6, 2007, from
http://www.rit.edu/~easi/itd/itdv10n1/wimberly.htm
Woodfine, B. P., Nunes, M. B., and Wright, D. J. (in press). Text-based synchronous e-
learning and dyslexia: Not necessarily the perfect match! Computers and Education.
Xu, M., Fountain, M., MacArthur, S., Braunstein, M., and Sooriamurthi, R. (2004,
November). Experience in developing a mobile course portal. Proceedings of the World
Conference on E-Learning in Corporate, Government, Healthcare, and Higher
Education. Washington, D.C.
Zafft, C., Sezun, S., and Jordan, M. (2004). Making experiential education accessible for
students with disabilities. Institute Brief, 19. Available at http://www.community
inclusion.org/ publications/pdf/ib19.pdf
Zaslow, J. (2005, June 30). When disabled children get too old for public education. Wall
Street Journal, at D1.
Zhang, D., Zhao, J.L, Zhou, L., and Nunamaker, Jr., J.F. (2004). Can e-learning replace
classroom learning? Communications of the ACM, 47(5), 75-79.
34 C.F.R. Part 300. (2006). Assistance to states for the education of children with disabilities.
In: Recent Trends in Education ISBN 978-1-60741-795-8
Editor: Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas © 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Chapter 2
LANGUAGE-COGNITION INTERACTIONS
DURING BILINGUAL LANGUAGE
DEVELOPMENT IN CHILDREN
ABSTRACT
A growing body of research suggests a close relationship between children‘s
development of linguistic and cognitive skills, with language development and cognitive
development mutually influencing each other. This chapter considers childhood
bilingualism as a special case of language acquisition, with implications for the
relationship between linguistic and cognitive processes. Specifically, during bilingual
language development, language input is phonologically more complex and spans two
languages instead of one. Given language input that includes phonetic inventories and
vocabulary from two languages (consisting of two labels for most concepts), linguistic
and cognitive implications for bilingual development are discussed. In addition, the
chapter considers the consequences of simultaneous activation of two language systems
and the juggling of two language codes in children. It is suggested that bilingual children
may have a higher cognitive processing load during language use and learning, resulting
in both linguistic and cognitive differences compared with monolingual peers. Recent
research on linguistic and cognitive differences between monolingual and bilingual
children is placed in the context of current theoretical models of language learning,
development, and processing. In particular, we consider recent findings from
monolingual and bilingual children in the context of learnability theory (with an emphasis
on the influence of input complexity on linguistic / cognitive development), and in light
of usage-based accounts of language acquisition (with an emphasis on the development
of potentially different cognitive skills in monolinguals and bilinguals). We conclude that
1 Corresponding Author: Henrike K. Blumenfeld, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, School of Speech, Language and
Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, 5500 Campanile Drive, San Diego, CA 92182-1518, E-mail:
[email protected].
40 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
childhood bilingualism provides a unique context for examining the interaction between
linguistic and cognitive mechanisms during development.
INTRODUCTION
Cracking the language code presents infants with a problem of extraordinary
computational complexity. Infants who learn their mother tongue have to identify patterns in
the string of speech sounds that they hear, and map these sound patterns onto meanings that
refer to objects, events, and other aspects of language. For children who grow up in bilingual
environments, decoding and separating two language systems is likely to provide an even
bigger challenge. What cognitive tools are available to babies to accomplish such impressive
feats? A growing body of research suggests that specific types of cognitive processes may
guide children‘s language acquisition, and that linguistic and cognitive development mutually
influence each other.
In the present chapter, we consider childhood bilingualism as a special case of language
acquisition, with implications for the relationship between linguistic and cognitive processes.
Since simultaneous acquisition of two languages is likely to pose particular cognitive
challenges, ways in which cognitive mechanisms are employed to acquire language may be
especially salient in bilingual children. In general, examination of the relationship between
linguistic and cognitive nonverbal processing is a central area of study in the cognitive
sciences (e.g., Bates, Dale, and Thal, 1995; Fodor, 1983; Tomasello, 2007). Theoretical
research in this area aims to delineate relationships between language and cognition and to
develop models that specify the involvement of nonlinguistic cognitive mechanisms during
language development and processing (e.g., Bates and MacWhinney, 1987, 1989; Dijkstra
and Van Heuven, 2002; Green, 1998; Li and Farkas, 2002; Norman and Shallice, 1986;
Tomasello, 2007). This line of research contributes to the decade-old debate between the
modular- / domain-specific view of language as an encapsulated and specialized module (e.g.,
Fodor 1983, 1985; Chomsky, 1988; Pinker, 1984) and the domain-general view of language
as a system that is tightly linked to nonverbal (and domain-general) cognitive abilities (e.g.
Bates, Thal and Marchman, 1991; Bates, Thal and Janowsky, 1992; Bates and MacWhinney,
1987; Tomasello, 2007)2. The modular / domain-specific view claims that language is a
2
In the present chapter, the term ―domain-specific‖ will be used to refer to processes that act within language and
are likely to be unique to language. Conversely, the term ―domain-general‖ will be used to refer to processes that
are involved in both language-specific and nonlinguistic tasks, and can therefore be assumed to be located
‗outside‘ and ‗above‘ language in the cognitive hierarchy.
Bilingual Language Development 41
separable ―mental organ‖ (Chomsky, 1988), and that the neural correlates underlying this
mental organ are subsumed only by language processing and no other (nonlinguistic)
cognitive mechanisms. In contrast, the domain-general view claims that linguistic abilities are
(at least to some extent) supported by nonlinguistic cognitive processes.
The modular and domain-general views of language embody two distinct theories of
language acquisition. The modular view is that language abilities are cognitively
encapsulated. As a consequence, the modular view suggests that the linguistic system
develops without exterior cognitive influences, and that its features are largely innate.3 As the
infant becomes exposed to language input, only the specific features of the child‘s native
language(s) are determined with exposure, thus ―calibrating‖ a pre-existing system for a
specific linguistic community. This theoretical orientation is supported by the Poverty of the
Stimulus argument: Child language input is frequently incomplete and not always optimally
grammatical; nevertheless, children consistently acquire full linguistic systems (e.g.,
Bickerton, 1981; Pinker, 1984).
In contrast to the modular view, the domain-general view is that language abilities are
closely related to other cognitive abilities. As a consequence, language develops through
interaction with other cognitive abilities. That is, language emerges with exposure to, and
under the guidance of general cognitive learning mechanisms. This theoretical orientation is
supported by evidence that language is learned by way of general learning mechanisms that
systematically distill and make sense of the regularities within the language input. Research in
this area suggests that child language input is in fact not as poor as was previously believed,
and may be sufficient for the child to construct a language system, given a set of cognitive
abilities and biases (e.g., Dietrich, Swingley, and Werker, 2007; Maye, Werker, and Gerken,
2002). In the present chapter, recent findings in bilingual language development will be
considered through the prism of usage-based models of language learnability, which assume
that the linguistic input that children receive contains more information than is apparent, and
that children are in fact able to derive a language system based on patterns within this input
and under the guidance of general cognitive mechanisms (e.g., Tomasello, 2007).
The study of language-cognition interactions in bilingual development holds great
promise for the debate on how language is learned in general. Examination of the interface
between language and cognition in bilingual children, compared to their monolingual peers,
has the potential to contribute to debates in the cognitive sciences about the nature of
language, and to models of language development. In addition, it is becoming increasingly
important to understand mechanisms and pathways of bilingual language acquisition in
childhood, as the bilingual population is growing. Currently, approximately 5.2 million
bilingual children are enrolled in schools in the United States, which marks a 61% increase
from 1994 (c.f., Goldstein and Fabiano, 2007). It is projected that, by 2030, 40% of school-
age children in the United States will be native speakers of another language, and will be
acquiring English as a second language (e.g., Goldstein, 2000; Roseberry-McKibbin and
Brice, 2000). Therefore, the incidence of childhood bilingualism in the United States is
predicted to grow, and the numbers of adult bilinguals who learned two languages at an early
age will grow with it. In general, with the majority of adults throughout the world proficient
in a second language (e.g., Romaine, 1995), and with bilingualism on the rise in the United
3
But note that the language-specificity and innateness arguments are separable, since cognitive mechanisms that
are domain-general may also be innate.
42 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
States (e.g., US Census, 2000), the influence of bilingual experience on cognitive function
becomes increasingly relevant.
In the present chapter, we consider the nature of bilingual language input, the cognitive
consequences of such input, and possible mechanisms of bilingual language acquisition. First,
recent evidence is presented on how bilingual children separate two phonological and lexical
codes from the input, and how these language acquisition paths may influence and shape
cognitive processes, both in the linguistic and in the nonlinguistic domains. Second, we place
recent findings on bilingual language development in the context of current models of
language learnability- and usage-based accounts, and conclude that examination of linguistic
and cognitive processes in bilingual children, compared to their monolingual peers, may yield
evidence in support of theories that language development is guided by domain-general
cognitive processes.
(e.g., Kuhl, 2000; Polka and Werker, 1994), their sensitivity to non-native prosody variations
(such as tones in tone languages) is lost between 6-9 months of age (Mattock and Burnham,
2006), and their sensitivity to non-native consonants is lost between 10-12 months of age
(Werker and Tees, 1984). As a result of this perceptual reorganization, bilingual infants are
typically sensitive to a wider range of speech sounds than their monolingual peers by the end
of their first year of life, although this sensitivity may develop gradually, with one language
leading the other (for a review, see Sebastián-Gallés and Bosch, 2005).
Bilingual infants‘ ability to tell their languages apart from an early age, and to establish
fine-grained phonetic categories for each language independently, begs the following
questions: (1) Do bilingual children develop two phonological systems or an integrated
system, and (2) how do bilingual infants distinguish between phonological codes of their two
languages. Whether bilingual infants‘ phonological systems are initially shared or separated
remains an active area of research (e.g., Bhatia and Ritchie, 1999; Kehoe, 2002; Ingram,
1982; Keshavarz and Ingram, 2002; Paradis, 2001; Vihman, 2002). Models have been posited
to illustrate the view of an initially shared phonological system (the Unitary System Model,
Bhatia and Ritchie, 1999), the view that phonological systems develop separately from the
beginning (the Dual Systems Model, Keshavarz and Ingram, 2002), and the view that
phonological codes of the two languages are highly interactive from early on (the
Interactional Dual Systems Model, Paradis, 2001).
Evidence in support of an initial Unitary System includes findings that children may first
use components of the adult sound inventories that overlap across both of their languages
(e.g., Leopold, 1970) and that children may acquire phonological properties of their two
languages sequentially rather than simultaneously (e.g., Kehoe, 2002; Sebastián-Gallés and
Bosch, 2005). In turn, evidence in support of an initial Dual System includes findings that
children can distinguish between input from their two languages at an early age, and that
bilingual children‘s first words can show drastic differences across the two languages in terms
of phonological templates (e.g., 2-syllable templates in Italian and 1-syllable templates in
English, see Ingram, 1982). Evidence for an Interactional Dual System includes findings that
simultaneous acquisition of two phonological codes may result in acceleration of certain
aspects of learning (e.g., earlier Spanish coda acquisition in children acquiring Spanish and
German, Kehoe, Trujill and Lleo, 2001), and that initial words in one language may reflect
stress patterns of the other language (Keshavarz and Ingram, 2002). It has also been argued
that children do not develop phonological systems, but rather develop distributed knowledge
of a set of phonetic contrasts, and that phonological systems emerge in tandem with language-
specific vocabularies (Vihman, 2002). In sum, while the nature of early phonological
representations in bilingual children remains under debate, bilingual children can distinguish
between their languages early on, and are able to learn language-specific phonological
features, with occasional interaction between the two systems.
The nature of early phonological representations is likely to be tightly linked to how
bilingual infants approach the task of distinguishing between two types of language input. In
general, early evidence suggests that bilinguals‘ approach in distinguishing between
languages may differ from monolinguals‘ approach from an early age. Specifically, Bosch
and Sebastián-Gallés (1997) found differences in how monolingual and bilingual infants
distinguish between native and unfamiliar languages at 4 months of age. To assess speech
perception in pre-linguistic infants, the authors employed a Head Turn Task (also see Werker,
Polka, and Pegg, 1997), where infants were taught to turn towards a speaker from where they
44 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
heard a story told. Story sentences from one language were presented from a speaker to the
right, and sentences from another language were presented from a speaker to the left. Infants‘
attention to a space between the two speakers was restored after each trial by presentation of
colorful moving images. The authors measured how long it took infants to turn towards each
speaker, and found that monolingual children were quicker to orient towards their native
language than towards an unfamiliar language. In contrast, bilingual children were slower to
orient towards a familiar language than towards an unfamiliar language. The authors
suggested that bilingual infants‘ delay in orienting towards their familiar language (relative to
an unfamiliar language) may be due to an initial attempt to discern which of their two
languages is being spoken (also see Werker and Byers-Heinlein, 2008). Alternatively, it is
possible that, since bilingual infants are exposed to more complex and varied auditory input
(from two language systems instead of one), they may respond differently to unfamiliar input
in general, and may maintain an interest in relatively unfamiliar phonological input for a
longer period of time. In sum, findings suggest that bilingual infants approach language
differently than their monolingual peers early on during the acquisition process, with
differences likely to emerge as these future bilinguals start to separate two phonological
codes.
Further research is needed to examine whether bilingual infants start to develop separate
mechanisms for controlling their language systems at this early age. For example, children‘s
ability to discern which of their two languages is being spoken at any point in time would
imply early emergence of mechanisms to make such distinctions. Possible mechanisms for
early language differentiation might be early precursors of language tags (e.g., Green, 1998)
or language nodes (e.g., Dijkstra and Van Heuven, 1998, 2002) that emerge with
phonological knowledge, as well as a growing awareness of phonological regularities within
the input of each language. Language tags and language nodes have been posited to be part of
adult bilingual systems, and serve to identify and distinguish between the two languages
during processing. Language nodes and language tags are terms that refer to representations
that signal the language-membership of a word (e.g., Dijkstra and Van Heuven, 1998, 2002).
While a language tag has been posited to be part of each word‘s lexical representation, which
is in turn linked to cognitive control mechanisms (Green, 1998, see Figure 1A), language
nodes have been conceptualized as units that capture overall activation of a language across
multiple lexical items and across time (Dijkstra and Van Heuven, 1998, see Figure 1B). These
components of bilinguals‘ cognitive systems have been thought of as higher-level
representations that are closely related to a goal-oriented cognitive system.
Bilingual infants can distinguish between the phonologies of their two languages at 4
months of age. While they may start to develop precursors to the adult language control
system, it is also possible that bilingual infants rely solely on knowledge of distributional
sound regularities to distinguish between their languages at this early stage (e.g., Maye,
Werker, and Gerken, 2002; Li and Farkas, 2002), without formalizing attentional
mechanisms.
Differences in phonetic sensitivity (spanning two language systems instead of one) may
be reflected in bilingual infants‘ approach to language. That is, instead of starting to develop
cognitive mechanisms specific to distinguishing between two native languages, differences
between monolingual and bilingual infants in orienting towards an unfamiliar language may
be the result of a general prolonged sensitivity to contrasts within unfamiliar input.
A. The Inhibitory Control Model B. The Bilingual Interactive Activation Model
Adapted from Green (1998) Adapted from Dijkstra and Van Heuven (1998)
Activation of the bilingual lexicon is controlled at various Activation of the bilingual lexicon is controlled at the
levels (at the level of specific language task schemas, and at the language node level. For example, activation of English word
level of a general supervisory attentional system, SAS). Within the representations, based on English input, results in activation of
lexicon, language-membership of words is determined via the English language node, which signals use of the English
language tags that are part of each words‘ lexical representation. lexicon by inhibiting words in the Dutch lexicon.
Figure 1. Cognitive control mechanisms that differentiate between languages during adult bilingual processing, including language tags as posited by the
Inhibitory Control Model, and language nodes as posited by the Bilingual Interactive Activation Model. Within each model, cognitive control components are
highlighted in grey.
46 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
As bilingual children‘s phonological systems become more elaborate over the course of
phonological and lexical acquisition, it remains an open question how the child‘s linguistic
system evolves towards the bilingual adult‘s linguistic system. Bilingual adults activate their
two languages in parallel when words in their two languages have phonological overlap with
each other. For example, if a Russian-English adult hears the word marker in English, s/he
may also look at a stamp (because the Russian word for stamp is marka) before identifying the
marker (e.g., Marian and Spivey, 2003a,b; also see Blumenfeld and Marian, 2007; Ju and
Luce, 2004; Weber and Cutler, 2004). Bilingual adults‘ ability to match auditory input to
words in both of their languages within the first 200-400 milliseconds of comprehension, and
to resolve this cross-linguistic competition in an efficient manner, reflects the flexibility of the
bilingual system. It is likely that the capacity to consider words from both languages, and to
resolve subsequent competition, is central to bilinguals‘ ability to switch between languages,
and that it requires cognitive control (e.g., Blumenfeld and Marian, in preparation; Green,
1998).
Early evidence suggests that the ability to resolve phonological competition is acquired
gradually. Early evidence from an eye-tracking study in 5- and 6-year old monolingual
children suggests that children also activate similar-sounding words in parallel within their
native language (similar to adults, Marslen-Wilson, 1987; McClelland and Elman, 1986), but
that competition from these similar-sounding words lasts over 1 second longer than it does in
monolingual adults (Sekerina and Brooks, 2007). This delay in competition implies that, at 5-
and 6 years of age, children are less efficient than adults at resolving competition at the
phonological level. It is likely that cross-linguistic activation of similar-sounding words
follows a similar pattern, and that the cognitive resources necessary to more efficiently reduce
phonological competition develop with age (e.g., Booth et al., 2003; Comalli, Wapner, and
Werner, 1962; Posner, Rothbart, Farah, and Bruer, 2001).
To summarize, as is the case in adults, children consider multiple similar-sounding words
in parallel during comprehension. However, children show delays in biasing the system
towards one of these words, suggesting less efficient mechanisms than adults in ―zooming in‖
on relevant linguistic information. It is likely that the efficiency of resolving within-language
competition mirrors the efficiency of resolving between-language competition (e.g., Marian
and Spivey, 2003a,b). Understanding the nature of between-language competition is linked to
understanding the need to formally distinguish between the two languages to avoid
interference. On the one hand, it is possible that this need arises early on during language
development, while the two phonologies are being acquired and elaborated. Specifically, the
ability to activate one language system more than the other may become crucial in order to
fully learn language-specific information, such as acoustic boundaries of phonemic categories,
and later phonotactic rules and phoneme-to-phoneme transition probabilities. For instance,
children become sensitive to specific phoneme transitions that occur frequently in the input,
and therefore have high transitional probabilities (Maye, Gerken, and Werker, 2002). As a
consequence, bilingual children may focus their attention on different clusters of phonemes
with high transitional probabilities in each of their languages, in order to parse out likely
words from the speech stream (Fennell, Byers-Heinlein, and Werker, 2007; for a review, see
Sebastián-Gallés and Bosch, 2005). On the other hand, it is possible that the need to formally
distinguish between the two languages only arises once the bilingual child accumulates
substantial word knowledge, and needs to efficiently retrieve words from one of the two
languages, while discounting similar words in the other language.
Bilingual Language Development 47
1
Quine illustrated the indeterminacy faced by the child in linking a specific sound sequence, such as gavagai, to a
specific referent in the environment. In the Gavagai example, if the child saw a rabbit run by, and an adult
speaker exclaimed ―gavagai!‖, would this word refer to the rabbit itself, to part of the rabbit, to the action of
running, or to any other aspect of the visual scene?
48 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
placing the objects in the artifact category, even if these objects had eyes. The finding that
conceptual knowledge can drive attention suggests that attention to specific aspects of
referents is allocated top-down, and is cued through an interface with the conceptual system. In
sum, while joint attention provides external cues about objects and their names, paying
attention to specific characteristics in the environment or the input can be seen as an internal
process that is initiated by the child based on experience.
In addition to paying attention to specific aspects of objects, such as shape or texture,
children may need to pay attention to fine-grained sound-differences between similar names
that reference different objects. For example, Stager and Werker (1997) showed that children
have initial difficulty in learning words that differ by only one phoneme, and are thus minimal
pairs (e.g., /bih/ and /dih/). When Fennel and Werker (2003) examined bilingual infants‘
ability to learn object names that were minimal pairs, they found that bilinguals could not
successfully learn such words until 20 months of age (while monolinguals had succeeded by
17 months, also see Fennel, Byers-Heinlein, and Werker, 2007). Since both monolingual and
bilingual infants were able to perceive phonological contrasts at an earlier age, the authors
concluded that limited cognitive resources might account for the findings, with a scarcity of
available resources resulting in an inability to encode fine-grained phonological contrasts
during word learning. It is possible that, because minimal pairs are perceptually the most
challenging, they require the most attentional resources to process, leaving fewer resources for
storage.
Specifically, bilingual children may initially have fewer cognitive resources available to
pay attention to nuanced sound-differences during word learning. Within a limited resources
framework, Bjorklund and Harnishfeger (1990) propose that, in development, a majority of
cognitive resources are utilized for processing. At this young age, processing is thought to
remain inefficient, necessitating allocation of these cognitive resources. As processing and the
coordination of linguistic mechanisms become more efficient, more cognitive resources can be
freed up for storage. Bilingual infants may recruit more cognitive resources overall to
accommodate early phonological and lexical development across two language systems.
Therefore, bilingual infants may have fewer resources available to pay attention to, and store,
highly similar-sounding words, resulting in later onset of word learning that requires nuanced
phonological distinctions. In sum, the finding of delays in minimal pair learning in bilingual
infants is consistent with the idea that bilingual infants expend additional processing resources
during word learning, since they have to make decisions about language membership, retain
two sets of phonotactic rules, and map sound sequences onto words in two languages.
As bilingual children overcome initial barriers in learning words from two language
systems, they soon encounter another hurdle that may prove more challenging for them than
for their monolingual peers: objects frequently have multiple labels. Learning of translation
equivalents for the same object may pose a special challenge to bilingual children because
early word learning has been shown to be biased towards assignment of one label to each
referent (e.g., Markman and Wachtel, 1988). This well-studied learning bias, known as the
Mutual Exclusivity Principle is one in a set of word learning constraints that is believed to help
children solve the Gavagai problem (Quine, 1960): If children hear a new word, but one of the
objects in their environment has already been assigned a name, then the new word must refer
to either a property or aspect of the already-named object, or to another object or action (e.g.,
Golinkoff, Mervis, and Hirsh-Pasek, 1994; Woodward, 2000). Research in the monolingual
language acquisition literature suggests that, by 5 years of age, monolingual children loosen
Bilingual Language Development 49
the constraints of the mutual exclusivity principle, and start assigning multiple names to single
objects (e.g., tricycle, trike, bicycle, bike, Johnson, 1994). In an experiment measuring
response latencies during word naming in high-constraint and low-constraint contexts (e.g.,
contexts where multiple labels for an object can be used vs. contexts where only one label for
an object can be used), Johnson (1994) showed that, by the age of 9, children were sensitive to
nonlinguistic context, using context-appropriate words when multiple labels were available. In
addition, 9-year old children showed processing costs (delayed responses) when naming one of
multiple object names during high-constraint situations. The authors took their findings to
suggest that 9-year olds inhibited alternative names for the same object. The fact that Johnson
(1994) found an inhibition effect for 9-year olds, but not for 7- or 5-year olds (also see
Simpson and Lorsbach, 1983), is consistent with evidence that inhibitory control emerges
relatively late in childhood (Bjorklund and Harnishfeger, 1990; Booth et al., 2003).
Since bilingual children face the challenge of learning multiple object labels, not only
within-language but also between-language (i.e., translation equivalents), it can be predicted
that they start disregarding the mutual exclusivity principle earlier than their monolingual
peers. In fact, bilingual children have been shown to know both translation equivalents of
early-learned words as early as within their first 50 words (Mikes, 1990). Mattock, Polka, and
Rvachew (2006) recently showed that 17-month old bilingual infants were better than their
monolingual peers at learning multiple labels for newly-trained pseudowords. Au and Glusman
(1990) presented 3- and 6-year old bilingual children with novel stuffed animals, and provided
labels in both English and Spanish (presented by two different speakers). They found that
bilingual children accepted both labels for the object when they knew that the labels came
from different languages. To examine whether suspension of the mutual exclusivity principle
between languages would also result in its earlier suspension within language, Davidson and
Tell (2005) conducted a word naming experiment with 3- and 6-year old monolingual and
bilingual children. Children were shown familiar and unfamiliar objects, either by themselves,
or with an attached spare part. They were then presented with novel names for these objects,
and were asked whether the name referred to the whole object or to part of the object. The
authors found that 6-year old bilingual children did indeed suspend the mutual exclusivity
principle more frequently than their monolingual peers, and were more willing to accept a
novel name to refer to an already-named object. It is likely that bilingual children‘s lower
reliance on the mutual exclusivity principle is due to more frequent exposure to multiple
names for one object, and that it paves the way for acquisition of two lexicons. It remains an
open question whether bilingual children employ inhibitory control mechanisms to control
activation of multiple object names across languages. In the adult bilingualism literature,
evidence exists that selection of a word for production entails inhibition of its translation
equivalent (e.g., Green 1998). Given the evidence from monolingual children for use of
inhibitory control during naming of objects with multiple labels, it is likely that bilingual
children also employ cognitive control mechanisms to functionally separate multiple object-
names.
In sum, construction of a lexicon involves multiple stages, including detection of statistical
regularities and recurrent patterns within the speech stream, identification of words, and
separation of words that are similar to each other (either at the phonological level or at the
semantic or contextual level). Throughout each step in this process, bilingual children
encounter additional challenges, due to the complexity and breadth of input across their two
language systems. Therefore, throughout their early years of language acquisition, from
50 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
Bilingual children have been shown, on a number of tasks, to be more aware of the
function that language serves to accomplish certain communicative goals and that linguistic
symbols in themselves are arbitrary. This ability has been termed Metalinguistic Awareness
(e.g., see Bialystok, 2001; Ricciardelli, 1992). Bilingual advantages in metalinguistic
awareness have been examined at different levels of language processing. To probe
metalinguistic awareness at the phonological level (also see Bialystok, Luk, and Kwan, 2005;
Bialystok, Majumder, and Martin, 2003; Campbell and Sais, 1995; Rubin and Turner, 1989),
Chen et al. (2004) compared the performance of first-, second-, and fourth-graders who were
either Mandarin monolinguals or Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Children performed same-
different tasks, where they had to judge whether two words sounded the same or different in
terms of tone, onset phoneme, or rime. In addition children performed oddity tasks, where they
had to choose the odd one out (in terms of tone, onset phoneme, or rime) upon hearing three
words. Overall, bilingual children outperformed monolingual children in terms of tone
2
The day / night Stroop task, based on the classical color-ink Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), is a cognitive control task
developed for use with children, and assesses children‘s ability to apply new linguistic labels and inhibit
previously-learned linguistic labels. Children are taught to say ―day‖ when they see a card with a moon on it,
and to say ―night‖ when they see a card with a sun on it. Young children have great difficulty with this task,
with performance improving around 6 years of age.
Bilingual Language Development 51
awareness (Cantonese has a richer tone system than Mandarin), as well as for onset phoneme
and rime conditions. The authors suggested that bilingual children‘s exposure to two sound
systems lent them an advantage in terms of their analyses of sound units, especially at young
ages. Specifically, bilinguals‘ advantage was strong in the second grade, but became weaker in
the fourth grade.3 Research across a number of different phonological awareness tasks and age
groups suggests that bilingual children may not show phonological awareness advantages
across all tasks at all ages (e.g., onset rime awareness, syllable awareness, phoneme awareness,
etc), but rather that advantages are specific and shift as children get older, and with the onset
of literacy (e.g., Bruck and Genessee, 1995; Chen et al., 2004).
To probe metalinguistic awareness at the lexical level (also see Yelland, Pollard, and
Mercuri, 1993), Bialystok (1988) had 6- and 7-year old bilinguals and monolinguals perform
two word tasks to evaluate their sense that word labels are arbitrary. On a word substitution
task (for early versions of this task, see Piaget, 1929, and Ianco-Worral, 1972), children were
asked ―…suppose […] everybody decided to call the sun ‗the moon‘ and the moon ‗the sun‘.
What would you call the thing in the sky when you go to bed at night?‖ Bialystok predicted
that the word substitution task would measure both (a) children‘s sense of the arbitrariness of
words and (b) their cognitive control in maintaining word substitutions, and found that
bilingual children outperformed monolingual children on this task. In a more abstract task,
Bialystok then had children participate in a word concept definition task, where children were
asked ―What is a word? How can you tell if something is a word?‖ Bialystok found that
bilingual children were better than monolingual children at describing the concept of what a
word is, with partially bilingual children performing better than monolinguals, but worse than
proficient bilinguals.
Finally, to probe metalinguistic awareness at the grammatical level, Ricciardelli (1992)
had 5- and 6-year old bilinguals and monolinguals perform a word-order correction task,
where they were asked to help a puppet character correct her sentences (also see Bialystok,
1988). Children were presented with sentences such as ―Daddy the car washes‖. The authors
found that bilingual children were more successful at correcting these sentences than
monolingual children were. The authors suggest that, for bilingual children, increased
exposure to multiple structures allows them to think about language more flexibly, allowing
them to recognize the intended sentences, and produce appropriate corrections. Together,
findings of metalinguistic awareness across different language processing levels suggest that
bilingual children may develop a greater linguistic flexibility than their monolingual peers.
In addition to tasks that probe metalinguistic awareness at various processing levels, other
findings comparing bilinguals‘ and monolinguals‘ linguistic processing also suggest bilingual
advantages that may be linked to more detailed analysis of language in bilingual children. At
the syntactic-semantic interface, Sheng, McGregor, and Marian (2007) recently found that 5-
to 8-year old bilingual children may reach the syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift earlier than their
monolingual peers. The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift marks a gradual re-organization of the
child‘s semantic system, where children shift from closely associating words that co-occur at
the sentence level (e.g., cold and outside) to closely associating words that occur in similar
contexts (e.g., cold and hot). This shift is believed to require a re-analysis of language that
3
The weaker bilingual advantage by the fourth grade is likely due to the fact that both monolingual and bilingual
children receive more explicit instruction about their language systems as they progress in school and may, as a
result, perform at ceiling in many phonological awareness tasks.
52 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
progresses from analyses of surface structure towards analysis of internal structural patterns. It
is possible that a quicker re-organization in bilingual children‘s semantic systems is brought
about by a greater awareness that the surface level is in fact arbitrary and serves to
communicate a consistent underlying system of meaning (e.g., see Bybee, 2001). In sum,
influences of bilingual exposure on processing can be observed throughout the linguistic
system, including the phonological, lexical, grammatical, and semantic levels. It is likely that
monolingual / bilingual differences across these processing levels can be attributed to
differences in processing demands as bilingual children construct their linguistic systems
across these levels.
Bilinguals‘ superior performance on these two inhibitory control tasks has been shown to
extend into adulthood (e.g., Bialystok, 2005). Others have replicated a bilingual advantage in
young adults on the nonlinguistic arrow inhibition task (Blumenfeld and Marian, in
preparation) and on Stroop-like flanker inhibition tasks (e.g., Costa, Hernandez, and Sebastián-
Gallés, 2008; Colzato et al., 2008), that involve identification of an arrow‘s direction (―<‖
points left), given surrounding arrows that point the opposite way (e.g., ―>>>> < >>>>‖). In
adult bilinguals who had learned both of their languages at an early age, Blumenfeld and
Marian (in preparation) recently showed a close correlation between performance on the
nonlinguistic arrow inhibition task and inhibition of similar-sounding words during language
comprehension. Therefore, early evidence suggests that inhibitory control performance on a
task that yields a bilingual advantage as early as at 4-6 years of age in bilingual children
remains a task at which adult bilinguals outperform monolinguals, and is directly related to
processing of ambiguous information during auditory word comprehension. As discussed in
the first section of the present chapter, bilingual children face language input that spans two
linguistic codes, resulting in more frequent ambiguity during language learning, both at the
phonological and lexical levels. We can thus speculate that a link between nonlinguistic
inhibitory control and auditory comprehension processing in bilingual adults is tied to
language development4, as children start to separate and elaborate two phonological codes and
start to extend them into two language systems that have to be easily accessible, with minimal
interference between the two systems.
DEVELOPMENTAL MECHANISMS
OF BILINGUAL / MONOLINGUAL DIFFERENCES
4
Whether second language development in later childhood or in adulthood can also be linked to changes in
nonlinguistic inhibitory control mechanisms remains largely unstudied, but evidence is emerging that cognitive
changes, perhaps of a somewhat different nature, are also linked to late bilingualism (e.g., Linck,, Kroll, &
Sunderman, in press).
54 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
Inhibitory control mechanisms, which aid in correct identification of one response, while
another response is suppressed, have traditionally been localized to prefrontal cortex during
neuroimaging studies and have been shown to develop slowly throughout childhood (e.g.,
Booth, Burman, Meyer, Lei, Trommer, Davenport, et al., 2003).
It is likely that development of inhibitory processes has its origin in children‘s first
confrontations with duality. For example, the ―terrible twos‖ generally mark a stage when
children begin to understand that their own wishes do not necessarily coincide with those of
others, and the word ―no‖ reaches peak frequencies of usage as children systematically explore
this phenomenon (e.g., Gopnik, Meltzoff, and Kuhl, 1999). Despite evidence that inhibitory
control processes may start to develop early in childhood, findings from neuroimaging suggest
that prefrontal cortex and executive control functions are not fully developed until the late
teens and early twenties (e.g., Luciana, Conklin, Hooper, and Yarger, 2005). Therefore,
language acquisition proceeds in the absence of fully developed cognitive control mechanisms.
This general delay in development of executive function, together with findings that childhood
bilingualism results in cognitive advantages in inhibitory control within the nonlinguistic
domain, suggests that bilingual experience may in some instances support the gradual
development of children‘s inhibitory control processes. Therefore, findings comparing
bilingual and monolingual children‘s performance on inhibitory control tasks suggest that
linguistic development may influence cognitive development. Specifically, it is likely that the
processing demands associated with language development train and hone cognitive function.
An example of such a potential relationship may be the case of inhibitory control and selective
attention, which provides a useful tool at various stages of phonological and lexical
acquisition. Use of these mechanisms within the context of language may render them
increasingly efficient as language acquisition proceeds and as language processing demands
grow. Conversely, more developed cognitive mechanisms are likely to constrain and guide
language development. For example, the cognitive constraint instantiated with the mutual
exclusivity principle aids children in directing their attention towards objects or features that
have not previously been labeled and might therefore map onto novel phonological input.
Similarly, instinctive use of cues in the visual environment creates an important framework for
mapping phonological sequences onto the visual world.
In sum, although a large aspect of monolingual language acquisition research is dedicated
to how children make use of cognitive mechanisms during language acquisition, and how
language acquisition may trigger the development of cognitive mechanisms, a need remains
for research and theoretical frameworks that explicitly link aspects of language development to
aspects of cognitive control. Establishing direct links between linguistic and cognitive domains
will have practical applications for education and speech-language services. In addition,
establishing links between linguistic and cognitive processes has theoretical implications.
Specifically, the finding that early bilingual exposure influences nonlinguistic cognitive
control functions provides strong support for a domain-general view of language where
general cognitive mechanisms are intimately involved with language processing. Clarification
of how cognitive and linguistic processes interact will further constrain the debate on
modularity vs. domain-generality, identifying which aspects of language can be tied to general
cognitive mechanisms, and which aspects may be domain-specific.
Bilingual Language Development 55
English bilinguals, was acquiring two languages that had more similar phonetic inventories.
With Chinese-English bilingual children, Bialystok, Majumder, and Martin (2003) found a
disadvantage over monolinguals on phoneme segmentation. In contrast, with Spanish-English
bilingual children, the authors found an advantage over monolinguals on phoneme
segmentation. Given the differences between Chinese and English phonetic inventories, and
the relative similarities of Spanish and English phonetic inventories, it is possible that, while
Chinese-English children did not receive enough positive evidence for phonetic distinctions in
their languages, Spanish-English children did receive enough positive evidence and were able
to derive benefit from complex input.
In addition to the linguistic system, complexity and frequency of input may also influence
the cognitive system in bilingual children. Specifically, Wexler (1982) suggested that more
complex input requires more processing resources. It is possible, then, that children who
routinely receive language input that is more complex than the input of monolingual peers may
consistently recruit more cognitive resources for processing. Such increased recruitment and
repeated use of resources may eventually result in a more efficient processing system, as
evidenced by findings of bilingual processing advantages in the nonlinguistic domain (e.g.,
Martin-Rhee and Bialystok, 2008). Bjorklund and Harnishfeger‘s (1990) proposed a model of
limited resources, where increased recruitment of resources towards processing was posited to
incur costs at the storage (i.e., representational) level. Bialystok (1999) proposed a similar
analysis-control model to account for frequently observed cognitive advantages in bilingual
children, as well as for possible early representational limitations, evidenced in slower
bilingual vocabulary growth relative to monolingual peers (e.g., Verhoeven, 1994). While
learnability theory is typically concerned with linguistic behavior, given certain kinds of input,
the interaction between language and cognition in development can also be thought of within a
class of models known as usage-based accounts.
they differ from most current connectionist models in their functional approach. In short, the
intent to communicate and the ability to direct attention provide strong top-down biases on
perceptually based (i.e., bottom-up) language learning.
The linguistic challenges faced by bilingual children support a functionalist account of
language acquisition that incorporates an attentional component. Many cognitive differences
between monolingual and bilingual children emerge in the area of selective attention and
inhibitory control. For example, infants who acquire two languages have to orient towards one
language vs. the other, and separate two phonological and (later) lexical and grammatical
systems. Moreover, once bilingual children acquire language, they are likely to make more
decisions (such as what language to speak in) than their monolingual peers, necessitating a
cognitive/pragmatic decision component that is in close contact with the language system. In
this way, the manner in which bilingual children meet linguistic challenges may be more
clearly explained within a language learning account that incorporates an explicit attentional
component.
Although a strong case can be made for the presence of higher-level attentional
components in a functional framework of bilingual language acquisition, the consensus within
usage-based accounts remains that linguistic and cognitive learning is driven by input. At the
beginning of life, children listen to their environment and orient towards sources of salient
input. At this stage of development, the influence of goal-oriented cognitive biases is likely to
be minimal, with a focus on perceiving linguistic input, and identifying initial patterns in
making sense of this information stream through the use of general cognitive mechanisms. A
current developmental connectionist model of bilingual processing is the Self-Organizing
Model of Bilingual Processing (SOMBIP, Li and Farkas, 2002). The SOMBIP posits that the
bilingual system is dynamically organized with language acquisition, based on the principle of
lateral inhibition between representations. As such, inhibitory relationships emerge as the
bilingual system develops. Inhibitory control mechanisms within the Self-Organizing Model of
Bilingual Processing are language-specific, and are tightly linked to language representations
(despite the fact that lateral inhibition is a general and system-wide mechanism).
As bilingual representations are organized in the process of bilingual acquisition, native-
language and second-language representations form clusters, with lateral inhibition
mechanisms reinforcing this trend (Li and Farkas, 2002). As a consequence, it is likely that
stronger lateral inhibitory connections are present between language clusters (since activation
of one language typically implies de-activation of the other) than within language clusters
(since activation of within-language words co-occurs). Therefore, while the same inhibitory
control mechanism is present both within-language and between-language, it may act more
strongly across languages. An account of the present literature on bilingual development in the
context of the Self-Organizing Model of Bilingual Processing is particularly appropriate,
because the model is a dynamic developmental model, and may therefore be most suitable for
the description of emergence of linguistic and cognitive changes with bilingual experience.
Developmental models such as SOMBIP are heavily based on frequency of exposure to
language exemplars, and are based on the assumption that children derive linguistic knowledge
and structure from their linguistic environment. Empirical research suggests that this
assumption is a reasonable one. At the linguistic level, evidence exists that input frequency
influences learning. For example, research with infants suggests an especially strong reliance
on word frequency during word recognition at early stages of word learning, with gradual
maturation of the phonological system leading to better abilities to segment both high-
58 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
frequency words and nonwords from the speech stream (Singh, Nestor, and Bortfeld, 2008).
Nicoladis, Palmer, and Marendette (2007) showed that the accuracy with which French-
English bilingual 4- and 6-year olds produce regular and irregular verbs in their two languages
depends on the frequencies of these verbs within their input language (English and French
have different frequency distributions for regular and irregular verbs). Nicoladis (2005) found
similar frequency effects in a longitudinal case study of a child between the ages of 2 years 8
months and 5 years.
In general, words with high frequencies of occurrence in the language are more likely to
be words that children acquire early. A study with 8- and 13-year old monolingual and
bilingual children suggests that children perform better at recognizing high-frequency words
than at recognizing low frequency words (Windsor and Kohnert, 2004, but see findings by
Goldstein, Fabiano, and Washington, 2005, concerning overall frequency of language
exposure). Finally, recent evidence suggests that frequency-of-exposure effects may reach
beyond the word-level, with 17-month olds showing successful sound-to-meaning mappings
only for words that they had previously been exposed to during fluent speech, with
opportunities for segmentation (Graf Estes, Evans, Alibali, and Saffran, 2007), and with 2-year
olds showing better repetition of 4-word chunks that frequently occur in language input (e.g.,
―sit in your chair‖) than of chunks that are less frequent (Bannard and Matthews, 2008).
At the cognitive level, evidence of exposure-based influences is more sparse. Ricciardelli
(1992) compared monolingual and bilingual 5- and 6-year olds at different language
proficiencies on a number of cognitive tasks. Bilingual children only showed cognitive
performance advantages over their monolingual peers when they were highly proficient in both
of their languages. Based on these data, Ricciardelli proposed a Threshold Model of Bilingual
Cognitive Development (also see Cummins, 1979), with development of bilingual cognitive
advantages relying on the child reaching a certain level of competence in both languages. In an
earlier version of this model, Cummins (1979) had proposed a two-tiered threshold model,
where children‘s failure to reach the first (and lower) threshold of bilingual language
proficiency would result in cognitive performance lower than that of monolingual peers,
children‘s reaching the first (and lower) threshold of bilingual language proficiency would
ensure cognitive performance similar to that of monolingual peers, and children‘s reaching the
second (and higher) threshold of bilingual proficiency would ensure certain cognitive
advantages. This model is consistent with the idea that cognitive changes develop gradually
and dynamically as children acquire their two languages (e.g., for behavioral evidence of
gradually emergent cognitive changes see Bialystok, 1988; Kaushanskaya and Marian, 2008;
Yelland, Polard, and Mercuri, 1993), and it accounts for frequent variability in the literature in
terms of finding bilingual advantages.
For example, Kaushanskaya and Marian (2008) found that adult bilinguals who learned
their second language early in life (i.e., at an average of 3 years of age) were more successful
than monolinguals at learning novel words in adulthood and showed changes from
monolinguals in how they recruited working memory mechanisms to recall novel words. In
contrast, a group of bilinguals who learned their second language later in life (i.e., at an
average age of 12 years of age) showed trends towards the performance pattern of early
bilinguals, both in terms of learning novel words and in terms of recruitment of working
memory mechanisms (also see Van Hell and Mahn, 1997). Consistent with the threshold
model, it is likely that reaching of bilingual competence early in life conferred a set of
cognitive benefits on early bilingual participants that were reflected in different relationships
Bilingual Language Development 59
between linguistic and cognitive processing, and in a lasting improvement in the ability to
learn new verbal material. In sum, bilingual children who show cognitive advantages are
typically raised in environments where both languages are frequently used from an early age,
and adult studies that have found bilingual advantages have also featured highly proficient
bilingual participants.
An emerging literature on bilingual language development suggests that, from its earliest
stages, bilingual acquisition differs from monolingual acquisition, not only in the fact that two
language systems are created instead of one, but also in terms of how cognitive resources are
employed to accomplish this end. While current studies are mostly concentrated in the
linguistic and nonlinguistic domains, research is needed that directly examines links between
linguistic and cognitive processes during bilingual development, in order to more fully specify
a developmental account of language-cognition interactions in bilinguals. Given current
findings in the linguistic and nonlinguistic domains, a picture of bilingual development is
emerging where increased complexity within the perceived input recruits and shapes cognitive
processes of selective attention and inhibitory control at early stages of language learning.
Table 1 provides a brief summary of this picture, where language acquisition challenges that
children face as they become bilingual are related to nonlinguistic, metalinguistic, and
linguistic aspects of processing that may be related to these learning hurdles.
While no current model of bilingual development can fully account for the confluence of
factors that interact during bilingual language development, we believe that a number of
developmental models make important contributions, and may in the future be combined into a
wider framework of linguistic / cognitive development. Specifically, connectionist frameworks
can effectively simulate children‘s statistical learning of regularities and patterns within the
phonological input, and the Self-Organizing Map of Bilingual Processing by Li and Farkas
(2002) realistically simulates bilingual infants‘ separation of auditory input into two distinct
phonological systems, based on the cognitive mechanism of lateral inhibition. Nevertheless,
the connectionist framework is currently lacking the functional cognitive dimension that is
likely to be an important contributor and guide to bilingual acquisition.
While adult models of bilingual language processing posit levels of intentionality and
language control (Dijkstra and Van Heuven, 2002; Green, 1998), the origin of such levels in
development has not currently been addressed. In the spirit of usage-based accounts, it may be
possible to derive cognitive mechanisms that support bilingual language development and
processing from domain-general cognitive resources, with increased efficiency and specificity
of cognitive mechanisms visible as the system matures and as language development
progresses.
60 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
In general, the current state of science on bilingual development suggests that a broader
approach must be taken towards modeling language development, with a tight link to cognitive
development, and a cognitive system that encounters duality and conflict, and is trained to
appropriately resolve such situations. Importantly, the mutual influence of linguistic and
cognitive development on each other must be represented, such that linguistic exposure can
effectively trigger changes in cognitive control mechanisms. Finally, current functionalist
approaches can account for the demands of bilingual language acquisition, with language
development driven by an intention to communicate, and to achieve specific communicative
goals.
Bilingual Language Development 61
Childhood bilingualism provides a unique context for examining the interaction between
linguistic and cognitive mechanisms in development. Specifically, evidence that bilingual
children differ from their monolingual peers in the linguistic and nonlinguistic domains,
supports the view that general cognitive tools may support language acquisition. In the words
of Liz Bates, ―[n]ature is a miser. She clothes her children in hand-me-downs, builds new
machinery in makeshift fashion from sundry old parts, and saves genetic expenditures
whenever she can‖ (Bates, 1979, p. 1). More recently, Gary Marcus wrote about language
―[w]here Shakespeare imagined infinite reason, I see something else, what engineers call a
―kluge.‖ A kluge is a clumsy or inelegant – yet surprisingly effective – solution to a problem.
Nature is prone to making kluges because it doesn‘t ―care‖ whether its products are perfect or
elegant. […] Adequacy not beauty is the name of the game‖ (Marcus, 2008, pp. 3-6).
Research in bilingual language development confirms the notion that children build a
language using general cognitive guides and building blocks, by showing that linguistic
experience in childhood seems to influence general cognitive processing mechanisms. Future
research on childhood bilingualism may further elucidate specific influences of bilingual input
on cognitive processing, while taking into consideration important factors in bilingualism
research, such as age of acquisition, language dominance, language exposure, the specific
language pairs that are being acquired, etc. (e.g., see Marian, 2008). Considering such factors
is important for theoretical reasons, in order to isolate specific mechanisms and thresholds for
language-cognition interaction, and for practical reasons, in order to define specific cognitive
consequences of childhood bilingualism across a tremendously diverse population of bilingual
children (e.g., see Goldstein, 2000).
Although bilingualism is pervasive (e.g., US Census, 2000), it is sometimes seen as a
disadvantage rather than an advantage, especially in education settings where English non-
native bilingual children may initially lag behind their monolingual peers in vocabulary
development (e.g., Verhoeven, 1994). It is less well-known that bilingualism may yield
significant cognitive benefits, both in childhood development and across the lifespan (e.g.,
Bialystok, Craik, Klein, and Viswanathan, 2004; Bialystok, Craik, and Freedman, 2007).
Research on how linguistic input influences cognitive capacities is increasingly necessary to
make the case for the teaching and maintenance of two languages in the education system.
Findings that cognitive benefits can be tied to aspects of bilingual processing may encourage
creation of language-learning environments where English non-native children can maintain
their first language (such as dual-immersion programs), and may provide support for early
foreign-language learning and immersion in standard curricula.
In sum, we propose that the study of language-cognition interactions in childhood
bilingualism can inform dynamics of language development in general, as well as the nature of
language. Specifically, current evidence that bilingual exposure during childhood results in
cognitive changes in the nonlinguistic domain lends support to the view that language is not a
strictly modular system, but is highly interactive with general cognitive mechanisms, and is
therefore (at least in part) a domain-general system. As a corollary, this recent evidence lends
support to usage-based accounts of language acquisition, with linguistic systems emerging
given statistical analysis of spoken input and general cognitive mechanisms that guide and bias
62 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
learning. The view of bilingual development that is proposed here is consistent with the view
of a dynamic bilingual system, where continuous reorganization of representation goes hand in
hand with language learning, and the system evolves with continued bilingual exposure (e.g.,
Kroll and Stewart, 1994; Li and Farkas, 1998). Recent evidence (e.g., Spivey, 2006) suggests
that, not only do linguistic representations undergo continuous re-organization during language
acquisition, but general nonlinguistic representations may also be honed and re-shaped with
continued exposure to specific language environments. The present chapter makes first strides
in extending this dynamic model to include higher-level cognitive function in bilinguals.
AUTHOR NOTE
Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by NIH grant NICHD-1R03HD046952-
01A1 to Viorica Marian. The authors would like to thank Margarita Kaushanskaya, Anthony
Shook, Scott Schroeder, and Caroline Engstler for comments and feedback on earlier drafts of
this manuscript.
REFERENCES
Au, T.K. and Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: To
honor or not to honor? Child Development, 61 (5), 1474-1490.
Bannard, C., and Matthews, D.E. (2008) Stored Word Sequences in Language Learning: The
Effect of Familarity on Children's Repetition of Four-Word Combinations. Psychological
Science , 19, 241-248.
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New
York: Academic Press.
Bates, E., Dale, P.S., and Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for
theories of language development. In P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of
Child Language. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1995.
Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the Competition Model. In B.
MacWhinney and E. Bates (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In B.
MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Bates, E., Thal, D., and Janowsky, J. (1992). Early language development and its neural
correlates. In I. Rapin and S. Segalowitz (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 7:
Child Neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bates, E., Thal, D., and Marchman, V. (1991). Symbols and syntax: A Darwinian approach to
language development. In N. Krasnegor, D. Rumbaugh, E. Schiefelbusch and M. Studdert-
Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and Behavioral Determinants of Language Development.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 29 - 65.
Bhatia, T., and Ritchie, W. (1999). The bilingual child: some issues and perspectives. In W.C.
Ritchie and T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of child language acquisition (pp. 569–643).
San Diego: Academic Press.
Bilingual Language Development 63
Fodor, J.A. (1985). Multiple book review of 'The modularity of mind'. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 8, 1-42.
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Gierut, J.A. (2007). Phonological complexity and language learnability. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 6-17.
Gierut, J.A., and Dale, R.A. (2007). Comparability of lexical corpora: Word frequency in
phonological generalization. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21 (6), 423-433.
Goldstein, B.A. (2000). Cultural and linguistic diversity resource guide for speech-language
pathologists. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.
Goldstein, B. A., and Fabiano, L. (2007). Assessment and intervention for bilingual children
with phonological disorders. The ASHA Leader, 12 (2), 6-7, 26-27, 31.
Goldstein, B.A., Fabiano, L., and Washington, P.S. (2005). Phonological skills in
predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish-English
bilingual children. Language Speech and Hearing Sciences in Schools, 36 (3), 201-218.
Golinkoff, R., Mervis, C. and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994) Early Object labels: The case for a
developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21 (1), 125-156.
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A.N., and Kuhl, P.K. (1999). The Scientist in the Crib: Minds, Brains,
and How Children Learn. New York, NY: William Morrow and Company, Inc.
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J.L., Alibali, M., and Saffran, J.R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to
newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological
Science, 18 (2), 254-260.
Green, D.W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism:
Language and Cognition, 1, 67-81.
Ianco-Worral, A. (1972). Bilingualism and cognitive development. Child Development, 43,
1390-1400.
Ingram, D. (1982). The emerging phonological system of an Italian-English bilingual child.
Journal of Italian Linguistics, 95-113.
Johnson, C.J. (1994). Inhibitory mechanisms in selection among multiple object names.
Cognitive Development, 9, 293-309.
Ju, M., and Luce, P.A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears. Psychological Science, 15(5), 314-
318.
Kaplan, F., Oudeyer, P.-Y., and Bergen, B. (2008). Computational models in the debate over
language learnability. Infant and Child Development, 17, 55-80.
Kaushanskaya, M., and Marian, V. (2008). Age-of-acquisition effects in the development of a
bilingual advantage for word learning. Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Boston University
Conference on Language Development, 213-224. Cascadilla Press; Somerville, MA.
Kehoe, M. (2002) Developing vowel systems as a window to bilingual phonology.
International Journal of Bilingualism, 6 (3), 315-334.
Kehoe, M., Trujill, C., and Lleo, C. (2001). Phonological acquisition of bilingual children: An
analysis of syllable structure and voice onset time. In K. Cantone and M. Hinzelin (Eds.),
Proceedings of the Colloquium on Structure, Acquisition, and Change of Grammars:
Phonological and Syntactic Aspects, Volume 2 (pp. 38-54). Working Papers in
Multilingualism. Series B.
Keshavarz, M., and Ingram, D. (2002). The early phonological development of a Farsi English
bilingual child. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 255–269.
66 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
Kroll, J. F., and Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming:
Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal
of Memory and Language, 33, 149-174.
Kuhl, P. (2000). Language, mind, and brain: Experience alters perception. In M.S. Gazzaniga
(Ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 99-115). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Leopold, W. (1970). Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist's record. (Volumes
1–4). New York: AMS Press.
Li, P., and Farkas, I. (2002). A self-organizing connectionist model of bilingual processing. In
R. Heredia and J. Altarriba (Eds.), Bilingual Sentence Processing (pp. 59-85). Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., and Sunderman, G. (in press). Losing access to the native language
while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second
language learning. Psychological Science.
Luciana, M., Conklin, H.M., Hooper, C., Yarger, R. (2005). The development of nonverbal
working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development, 76,
697-712.
Marcus, G. (2008). Kluge: The Haphazard Construction of the Human Mind. Boston, MA:
Houghton Mifflin Company.
Marcus, G. F. (1993). Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition, 46, 53-85.
Marian, V. (2009). Audio-visual integration during bilingual language processing. In A.
Pavlenko (Ed.), The Bilingual Mental Lexicon. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Marian, V. (2008). Bilingual research methods. In J. Altarriba and R. R. Heredia (Eds.), An
Introduction to Bilingualism: Principles and Processes. Mahawah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Marian, V., and Spivey, M. (2003a). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing
lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173-193.
Marian, V., and Spivey, M. (2003b). Competing activation in bilingual language processing:
Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97-
115.
Markman, E.M., and Wachtel, G.F. (1988). Children‘s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain
the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121-157.
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition,
25, 71-102.
Martin-Rhee, M.M. and Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory
control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11,
81-93.
Mattock, K., and Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants‘ tone perception: Evidence
for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10 (3), 241-265.
Mattock, K. , Polka, L. and Rvachew, S. (2006, Jun) "Delay? No way! Bilingual infants are
more efficient word learners" Paper presented at the annual meeting of the XVth Biennial
International Conference on Infant Studies, Westin Miyako, Kyoto, Japan.
Maye, J., Werker, J.F., and Gerken, L.A. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information
can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101-B111.
McClelland, J.L., and Elman, J.L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive
Psychology, 18, 1-86.
Bilingual Language Development 67
Mikes, M. (1990). Some issues of lexical development in early bi- and trilinguals. In G. Conti-
Ramsden and C.E. Snow (Eds), Children‘s Language, Volume 7. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Nakamichi, K. (2007). Young children‘s conditional reasoning, working memory, and
inhibitory control. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 55 (3), 347-358.
Nazzi, T. et al. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the
role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 24, 765-766.
Nicoladis, E. (2008). Bilingualism and language cognitive development. In J. Altarriba and R.
R. Heredia (Eds.), An Introduction to Bilingualism: Principles and Processes. Mahawah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Nicoladis, E. (2005). The acquisition of complex deverbal words by a French-English
bilingual child. Language Learning, 55 (3), 415-443.
Nicoladis, E., Palmer, A., and Marentette, P. (2007). The role of type and token frequency in
using past tense morphemes correctly. Developmental Science, 10 (2), 237-254.
Norman, D., and Shallice, T., 1986: Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of
behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, and D. Shapiro, (Eds.) Consciousness and Self
Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory, Volume 4. Plenum, New York, NY.
Paradis, J. (2001). Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems?
International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 19-38.
Piaget, J. (1929). The Child‘s Conception of the World. London, UK: Routlege and Kegan
Paul.
Pinker, S. (1984) Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Polka, L., and Werker, J.F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of non-native vowel
contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20,
241-435.
Posner, M., Rothbart, M., Farah, M., and Bruer, J. (2001). The developing human brain.
Developmental Science, 4, 235-387.
Quine (1960). Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ricciardelli, L. (1992). Bilingualism and cognitive development in relation to threshold theory.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21 (4), 301-316.
Romaine, S. (1995). Bilingualism. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Roseberry-McKibbin, C., and Brice, A. (2000). Acquiring English as a second language:
What‘s "normal," what‘s not. The ASHA Leader, 5 (12), 4-7.
Rubin, H., and Turner, A. (1989). Linguistic awareness skills in grade one children in a French
immersion setting. Reading and Writing, 1, 73-86.
Sebastián-Gallés, N., and Bosch, L. (2005). Phonology and bilingualism. In Handbook of
Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (Kroll, J.F., and de Groot, A.M.B., eds), pp.
68-87. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Sekerina, I., and Brooks, P.J. (2007). Eye movements during spoken word recognition in
Russian children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 20-45.
Sheng, L., McGregor, K., and Marian, V. (2007). Lexical-semantic organization in bilingual
children: evidence from a repeated word association task. Journal of Speech, Language
and Hearing Research, 49, 572-587.
68 Henrike K. Blumenfeld and Viorica Marian
Simon, J.R. (1969). Reaction toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 81, 174-176.
Simpson, G.B., and Lorsbach, T.C. (1983). The development of automatic and conscious
components of contextual facilitation. Child Development, 54 (3), 760-772.
Singh, L., Nestor, S.S., and Bortfield, H. (2008). Overcoming the effects of variation in infant
speech segmentation: Influences of word familiarity. Infancy, 13 (1), 57-74.
Smith, L.B., Jones, S.S., Landau, B., Gershkoff-Stowe, L., and Samuelson, L. (2002). Object
name learning provides on-the-job training for attention. Psychological Science, 13, 13-19.
Spivey, M. (2006). The Continuity of Mind. New York, NY : Oxford University Press.
Soto-Faraco, S., Navarra, J., Weikum, W.M., Vouloumanos, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., and
Werker, J.F. (2007). Discriminating languages by speech reading. Perception and
Psychophysics, 69 (2), 218-237.
Stager, C.L., and Werker, J.F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech
perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381-382.
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 18, 643-662.
Tomasello (2007). Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition.
Massachusetts, MA: Harvard University Press.
U.S. Census (2000). Retrieved from www.census.gov/population/www/socdemo/lang_use.
html.
Van Hell, J.G., and Mahn, A.C. (1997). Keyword mnemonics versus rote rehearsal: Learning
concrete and abstract foreign words by experienced and inexperienced learners. Language
Learning, 47, 507-546.
Verhoeven (1994). Transfer in bilingual development: The linguistic interdependency
hypothesis revisited. Language Learning, 44, 381-415.
Vihman, M. M. (2002). Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in
bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6 (3), 239-254.
Weber, A., and Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken- word recognition.
Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 1-25.
Weikum, W., Vouloumanos, A., Navarro, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., and
Werker, J.F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316 (5828), 1159.
Werker, J. F., and Byers-Heinlein, K. (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: first steps in perception
and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (12), 144-151.
Werker, J.F., Polka, L. and Pegg, J.E. (1997). The conditioned head turn procedure as a
method for testing infant speech perception. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 171-
178.
Werker, J.F., and Tees, R.C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for
perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7,
49-63.
Wexler (1982). A principle theory for language acquisition. In E. Wanner and L.R. Gleitman
(Eds.), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art (pp. 288-315). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Windsor, J., and Kohnert, K. (2004). The search for common ground: part I. Lexical
performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing
Research, 47 (4), 877-890.
Bilingual Language Development 69
Chapter 3
ABSTRACT
This paper discusses some topics that stem from recent contributions made by the
history, the philosophy, and the didactics of science. We consider these relevant to the
introduction of the special relativity theory in secondary education, adopting a
contextualized approach from a historical, epistemological and pedagogical point of
view.
The results of several investigations into the teaching and learning of the Special
Relativity Theory are presented. We discuss the pedagogical approach that we consider
appropriate to introduce this theory at high school level as well as how the treatment
given to the theory by the textbooks more commonly used by teachers can influence their
pedagogical decisions and the students‘ understanding of this topic.
We analyze the mental images of relevant concepts of Mechanics that Argentine
secondary students have and which are necessary in order to understand the Special
Relativity Theory. The results seem to indicate that the examined group of students has
not developed appropriate schemata to solve problems in which these concepts are
involved. Based on these results, we have formulated some objectives-obstacles that
should be overcome by students in a classroom context adopting a specific teaching
proposal that will provide for the significant learning of the special relativity theory.
*
[email protected].
†
[email protected].
72 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
We offer an outline of a teaching proposal to deal with the Special Relativity Theory
in Argentina at high school level. This proposal was designed within a theoretical
framework that takes into account epistemological, historical, psychological and
pedagogical aspects which are compatible among themselves.
1. INTRODUCTION
Albert Einstein is possibly the first Science celebrity who emerged from the media age
we live in. He was associated with ideas of transgression and rebelliousness. In addition to
this, several of the ideas related to his theory of relativity have been linked to the plots of
many science fiction productions. Although these connections could be arguable from the
point of view of science, they have certainly taken root in the minds of many young people.
Research into the different aspects of the incorporation of Contemporary Physics into high
school teaching has been carried out on a sample of students from more than fifty percent of
the Argentine provinces. The results of the research revealed that it is precisely the theory of
relativity the best-known reference to Physics among students, and that it is the one they
would be most interested in learning about. Nowadays it is extremely hard for teachers to
motivate teenage students to study Physics. Considering this, the good press given to
Einstein, especially during the World Year of Physics 2005, as well as his scientific
achievements, would contribute to the students‘ interest in the brilliant Special Relativity
Theory (SRT).1
On the other hand, the SRT is an especially rich theme for Science Education since the
students‘ first encounter with this theory should mean a crucial point in the learning of
Physics given the fact that the sense of continuity that may exist between traditional Physics
and relativist Physics is less relevant than what makes them different. In view of this, it is
desirable that students should develop a conceptual evolution as regards the SRT. We use this
term in the sense conveyed by Toulmin2 who uses it to refer to the coexistence of both
conceptions. The student is then able to identify the scope of validity of the ―old‖ conception
and to accept the truth and efficiency of the ―new‖ one.
Limited research work has been done on the students‘ and teachers‘ understanding of
concepts specifically connected with the SRT; most of the research dealing with concepts
related to the notion of relativity has to do with the relative movement within the Galilean
context. There exist many studies aimed at particular aspects of the SRT even though these
are mainly found within the sphere of university education, and to a lesser extent at secondary
level. (Saltiel y Malgrange, 1980; Hewson, 1982; Posner et al., 1982; Villani y Pacca, 1987;
Panse et al., 1994; Ramadas et al., 1996; Aleman Berenguer, 1997; Pietrocola e Zylbersztajn,
1999; Aleman Berenger y Pérez Selles, 2000; Martins y Pacca, 2005).
During the last few years, different approaches to teaching the SRT have been tried out
with quite varied results (e. g.: Bertali et al., 1979; G.I.R.E.P., 1991; Solbes, 1986 y Borghi et
al. 1993 en Villani y Arruda, 1998). On the one hand, some results obtained would suggest
that the ―enthusiasm‖ for learning the SRT could help overcome the difficulty that this
entails. On the other hand, however, according to some less optimistic results, the students do
1
Capuano et al., 1997.
2
Toulmin, 1997.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 73
not interpret the relativist concepts taking the new concepts provided by the SRT as a starting
point, but in the frame of their spontaneous beliefs. What is more, according to those results,
the students‘ acceptance of this theory would be just temporary since it is opposed to their
deep convictions3. The difficulties one faces when teaching the SRT are certainly numerous.
Perhaps the most serious ones are the teachers‘ own professional training and the debatable
way in which many textbooks introduce the SRT giving distorted conceptual, historical and
epistemological views. That is the case in several countries such as Spain, Brazil, and
Argentina, where similar research has been conducted4.
In spite of these real obstacles, we reject the kind of statements that suggest that the SRT
is too abstract or that the students are not ready for it. This is sometimes argued by some
teachers in order not to introduce this topic (however valid it could prove to be.) Considering
the students‘ everyday experience, the traditional Mechanics we teach is also abstract.
According to the results of research into the students‘ understanding of traditional Mechanics
issues, the students have difficulty in constructing models in accordance to the ones
scientifically accepted by this theory which is considered to be more intuitive than others
including the SRT. Once again, it is worth stressing the importance of the students‘ interest in
the contents related to the SRT, which, as Ausubel5 points out, is one of the main conditions
for significant learning. Naturally, this is not enough for learning the SRT, but it is a key
previous factor.
In this chapter we introduce some elements in order to encourage teachers to deal with
the SRT in their classes. It consists of five sections of which the first two (items 2 and 3)
present a series of historical and epistemological reflections and some of the concepts we
consider appropriate for discussion. In the third section an example of a possible teaching
sequence is given, which has been put into practice at an Argentine secondary school in
Buenos Aires province (item 4). The results of the application of the teaching proposal are
discussed in the fourth section (item 5), and the last section presents possible approaches
(item 6).
3
Villani,1992.
4
Rodríguez y Pietrocola, 1999; Arriassecq y Greca, 2005ª, 2006.
5
Ausubel et al, 1991.
74 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
According to the advocates of this contextualized approach, the history of science should
be used in class with the aim of helping students appreciate, among other things7,
a) The difficulties and obstacles that had to be overcome and the cultural, philosophical,
technological, etc. contexts in which the scientific theories developed and which
were different from the present one.
b) The fact that science is a human activity done by people who make partial
contributions by answering questions formulated in a particular period in history.
Seldom is there only one ―discoverer‖.
c) The fact that, in each period of history, scientists were not thinking about our
―present terminology‖ since they used the logic, methodological, and epistemological
tools, as well as the traditions prevailing at the time.
To sum up, through the HPS students should realize that the present knowledge is the
result of a long-term process in which theory and practical experience interrelate with each
other, and which is strongly influenced by philosophical, cultural, social, and even aesthetic
factors. Nevertheless, it has to be made clear that when using the HPS as a teaching tool to
provide scaffolding for a complex topic, oversimplification should be avoided. Otherwise, the
sense of the history of science, science itself as well as its concepts in teaching could be
distorted.
Even though the whole Physics syllabus could be approached from this viewpoint, the
SRT is too complex a topic to be dealt with in this way. The influence of the SRT has
exceeded the scope of Physics, and the knowledge of this theory is necessary in order to
understand the cultural productions of the 20th century. As Holton points out (1992), certain
breakthroughs in the field of science have had important consequences in other fields to such
an extent as to bring about substantial changes in the culture of that particular period. In the
same way as Newtonian Mechanics and Optics have influenced artists, thinkers, philosophers,
and politicians, some of Einstein‘s scientific works have had a significant influence on other
fields like philosophy, visual arts, or literature. Taking this point of view into account, we are
going to suggest some principles we consider necessary for introducing the SRT in high
school. A more detailed discussion of this can be found in Arriassecq y Greca (2002).
6
Matthews, 1994.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 75
conceived the universe as corpuscles moving freely in a vast and uniform vacuum. It should
be noted that the other branch of Mechanics, the Cartesian one, did not support this
conception. They did not consider the universe a vacuum but a plenum. Since the birth of this
Newtonian idea of the universe, for almost two hundred years, its proponents had been
convinced they had the right theoretical framework within which they could explain ―every‖
phenomenon in nature, including the optical, caloric, magnetic, electrical, chemical, and even
biological ones. To put it briefly, ―At the same time, Mechanics gained precise meaning. It
became the theory that asserted that any observable phenomenon could be explained through
Newtonian Mechanics applied to the corpuscles that made up the universe and which are the
only real elements found in space and time‖8.
During the last third of the nineteenth century, the Newtonian program turned out to be
incompatible with the Physics of the time, especially with the Physics related to
electromagnetic phenomena. As a result, several scientists contributed to a new view of
nature. For example, whereas the Newtonian Mechanics program proposed the corpuscles as
the only real elements, Faraday suggested that the only real physical entity was force.
According to him, the world was a field of forces where some of them exerted pressure on the
contiguous ones, the particles being the points where potent forces converged. Since in the
explanation offered by Faraday there is no distance action, every change in one of the points
in space would bring about a change in the contiguous ones. As this process takes some time,
the disturbance would be propagated at finite speed. Through this theory that opposes
Newtonian Mechanics ideas, Faraday was able to explain some electrical phenomena.
Another important contribution was made by Maxwell, who developed a theory based on
the results of experiments on electrical and magnetic phenomena which had been carried out
during decades. This was an electrical and magnetic unified theory, condensed in four
equations that made it possible to demonstrate that the interaction between electrical and
magnetic phenomena can create permanent electrical and magnetic movement in the form of
waves that propagate at the speed of light. In accordance with the Mechanics principle that
established that waves required a material medium to be propagated, Maxwellian waves also
needed a corporeal medium so that their vibrations could support the electromagnetic forces
of the theory.
It is important to note that Maxwell had restated the notion of ether. Actually, as we have
already pointed out, the idea of the non-existence of a vacuum was still true for the Cartesian
branch of the Mechanics program. What Maxwell did was to introduce this idea in the
Newtonian branch, for the sake of the Mechanics in his theory. This hypothetical medium was
eventually called ―electromagnetic ether‖, and its search became a major topic of research.
There is another key characteristic of the electromagnetic theory proposed by Maxwell
that is related to what we mentioned above and contributed to the development of the SRT.
This is the fact that its equations enable us to deduce that, from the point of view of an
observer placed in that hypothetical ether, the waves are always propagated at the same speed,
irrespective of the speed of the source of those waves in relation to the observer. This concept
contradicts the Galilean transformations9 accepted by the Newtonian Mechanics Program.
7
Hodson, 1986; Kragh, 1989.
8
Boido, 1996, p. 357.
9
Let us remember Galilean transformations. For a reference frame S that is in rectilinear and uniform motion in the
direction of the axis x in relation to S‘, we obtain, x‘ = x + vt; y‘ = y; z‘ = z y t‘ = t. These transformations are
76 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
How did scientists react to this obstacle? One of the choices they had was to dismiss Maxwell
equations. However, such equations had already ―passed‖ all the experimental tests.
Another possibility was to consider Galilean transformations to be wrong. This was not
easy since it would have involved revising the whole Newtonian Mechanics because
Newtonian laws are invariant in relation to Galilean transformations10.
As a result, a middle ground was established. Only when applied to the ether at rest or all
the reference frames that were at rest with respect to the ether, were Maxwell equations to be
considered valid. In the case of reference frames that moved at constant speed in relation to
the ether, Maxwell equations had to be modified and the speed of light would be calculated
through the Galilean transformation. This experimentally confirmed that such ether could be
discovered.
This assumption was the starting point for the well-known experiments conducted by
Michelson and Morley.
There were many attempts at identifying the ether, that is to say, at detecting a change in
the speed of light within the reference frame of the earth which would move at a constant
speed in relation to the ether. After repeated failures, some studies were carried out in order to
modify the Mechanics program without altering the fundamentals of such. It was precisely
Lorentz who was in charge of these attempts to modify the theory by proposing new
equations about light transformation. These were not compatible with Galilean ones and
the usual ones within Classical Mechanics, which considers that time is always the same in all reference
frames regardless of the movement of such frames, and they remain valid for much lower speeds than that of
light.
10
The Galilean relativity principle stated that the laws of motion of bodies in a given space are the same in relation
to each other, provided they remain stationary or that they move in a rectilinear and uniform way - or, in other
words, that the laws that rule the phenomena preserve the same shape in both reference frames. This means
that all the phenomena studied, on a spaceship that is in motion for instance, should appear to be the same to
those on a stationary spaceship, with no way to determine, without external observation, whether the body is at
rest or in motion. As it is known, Newtonian laws are invariant with respect to Galilean transformations. The
problem arises because Maxwell‘s laws, which in principle originate in the same Newtonian theory, are not
invariant with respect to these transformations; Maxwell‘s equations do not preserve the same shape and, on a
spaceship in motion, the electrical and optical phenomena will be different from those of a stationary
spaceship, and so there would be a way of determining, without external observation, whether it is stationary
or in motion. For example, only from the perspective of an observer at rest, would the speed of light be the
same in every direction. In order to succeed in making electromagnetism equations invariant with respect to
Galilean transformations and, therefore, preserve Newtonian relativity, it was necessary to introduce new
terms into the equations that contained predictions of new electrical phenomena that did not exist. For this
reason, this attempt was abandoned.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 77
provided an explanation for the results of the experiment conducted by Michelson and
Morley. Such equations are called Lorentz‘s transformations11.
A more ―epistemological‖ influence can be detected in the origins of the SRT. This
influence was due to Mach‘s criticism of Newtonian Mechanics. As Holton (1982) points out,
this influence is demonstrated in two crucial points of an article by Einstein published in
1905. In the first place, Einstein insists, right from the beginning of his article, on the great
importance of a profound epistemological analysis in order to solve Physics problems,
especially those regarding the meaning of the notions of time and space. The second point
that denotes Mach‘s influence is the fact that Einstein identifies reality with what can be
perceived through the senses.
Such was the context in which the SRT was conceived. When the old view of the cosmos
was being questioned due to a number of empirical, theoretical and epistemological problems,
the SRT offered possible solutions to them although they would mean a change in the idea of
the universe.
Apart from this historical contextualization regarding scientific matters, it is important to
highlight the general historical context, which will provide for an understanding of the
influence the SRT exerted outside the field of science. The SRT as well as quantum
Mechanics, which is another great theory that caused a revolution in science, were conceived
at the beginning of the twentieth century. That was a period of big political, artistic and
scientific changes which had in common the aim of finding new ways to approach reality,
moving away from the old conceptions.
From an epistemological point of view, there are several aspects present in current
debates, which are worth discussing. Such aspects include the reflections on the origin of a
theory, its empirical contrasts, its applications, the role of the scientific community in the
development of a theory, as well as the influences of scientific production on society.
11
Lorentz‘s transformations, for a system that travels at a speed v in direction of x in relation to another frame, are,
x vt t vx c 2
x' t'
1 v 1 v
2 2
2
c2
y‘= y; z‘= z; c
, with c as the speed of light. Lorentz found that if his
transformations were applied, Maxwell‘s equations remained invariant.
12
Villani, 1981.
78 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
by even brilliant books such as Berkley‘s Physics and Feynman‘s Lectures on Physics, as
well as others of recent publication.
Holton13 suggests that several events helped to reinforce this view. These include:
The historical climate at that particular time, given the reluctance with which both
the results of Michelson‘s experiment as well as the SRT were accepted.
The views expressed by Einstein himself in his first educational publications.
The aim of textbooks, which is to convince students of the validity of a theory by
establishing connections between theory and experimentation.
The prevailing experimental rationality in the scientific community.
In spite of this, a detailed revision of the genesis of the SRT suggests that, even though
Michelson‘s experiment had not been carried out, this fact would possibly not have
influenced the development of the SRT. Even before 1905, through Lorentz‘ work, Einstein
had learned of several vain attempts at determining the movement of the earth in relation to
the ether. However, according to some authors, it was not a concern for contradictory
experimental results but an aesthetical dissatisfaction what led him to write his article. There
was a need to have a new viewpoint to solve the theoretical contradiction that arose when
Maxwellian electrodynamics gave an explanation for the effects of motion between a magnet
and a conductor, depending on which of the two is at rest or in motion14. That need was the
starting point for the development of the SRT. In fact, the first paragraph of Einstein‘s article
in 1905 is devoted to accounting for the asymmetry between observable phenomena (that
depend only on the relative motion of the magnet and the conductor) and the decided
difference generally appreciated in both cases (whether the magnet or the conductor is either
at rest or in motion).
Suggesting that Einstein used Michelson‘s experiments as a starting point for developing
the SRT, helps to create in students a distorted view of scientific activity, favouring in this
way a completely empirical conception of science. In view of all the evidence that disproves
this, from an epistemological point of view15, it would be unwise to support such idea.
13
Holton, 1973.
14
The explanation that emerges from Maxwell‘s equation is the following one; if the magnetic field is variable
(magnet in motion) there appears an electric field of certain energy that produces a current in the stationary
conductor. If the electric field is variable (conductor in motion) a field does not appear in the proximity of the
magnet at rest, but an electromotor force (e.m.f.) is established in the conductor. This force does not
correspond to any energy, but produces electric currents in the same direction and with the same intensity as
the previous electric forces – provided that the relative movement between the magnet and the conductor be
the same.
15
Matthews, 1994.
16
See, in this regard, a very interesting presentation in Villani‘s work (1981-1985), in which this author introduces
also the reason why Einstein‘s program succeeded in contrast to Lorentz‘, even though at the time there was
no empirical evidence for or against one of these theories.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 79
possibility to deal in class with epistemological aspects whose discussion can favourably
influence the idea of science that students form throughout the learning process.
The knowledge of theories that dealt with the same issues as the SRT can help to
understand the revolutionary aspect of this theory, no matter how much influenced by other
scientists Einstein was. Moreover, it should be noted that historians have not reached
complete agreement on which works were really known by Einstein before the publication of
his theory.
We can cite two different opinions on this subject. The first is the one given by Sir
Edmund Whittaker, who was a renowned theoretical physicist whose work was concerned
with the history of science and who witnessed the revolution caused by the SRT. The other is
the view expressed by the physicist and science historian Gerald Holton. The reason for
choosing these two perspectives is that, apart from having been provided by these two
prestigious physicists, they put forward opposing views that promote a critical analysis of the
origins of the SRT. According to Whittaker, in his article of 1905 Einstein ―…expounded on
Poincaré and Lorentz‘ relativity theory‖17. Briefly, in his article of 1904, Poincaré made an
analysis of the criticism of classical principles, including Galilean and Newtonian relativity
principle, from the point of view of the Physics of that time. Poincaré argued for the need of a
change in Physics. He said, ―… we should probably have to develop new Mechanics of which
so far we have only had a glimpse, and in which the speed of light will become absolute as a
result of an increase in the inertia with the speed‖. Nevertheless, he did not state the new
relativity principle nor expand on his suggestion. Whittaker claimed that Lorentz‘ article of
1904 was the major contribution to the SRT since he assumed Einstein had based his work on
Lorentz transformations. However, Holton18 asserted that Einstein and his scientific circle
repeatedly denied this. Even Einstein himself assured not having read such article.
The purpose of this example is to illustrate that, following a historical and
epistemological contextualized view of the teaching of science, it would not be right to
introduce the SRT as the invention of a scientific genius. On the contrary, we should point to
the existence of other scientists working at the same time on the same problems. In other
words, we should stress that no scientific theory is developed in a conceptual vacuum.
It also would be interesting to analyze together with the students the kind of explanation
the SRT provided the scientific circle with. Battimelli19 believes that the distinguishing
feature of Einstein‘s theory lies in the fact of offering a new strictly theoretical model based
on some fundamental principles in order to account for things that require a scientific
explanation. This paradigmatic explanation radically changes the usual way in which models
were established within the Mechanics program – by reconstructing the properties from the
interaction between the main components. Since the development of the SRT, a new
paradigm of explanation has been adopted in Physics. In this paradigm, the properties of
phenomena observed in an empirical way and considered to be universally valid principles,
are the starting point of the theory20.
17
Whittaker en Holton, 1995.
18
Holton, 1995.
19
Villani, 1981, p. 30.
20
Apart from these points, the ―ideas‖ the STR generated in the scientific community can be the starting point to
analyze the originality of the theory. From the perspective of a physicist such as Feynman (1963) the most
relevant ones would be the fact that ideas that have been supported for a long period of time and have been
clearly demonstrated could be wrong, the fact that ―strange‖ ideas, such as time dilation when one is in
80 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
motion, can not be abandoned a priori, whether one likes it or not, and the fact that it is necessary to notice the
symmetry in laws, which is the way in which laws can be transformed and remain the same. This last approach
has characterized the research into the physics of particles and fields.
21
The first evidence for time dilation was precisely obtained in 1938 by using radiant hydrogen atoms. The faster
the atoms moved, the slower their vibration was in accordance with the STR. In 1971 four cesium atomic
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 81
There are but a few theories that like the SRT have transcended the scientific field and
have had a sometimes strong influence on other areas, some of which students can hardly
imagine. These include philosophy, where it led to broad discussions, and the arts. As it was
previously pointed out, the fact of drawing attention to these aspects contextualizes scientific
knowledge and proves that this activity is not restricted to the scientific field but influences
other areas. However, students can realize this providing that the teacher encourages
discussion in this sense.
b)Impact On Philosophy
Many philosophical issues deal with concepts of central importance that have been
modified by the SRT. These concepts pose questions such as, what kind of object do we refer
to when we talk about space and time? , are they, or the combination of them, a substantial
unit?, or do they address groups of relationships among things different from themselves? Or,
as Kant believed, could they possibly be shapes the mind imposes on a non spatial and non
temporal world? Accordingly, it is interesting to consider the different possible answers
science can give to these philosophical questions and see how they changed as a result of the
development of the SRT.
Discussions of time and space, which will be examined later in this work, began twenty
five years ago. However, nowadays such discussions will inevitably take Einstein‘s work into
account22. For this reason, students should be shown this close relationship between
philosophy and science which dates from the very origins of science.
clocks were sent aboard planes that made commercial flights and it was found that when they were compared
to the clocks that remained on earth, the ones that ―moved‖ recorded an average of around 100 nanoseconds.
22
Boido et al., 1998.
82 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
23
In ―Feynamn‘s Lectures on Physics‖, Feynamn deals with this matter in a successful and humorous way.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 83
24
Bachelard, 1938, (ed.) (1997).
84 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
The following table presents a summary of the results of a survey25 which clearly shows
students‘ conceptions of the terms mentioned above. In the last row we indicate which
obstacles should be overcome by students in order to acquire new knowledge within the
framework of the SRT.
25
Arriassecq and Greca, 2005.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 85
3.1. Insight into the Notions of Reference Frame and Relative Motion
within the Framework of Classical Mechanics
First of all, and before introducing the SRT, a serious discussion about the notions of
―reference frame‖ and ―relative motion‖ in the Galilean sense should be conducted, even
though these topics have already been developed before the relativist approach. Different
studies suggest that students have alternative notions in this respect. They regard the reference
frames as a piece of space connected with a concrete object, whose size and shape delimits
that space. Also, according to them, this reference frame, where an observer watches the
phenomenon, can include other smaller bodies. Despite the fact that the Galilean relativity
principle laws would quite simplify the matter, they are rarely employed by students. Thus,
they mistake the invariability of the laws between reference frames in relative motion for the
invariability of time within a given frame. In addition to this, research has shown that students
of Physics, and even graduates, tend to regard the ―reference frame‖ as a ―decorative touch‖
with no explanatory purpose. This demonstrates they lack a metaconceptual understanding of
the concept to be used as a tool that allows an appropriate formulation of the physical
relativity principle. These concepts of ―reference frame‖ and ―relative movement‖ are found
in the fundamental postulates of the SRT. Such are the invariability of all physical laws and
the invariability of the speed of light in a vacuum within inertial reference frames. For this
reason, and given the difficulties they present, such concepts should be thoroughly discussed
before approaching this theory.
Figure 1.
26
Loma, 1999.
27
Mach, 1949.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 87
If the observer O (Figure 1), with his watch, is situated in the same position as the torch
and just in front of it, when the torch emits the beam of light the observer looks at his watch
and there will be no ambiguity as regards the time in which the event takes place (event and
instrument in the same place). However, if another observer is positioned, with his watch, at
A at a certain distance as the diagram shows, it will take some time for the same beam of light
to reach A. Therefore, when the observer positioned at A looks at his watch, the result will
not be the same as the one measured by O. If a third observer stands at B, he will record, for
the same event, a different time from the one recorded by O and A.
his proves how untenable are the Newtonian notion of time as absolute and universal,
and, as a result, the idea that simultaneity is a fact in any system. According to the SRT, two
events that appear to be simultaneous in a given reference frame, will not, in general, be
simultaneous in a second framework that is in motion with respect to the first one. Therefore,
simultaneity is not an absolute concept but depends on the state of motion of the observer.
This is the relativity of simultaneity.
This question is clearly illustrated in another thought experiment provided by Einstein.
Let us suppose a train carriage travels at constant speed and two bolts of lightning strike each
end of the carriage as shown in the picture. The bolts of lightning leave marks on the carriage
and on the ground, A’, B’, and A, B respectively. An observer O’, who is moving along with
the carriage and who is at rest with respect to it, is halfway between A’ and B’. Another
observer O, who is at rest on the platform, is equidistant from A and B (Figure 2). The events
observed by both O and O’ will be the flashes of light (Figure 3). Both flashes reach O at the
same time. As a result he will conclude that the events at A and B took place simultaneously
since they traveled the same distance at the same speed.
Figure 2
88 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Figure 3.
If both events are this time considered from the point of view of observer O’, when the
light reaches observer O, observer O’ has moved as it is shown in the picture on the right.
Thus, the flash of light has already reached O’ whereas the light from A’ has not.
Einstein‘s interpretation of this result is based on the assumption that the speed of light
has the same value regardless of the reference frame in which it is measured. If this is so, the
reason for the fact that one of the flashes of light from the bolts of lightning A’ and B’ arrives
before the other one, is that the bolt of lightning that strikes the front of the carriage does it
before the other lightning strikes the rear of such carriage. Therefore, two events which prove
simultaneous for O are not simultaneous for O’.
The concept of simultaneity may prove to be confusing for those students who do not
accept the relativity principle that demands the equivalence between the physical laws within
the inertial reference frames. Thus, students tend to regard the observation recorded by an
observer positioned on the ground as more real than the one by an observer who is in
motion28.
We have made a detailed analysis of this matter in order to show the need to discuss in
class the concepts of space and time as well as the role of these concepts in classical
Mechanics to be later ―reinterpreted‖ within the framework of the SRT. Even though we are
not going to enlarge on this, it can be appreciated that the observer plays a key role within the
SRT. In the field of classical Physics, in which the speed of light has an unlimited value, it is
enough to regard an observer as a person positioned within a reference frame, with a
particular instrument, such as a chronometer to record time. Ricci29 believes that students
would associate the idea of observer with that of a person who ―observes‖ in the sense of
―watching‖ or ―looking at‖. Therefore, it is essential that the student should realize, from a
Physics point of view, that ―observing‖ is not the same as ―measuring‖ within the context of
the SRT. At the same time, ―measuring‖ requires that the notion of observer should be
redefined. For this reason, the notion of ―observer‖ within the SRT should include the idea of
recording events that have occurred in distant places.
The deductions about the quantitative relations for time dilation and length contraction
could be simply made through any of Einstein‘s imaginary experiments which are found in
most textbooks. The reference frames, the measuring instruments as well as the operational
way of measuring, what has been observed by each observer in his reference frame, and also
the fundamental postulates of the SRT should be highlighted.
After the article on the SRT had been published in 1905, a new mathematical
interpretation was given to this theory when the mathematician Hermann Minkowski
developed the new concept of space-time. Einstein himself used the mathematical concept
developed by Minskowski, who was his professor at university, when he later constructed the
general theory of relativity. Even though the algebra involved in Minkowski‘s mathematical
28
Aleman Berenguer and Pérez Celles, 2000.
29
Ricci, 2000.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 89
formulation of the SRT could not be grasped by secondary students, the graphic solution30
offered by such approach could be dealt with. This, in addition, allows reintroducing the
discussion of the concepts of reference frame and simultaneity.
Based on the idea that not only the positions of the objects but especially the events that
take place at a certain time are important within the SRT, Mikowski proposed an
interpretation of the universe as a four-dimensional space-time web. This replaced the idea of
assigning one temporal and three spatial dimensions to each event.
Let us suppose that a person moves with URM along a railway track. An observer O who
is at rest with respect to the railway notices that the rear of the train enters the tunnel at the
same time as the front comes out of it. The following diagram (Figure 4) shows what that
observer O notices about the movement and position of the carriage at every instant. The line
of the diagram that indicates how an object progresses through space and time, in this case
how it moves, is called world line. The positions of the front of the carriage in space and time
recorded by O are represented by each point on that line.
Figure 4.
30
See, for instance, the supplementary topics A and B by Resnick, 1977, and the appendices A and B by Mook and
Varguish, 1998.
90 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
- The vertical lines represent the entry into the tunnel and the exit from it. Given the
fact that the tunnel is at rest with respect to the observer, the position is the same as
time progresses.
- There is a point where the world line of the front of the carriage is intersected by the
line representing the position of entry into the tunnel. That point at which the two
lines intersect represents an event, which is the entry of the train into the tunnel. The
other events are the exit of the front of the train (intersection of the world line of the
front of the train and the exit from the tunnel) and the entry of the back of the train
into the tunnel (intersection of the world line of the back of the train and the entry
into the tunnel). It can be appreciated that the rear of the train enters the tunnel at the
same time as the front emerges from it. This takes place at 8 units of time.
Let us analyze now a space time diagram for events that take place in two spatial
dimensions as well as a temporal one. Let us suppose a beam of light is emitted at a particular
instant of time, at a certain point in space. As time progresses, the beam will evolve into a
sphere of light. After a millionth of a second (1.10-6s) the sphere of light will have a 300m
radius; after two millionths of a second, a 600m radius, etc. The evolution of the flash of light
from the initial event can be graphically represented as a three-dimensional cone in four-
dimensional space-time which is called future light cone of the event (Figure 5). Another cone
can be symmetrically drawn. This cone is known as past light cone of the event, and it is
made up of the number of ―possible‖ events from which, provided they were beams of light,
the original event could be reached. These past and future light cones of a certain P event
(present event) divide space-time in three regions as can be seen in the picture.
Figure 5.
The absolute future of P event is the internal area of the future light cone of such event,
and it is made up of all the possible events which could be affected by what is happening to P
at that precise moment. The absolute past of P event is the internal area of the past light cone
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 91
of such event, and it is made up of the number of possible events that would be able to affect
P if they moved at the same or less speed than the speed of light. The elsewhere is the area of
space-time outside the past and future cones of light of event P and it comprises the events
that can not affect or be affected by P at that particular moment. Yet, an event B can
eventually affect us; if the sun exploded at that exact moment, we would only learn about
such event 8 minutes later and it could not affect us before that time.
This kind of diagram can help to imagine and represent some key concepts of the SRT. In
principle, a light cone can be built for each event that develops in space-time, which
comprises all the possible luminous ways such event may follow. The postulate that
establishes that nothing can travel faster than the speed of light is also represented. This
means the path any object takes through space-time is represented by a line that necessarily
falls within the light cone of an event that includes it (Figure 6).
Figure 6.
Now we are going to discuss how this kind of diagram allows deciding for or against the
simultaneity of events that occur in different reference frames. For this purpose, we will again
consider a thought experiment proposed also by Einstein. A passenger O‘ is aboard a train
carriage that travels at constant speed v with respect to another observer O who is positioned
within an inertial reference frame, for example at rest in relation to the platform. O‘ is
positioned at exactly the middle of the carriage and at a certain moment he turns on a torch.
Let us suppose the carriage is equipped with a sophisticated system that allows the two doors
P1 and P2, which are situated at the front and at the rear of the carriage, to open when the
light from the torch reaches them. Instead of using Lorentz‘ transformations to decide upon
the simultaneity of the events for O and O‘, Minskowski‘s diagrams can be used. This can be
done in a qualitative way, which means not assigning any speed to the train. It is only
assumed that it travels at a speed close to that of light. At the same time, and in order to
simplify the diagram, we will give the speed of light a value ―1‖ (in this way the straight line
that represents it will form a 45 degrees angle with both axes) and therefore, (if the speed is
calculated applying the condition that c = 1) instead of representing time on one of the axes (it
is a rule within the SRT to represent time on the vertical axis) we will be representing ct. As a
result, according to observer O, the space-time diagram for the events of the opening of the
door at the front (E1) and the opening of the door at the rear (E2) is the following (Figure 7).
92 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Figure 7.
- P1 and P2 are the world lines for the doors at the front and at the rear, which
according to O’, remain in the same place and are therefore represented by parallel
lines running perpendicular to the x-axis.
- H1 and H2 are the world lines for the beams of light emitted by the torch. As a value
of ―1‖ has been given to c, these beams are represented by two straight lines forming
a 45 degrees angle with respect to the axes.
- The intersections of the world lines of the beams of light and the carriage doors
represent the events of the opening of the doors E1 and E2. It can be appreciated in
the diagram that these events are simultaneous for O’.
Figure 8.
The behaviour of the beams of light is not modified, which agrees with the second
postulate of the SRT, even though the train aboard which the torch is would be in motion in
relation to O. For this reason, the world lines are the same as the ones in the previous
diagram. However, the representation of the world lines for the doors, which according to O
are moving at the speed of the train, does change. Accordingly, the lines are no longer
perpendicular to the axis x but form an angle that has the same value as the tangential arc of
the speed of the train measured by O. From O‘s point of view, event E1 precedes E2.
Based on the previous conclusion, we can discuss whether it is possible to alter the order
in which two or more events take place by only changing the reference frame. Or, in other
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 93
words, if it is possible to contradict the causality principle (every cause precedes the effect).
Minkowski‘s diagrams can be used in order to answer these questions.
Consider now the following thought experiment. A beam of light is emitted by a torch A
at the same instant of time as such torch is turned on (event 1), and another torch E is turned
on when the light from A has not reached it yet (event 2). The places where the events 1 and 2
occur are far enough as to allow that observers positioned in different inertial reference
frames could reach different conclusions as regards the temporal order in which the events
happen; event 1can precede, take place after, or at the same time as event 2, depending of
where the observer is positioned.
Let us see what happens in the following picture (Figure 9):
Figure 9.
In the case that has been represented, the places where the above-mentioned events occur
are close enough for the beam of light from torch A to reach torch E before it is turned on. In
this situation all the observers, regardless of the state of motion, will agree on the temporal
order in which the events take place; event 1 precedes event 2. This is the case when two
events are linked by causality. Let us suppose that event 1 is the cause of event 2 (the light
from torch A turns on torch E). In order for this to happen, a signal should travel from the
place where A is to the one where E is and, as we have already discussed it, the SRT does not
allow the speed of propagation of data to be higher than that of light. Therefore, both torches
must be close enough so that the light from A could reach E before it is turned on. The fact
that any observer notices the same temporal order for the events can be interpreted by
assuming that it is not possible for an observer to move in such a way as to record the effect
before its cause.
As the SRT started to be discussed within the scientific sphere, spirited debates were held
which were usually regarded as paradoxes. It would be interesting to encourage in class the
discussion of any of them and explain the reason why in fact they are not such. ―The twins‘
paradox‖ and the ―length contraction paradox‖ are among the best-known ones. In the twins
paradox two astronauts have the peculiarity of being identical twins in order to allow the
comparison of the biological effects of time dilation on each of them. Through an experiment,
the astronauts move away from each other at a speed comparable to that of light. It can be
supposed that both of them travel away from each other aboard spaceships with a rectilinear
and uniform motion. In this way, each of them makes calculations (assuming they know the
94 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
SRT) and arrives at the conclusion that it is the other the one who remains younger, given the
fact that the laws are invariant in both frames. However, they can not ―empirically
corroborate‖ this until they see each other face to face. In order to do this, one of them should
turn around and catch up with the other leading to the fact that the twin who comes back will
be younger than the other. How can this be possible if the situation is, in principle,
symmetrical? The return of one of the twins means regulating speed, and, even when it is
possible to disregard acceleration (making the trip last as much time as possible so that the
time that acceleration takes could be disregarded) and to assume that the return trip is made in
a rectilinear and uniform way, the twins will be in two different inertial frames. This fact
makes this situation asymmetrical for them and proves the paradox invalid. This is a
qualitative way of solving the paradox within the context of the SRT31.
Another interesting paradox, which is related to subatomic Physics, is the muons
paradox. The muons are basic and unstable particles with the same electric charge as the
electron and 207 times its matter; they can be produced when cosmic radiation strikes atoms
at a great height in the atmosphere. The average life of the muons is 2,2 s when it is
calculated within a reference frame at rest in relation to them. If, added to this, we take into
account that their speed is approximately the same as that of light it can be assumed that these
particles travel 600 meters before disintegrating. However, the distance between the higher
layers of the atmosphere surrounding the earth‘s surface (within the earth‘s reference frame)
is of much more than 600m and, in spite of that, the particles are experimentally detected on
the earth‘s surface. This apparent paradox is solved through time dilation. Let us consider
that, within the earth‘s reference frame, the muons travel in relation to it at a speed close to
that of light; therefore, the time measured within the earth‘s reference frame is t t . If
we assume that the speed of the muons is approximately of 0,99c, it follows that t 16s .
As a result, the distance covered by the muons as measured from the Earth is vt 4800m
(Figure 10).
Figure 10.
31
Another option is to adopt a graphic solution by using Minkowski‘s space-time diagrams.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 95
32
Martinand, 1986.
96 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
similar way to Bachelard‘s, Martinand stresses the need for not regarding the obstacles as a
negative aspect of learning, which could lead to a block preventing students from achieving
the desired objective, but as a dynamic and motivating aspect. It is necessary to evaluate from
this perspective all the existent and possible obstacles, and decide upon the ones that seem
could be overcome at the given level, within the given content and context in which they are
expected to be overcome. In this connection, Astolfi33 argues that the main problem in
promoting learning involves selecting, from among the range of possible objectives, the ones
which seem appropriate for a sequence, and which, in terms of obstacles that can be
overcome, are demanding enough for students to feel motivated but which, at the same time,
are carefully set so that, within the class context in interaction with other students, the teacher
and the materials at hand, the students are able to arrive at the desired solution.
In this way, based on the difficulties present in the conceptualization of time, space,
observer, simultaneity, measurement, and reference frame, we made a list of objectives-
obstacles which should be overcome by the student, within the classroom context and
mediated by the teacher, in order to meaningfully learn the SRT. These objectives-obstacles
appear in the table below.
CONCEPTS OBJECTIVES
To analyze the concept of time from different
perspectives: from a philosophical, a scientific, and a
psychological point of view.
To appreciate the range of possibilities of graphically
Time representing time magnitude.
To identify the concepts involved in the process of
measuring time.
To interpret the concept of time within the paradigm of
the SRT recognizing the differences with classical
Mechanics.
To redesign the space model built, adapting it to meet
the requirements of classical Mechanics.
Space To interpret the concept of space within the paradigm
of the SRT recognizing the differences with classical
Mechanics.
Observer To redefine the notion of observer adapting it to the
SRT.
To analyze the different possibilities of simultaneous
events within classical Mechanics.
Simultaneity To analyze the different possibilities of simultaneous
events within the SRT.
To appreciate from a Physics point of view that within
the context of the SRT ―to see‖ is not the same as ―to
Measurement measure‖.
To study the relation among the measurement process,
33
Astolfi, 1999.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 97
Taking into account the results obtained in various research works (Arriassecq y Greca,
2002, 2003, 2005 a, b, y c, y 2007) and the theoretical framework adopted, a teaching
proposal has been developed in order to deal with the SRT. Such proposal comprises five
stages.
In the first stage, questions of a historical and epistemological nature are discussed. These
refer to the notion of science, the characteristics of scientific work, the evolution of ideas in
science, and the influences of the social, historical, and cultural context on the birth and
validation of scientific theories.
In the second stage, the concepts of classical Mechanics which are necessary in order to
interpret the SRT or which are even substantially modified by the theory are thoroughly
revised.
Then, we deal with the concepts of electromagnetism that come into conflict with
Classical Mechanics and which are later reintroduced by Einstein in the SRT.
Afterwards, the fundamental aspects of the SRT are discussed, based on the original
article of 1905 and by analyzing it from different perspectives so that the student is able to
build new schemes that will enable him to deal with situations that demand the reformulation
of classical concepts.
Finally, in the fifth stage of the proposal, it is intended that students get acquainted with
some aspects of Alfred Einstein‘s life as an ordinary man, which go beyond the ―myth‖.
The key concepts of the SRT are reintroduced at different stages when the proposal is put
into practice, using a range of representations of them such as the algebra and graphics in
Minkowski‘s diagrams. This kind of representation allows qualitative estimates that include
the possibility of comparing the measurements made by different observers, deciding upon
the simultaneity of events, etc. An important part of the discussion takes as a starting point the
situations-problem in which the obstacles are present. The different stages of this proposal
and the estimated time each will take are detailed in the appendix.
In this section we describe the results of the implementation of the teaching proposal that
has been designed to teach the SRT and has been carried out at polimodal level at an
Argentine school.
A qualitative methodological approach has been adopted for this analysis. Such analysis
demands a detailed description of the contextual aspects involved. In this particular case,
these aspects, which are briefly described below, relate to the institution where the
investigation was conducted, the authorities, the teacher, the group of students, and the kinds
98 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
of bonds that existed among them, as well as the kind of work to which both teacher and
students were used to, the work strategies they applied, the estimated time the proposal would
take, and the adjustments that it was necessary to make considering the particular
circumstances that could not be anticipated.
The group of students that took part in the investigation was the third year class of High
Scool level with a scientific orientation at a traditional school of the town of Tandil in Buenos
Aires province. The group was composed of twenty seven girl students who had two weekly
hours of Physics. The topics of Physics they had dealt with in the two previous years involved
Newtonian dynamics and kinematics; in third year, they had dealt with topics concerning
optics (especially geometry) before approaching the SRT.
The school authorities gave us enthusiastic support over the course of the investigation.
We consider it important to highlight that in order to conduct the investigation within the
selected class, an agreement was made both with the teacher and with the students in which
the role of the researcher in the classroom was established; she would be an observer and not
a participant. The kind of relationship she intended to establish with the students and the
teacher who took part in the investigation was also made clear so that they could work
comfortably, in a relaxed way as they were used to.
The intended work atmosphere was generated and the students were inhibited when
working, even when the researcher approached them with the tape recorder in order to record
some discussion.
As regards the qualifications of the teacher in charge of the subject, she holds a
bachelor‘s degree in Physics and Mathematics and has ten year‘s teaching experience. When
this proposal was implemented, she had not done any postgraduate courses, neither on this
specific content nor on pedagogy; the courses she had attended were the training courses
offered by the Ministry of Education or the local university.
With regard to her knowledge of the SRT, this teacher, just like other teachers in this
town, had only been formally instructed in this topic when doing Modern Physics, which is
only one subject of the course of studies at the teaching training college.
On several occasions, the teacher said that due to her lack knowledge of the topic she felt
insecure, even though she was ready to face the challenge of approaching it. Every time she
solicited for support, this was given to her through meetings in order to discuss particular
aspects of the written material or the proposed activities.
favourable ones34, many of the proposed objectives have been achieved as it can be
appreciated in the fact that students overcame some of the epistemological obstacles detected
in the description of the initial profile of the group that took part in the investigation. The
results we have obtained show that the students were able to modify some concepts they used
when dealing with Classical Mechanics matters which were not correct from a scientific point
of view. Had these modifications not been made, the topics of the SRT that are scientifically
agreed upon would not have been incorporated in a meaningful way.
The analysis of activities related to Classical Mechanics concepts (initial test, problem
solving, making mind maps, devising stories, cartoons, and the final test) would suggest that
the obstacle of conceiving the concept of time in Physics as we do in daily life, or reducing
this concept to only its units of measure, has been overcome. The modification of this
conception would be due to the objective of identifying the concepts involved in the process
of measuring time.
At the same time, the debate on the history of the concept of time led the students to add
complexity to the concept, becoming aware of the difficulties that a pragmatic view of such
would entail. Therefore, the students‘ response to different activities and their comments in
class seem to indicate that the objective of analyzing time from different perspectives
(philosophical, scientific, etc.) has been achieved – in accordance with the first mentioned
objective.
The concept of simultaneity was approached in class through some activities. Despite the
fact that, as regards Classical Mechanics, the mind maps made by the students did not select
this concept, there seems to be a favourable development of other concepts connected with
this one, such as reference frame, observer and measurement, which are fundamental to the
SRT. In the answers to the initial questionnaire, which coincided with the results obtained
with other groups (Arriassecq and Greca, 2005 b and c), the first concept was not taken into
account in order to approach different proposed situations and the second one was reduced to
the idea of a person who ―watches‖ or, to put it redundantly, ―observes‖.
After working in class and doing different problem solving activities throughout the
implementation of the proposal, these conceptions were modified coming closer to a
scientifically accepted one. For the purpose of illustrating this point new ways of
conceptualizing have been transcribed; ―the movement of an object can only be described by
adopting a reference frame‖ (A1); ―in order to determine the speed of an object both a
reference frame and an observer are required‖ (A2); ―the observer who intends to describe
some physical phenomenon should adopt a reference frame and use the appropriate
instruments for each kind of measurement‖ (A3); ―if an observer has to record times, he needs
instruments that are synchronized with those of another observer in order to compare results‖
(A4).
According to the results, it can be assumed that the following objectives have been
achieved:
34
The two periods of Physics were divided in two different days. One of those days was Monday to which all the
national holidays were moved. Therefore, many classes were missed. On the other hand, several activities
typically done by students in their senior year (trips, special days) and other institutional ones ( teacher
training courses) took place on the days assigned to Physics classes.
100 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
The rest of the concepts that have been selected following an epistemological analysis,
are within the conceptual field of the SRT, and have been evaluated according to the results
obtained from the students‘ mind maps of the SRT, cartoons or stories, and a final test.
As regards the concept of space, it has been initially detected that the students‘
representations agreed with the Platonic model that states that ―space is the place that bodies
occupy‖. A radical modification of such interpretation of the concept of space was made, as
we can appreciate by comparing the initial test with the final one in which twenty six out of
twenty seven students disagree with such statement. In connection with this, the following
objectives seem to have been achieved:
To interpret the concept of space within the conceptual field of the SRT establishing
the differences with Classical Mechanics
To interpret the concept of time within the conceptual field of the SRT establishing
the differences with Classical Mechanics
All the four groups that made mind maps of the SRT were able to identify relativity both
in space as in time in relation to the adopted reference frame. As regards the cartoons, five out
of the six groups stated the same as it was expressed in the mind maps. With regard to the
final test, all of the students answered that the measurements of certain length depend on the
reference frame adopted by the observers, even though half of the answers that offered an
argument for which measurement would be smaller were incorrect. This shows that, even
though there is evidence that the students are able to notice one of the consequences of the
SRT, which is length contraction, they are not capable yet of solving specific problems that
involve this concept.
As regards the concept of simultaneity, out of the four mind maps made by the groups of
students, two considered this concept as relevant, and established appropriate connections
with other concepts, showing an understanding of the relativity of simultaneity as a
consequence of the SRT. One of the other two groups did not even mention this, and the other
one placed it on the mind map but did not establish important connections with other
concepts. On the other hand, only one group mentioned and made correct use of this concept
in the cartoons. In the final test, twenty four students selected the correct option that stated
that the simultaneity of two events can be established even when such events do not occur in
the same place. In spite of the fact that this point could suggest that the obstacle noticed in the
initial test would have been overcome –the students used to think that events could be
simultaneous provided they occurred in the same place – we are aware of the fact that there
was not enough time to deal with different cases of simultaneous events both within the field
of Classical Mechanics as within the field of the SRT as it was stated in one of the objectives.
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 101
We believe that this is the reason for the different answers in the final test and for the fact that
this concept did not appear as relevant in the maps and the stories and/or cartoons.
With regard to the concept of postulates, these were identified and appropriately
connected with other concepts of the SRT. In the stories and cartoons, two, out of the six
groups, included them in their productions, two other groups did not take them into account,
and the other two mentioned only one of the postulates. In the final test, almost all the
students (twenty three and twenty seven students respectively) gave a correct answer to the
items that involved the concept of postulate, which seem to indicate that they have been able
to incorporate the meaning given to this concept within the context of the SRT. This means
that the aim of analyzing the role of the postulates within the SRT has been accomplished.
However, there was no opportunity to see if from an epistemological perspective more
appropriate meaning was given to this concept than the one expressed in the initial test.
A similar case to that of the postulates applies to the concept of scientific theory. Even
though there are no elements to assert that they have modified the concepts from an
epistemological point of view, there seems to be evidence that the obstacle initially detected –
a narrow view of scientific theories – has been overcome, at least in relation to the example
they dealt with, the SRT. Therefore, the objective of discussing the present status of the SRT
has been achieved.
All of the students answered – in the final test – that the SRT is not a mere theoretical
speculation, but an experimentally verified theory accepted by the scientific community. Also
all of the students asserted that the SRT has technological applications and have knowledge of
one of such with more implications for daily life such as the GPS. Apart from that, almost in
all the answers (twenty six) it was asserted that SRT did not emerge in answer to Michelson
and Morley‘s failed experiments. As regards the ―creation‖ of the SRT, a bit more than half of
the answers stated that the theory had not been formulated exclusively by Einstein although,
as we have already suggested, it is possible that there has been a problem with the
interpretation of the instructions or with the formulation of such, since the ones who argued
for Einstein‘s exclusive authorship also added that he did not construct it in a conceptual
vacuum and that there were other scientists who made contributions to the SRT.
With regard to the personal aspects of Einstein‘s life – a stage of the proposal which
could not be dealt with at length for reasons of time – although that was not what the material
was intended for, the students highlighted the most ―scandalous‖ aspects of his private life
and they seemed to have intended to ―compare‖ his professional performance with his
personal attitudes. It seems as if they were unable to realize that these are two different
spheres, and that was precisely the idea that was intended to be conveyed through the
discussion but there was no opportunity to facilitate this in an appropriate way. The students
assumed that being bright, dedicated, creative, successful, renowned, etc. ―guaranteed‖ a
successful private life, too. Another aspect that was contrary to the nature of the proposal and
appeared in connection with this point was the fact that two groups considered two
characteristics of Einstein´s work – his ―solitary‖ work ―isolated from the problems of
society‖ – as desirable for scientists in the present.
To sum up, we consider that, given the circumstances in which this proposal was applied,
the results obtained, at least as regards the conceptual aspect, have been quite satisfactory,
taking into account that there are signs that indicate that most of the proposed objectives have
been achieved.
102 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Despite the difficulties with the application, the results achieved by this group of students
on this occasion in terms of the learning of concepts that are central to the SRT, seem much
better than the ones obtained when approaching the SRT in a way we could call traditional,
by having a textbook as only teaching resource.
Therefore, we consider it really is possible to introduce elements of the SRT in secondary
school by using the material we have designed, in spite of the teachers‘ lack of knowledge of
the topic and the little time available. Apparently, this introduction to the theory proves
profitable for students not only in the sense that they are introduced to more ―modern‖
Physics – even though this is one century old! – but also because it seems to allow them to
revise and gain a deeper understanding of classical concepts such as the concepts of time,
space, reference frame, simultaneity, observer and measurement, which go unnoticed in
traditional teaching practices.
On the other hand, the question that necessarily arises is whether this proposal proves too
lengthy (and ambitious) to be carried out at secondary level in the present circumstances. In
other words, is it possible for other teachers to apply this proposal? If the contents of the SRT
are dealt with during the last term of the last school year, as it was the case this time and
seems to be standard practice, the answer will be negative. The proposal should then be
abridged at the risk, of course, of omitting some fundamental elements of its epistemological,
educational, and psychological conception.
However, considering how relevant it proves to students to develop a better
understanding of classical concepts that emerges precisely from contrasting them with the
relativist ones, it would be desirable that teachers become convinced of the importance of
dealing in detail with the SRT and not leaving this topic for the end of the last year of
secondary school. From this perspective, the whole proposal could be perfectly put into
practice since, according to the results outlined in this chapter it does not present students
with any ―insurmountable‖ difficulties in understanding such topic, except for the problems
any new topic may pose.
In spite of these problems, the experience we have gained through sharing this material
with teachers working at secondary level – one of whom was the teacher in charge of the
class in which the proposal was put into practice – indicates that the material is relevant to
them, gives them a more global view of the SRT, of its central concepts and their relationship
with classical ones, and of the historical and epistemological aspects involved in its creation.
6. FINAL COMMENTS
In this chapter we aimed to contribute to the incorporation of topics that are more
relevant to the present into the science syllabus at secondary school, within a contextualized
framework for science education.
We believe that studying historical cases such as the SRT, and placing them in the time
such theories were formulated, would prevent students from adopting a distorted view of
scientific methodology and from considering scientists as ―super geniuses‖. Therefore,
students would be allowed to see how different ideas become more powerful concepts that
together build conceptual frameworks like scientific theories. Apart from that, this approach
makes it possible to appreciate the contributions made by several historical characters towards
Teaching and Learning the Special Relativity Theory at High School Level 103
the creation of a new scientific conception, even though later they were not successful, or did
not receive the recognition of their peers at the time they developed their ideas. We consider
it important to stress once again that the introduction of the SRT can not be restricted to a
historical and epistemological approach that does not include a conceptual discussion
especially of those concepts regarded as relevant and which precisely give sense to such
approach.
The proposed teaching sequence has been put into practice with students taking their last
year at secondary school, obtaining the positive results we have outlined, especially as
regards the significant learning of concepts that play a key role in the SRT, the understanding
of several questions concerning the construction and acceptance of a scientific theory, as well
as a more profound and interesting revision of many concepts which are central to Physics.
We are now working on the construction of applets through Minkowski‘s diagrams, so that
students could make use of them when this proposal is applied in the future. In this way
students would be able to work with these kinds of diagrams which would otherwise not be
possible since they are difficult to make without the aid of computers, and demand too much
time on students. We hope that the use of these applets will help students grasp concepts such
as reference frame, simultaneity, proper and improper time, and length, and will give them the
possibility of making qualitative and quantitative estimates within the context of the SRT.
Finally, we would like to point out that the teaching approach we have adopted – a
historically and epistemologically contextualized approach which was conceptually,
psychologically, and pedagogically supported – can be used to introduce at secondary school
other topics concerning modern and contemporary Physics, most of which pose problems
from a teaching perspective similar to those with the SRT. These include lack of conceptual
knowledge on the part of teachers, superficial or unattractive approaches in textbooks, and
sometimes the absence of any.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The first authoress of this work has received financing by the National Agency of
Scientific and Technological Promotion, FONCyT (BID 1728/OC-AR) - PICT - 05 Nº:
34479-234.
APPENDIX
FIRST STAGE (Estimated time: two-hour class plus the amount of time the student
devotes to reading and doing the activities outside the classroom). It comprises one activity
and one text discussion.
1) Introduction
2) Scientific knowledge: its origin and some distinctive features.
2.1. Aristotelian cosmology.
2.2. The main characteristics that differentiate scientific knowledge from other kinds of
knowledge.
104 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Second Stage (Estimated time: two classes of two hours each plus the amount of time the
student devotes to reading and doing the activities outside the classroom). It consists of ten
activities and text discussions.
1) Revision of the main concepts of Newtonian Mechanics which are necessary in order
to interpret the SRT.
Third Stage (Estimated time: a two-hour class plus the amount of time the student
devotes to reading and doing the activities outside the class). It consists of one activity
Fourth Stage This stage comprises a total of six activities and text discussions.
2) Determining the length of an object from the point of view of an observer who is in
motion in relation to it
3) Lorentz‘ equations of transformation
4) Transformation of velocities within the SRT
(Estimated time: a two-hour class plus the amount of time the student devotes to reading
and doing the activities outside the classroom)
5) New relation between the concepts of space and time within the SRT: the space-time
5.6.1 Minkowski‘s diagrams
5.6.2 Applications of Minkowski‘s diagrams
(Estimated time: a two-hour class plus the amount of time the student devotes to reading
and doing the activities outside the classroom)
REFERENCES
Aleman Berenger, R. y Pérez Selles, J. (2000) Enseñanza por cambio conceptual: de la Física
Clásica a la Relatividad, Enseñanza de las Ciencias, v. 18,nº 3, p. 463-471.
Arriassecq, I. y Greca, I. (2002). Algunas consideraciones históricas, epistemológicas y
didácticas para el abordaje de la teoría de la relatividad especial en el nivel medio y
polimodal, Ciência and Educação, vol. 8, Nro. 1, pp. 55 – 69.
Arriassecq, I. y Greca, I. (2003). Enseñanza de la Teoría de la Relatividad Especial en el ciclo
polimodal: dificultades manifestadas por los docentes y textos de uso habitual. Revista
Electrónica de Enseñanza de las Ciencias, 3, 2, Artículo 7,en http://www.saum.
uvigo.es/reec.
Arriassecq, I. y Greca, I. (2005a). Análisis de algunos aspectos de la enseñanza de la Teoría
de la Relatividad Especial en el nivel polimodal argentino a partir de un estudio de caso.
En Moreira, M. A., Caballero, C. y Meneses, J. (Eds.) Actas del II Encuentro
Iberoamericano Sobre Investigación Básica En Educación En Ciencias, Cd-Rom.
Burgos: Servicio de publicaciones de la UBU.
Arriassecq, I. y Greca, I. (2005b). Análisis de aspectos relevantes para el abordaje de la
Teoría de la Relatividad Especial en los últimos años de la enseñanza media desde una
106 Irene Arriassecq and Ileana M. Greca
Chapter 4
ABSTRACT
According to the teleological view of nature before Darwin the function of an organ
determined its form, in other words organisms were considered to possess those organs
that would enable them to perform all necessary functions properly. Darwin‘s
evolutionary theory advanced the priority of form over function, in the sense that the
existing variation and the available forms within a population posed constraints to the
possible performed functions. Considering these, it is interesting that studies in
conceptual development research have shown that young children exhibit an intuitive
teleological reasoning. Two different explanations for its origin have been proposed
which agree on that it is characteristic of students‘ explanations for biological properties
from very early in childhood. Since teleological explanations both in pre-Darwinian times
and in students‘ conceptual development are based on preconceived plans or on the
intention to achieve goals, in this chapter the major types of teleology that Darwin‘s
theory had to confront are described and studies in conceptual development research
focusing on intuitive teleological reasoning are reviewed. Then particular implications for
evolution education are discussed and it is concluded that evolution instruction should
focus on conceptual change from explanations that presuppose design etiologies, which
were popular in pre-Darwinian times and which also predominate in students‘ intuitive
reasoning, to explanations that presuppose consequence etiologies which are compatible
with Darwin‘s theory and current evolutionary biology.
*
[email protected] , [email protected].
†
[email protected].
108 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
INTRODUCTION
In order to explain a particular effect, one needs to identify its cause. Causes are usually
to be found in the past and to precede their effects. For example, the circulation of the blood
can be explained in terms of the function of the heart (‗blood circulates in the body because
the heart pumped it‘) and the particular arrangement of the books in a bookshop can be
explained in terms of the action of the bookshop owner (‗the books are distributed on the
bookshelves thematically because the bookshop owner put them that way‘). However, these
particular effects can also be explained in a different way. The circulation of the blood can be
explained in terms of the function of the heart in the human body (‗blood circulates in the
body in order to deliver oxygen to the tissues‘) and the particular arrangement of the books in
the bookshop can be explained in terms of the intention of the bookshop owner (‗the books
are distributed on the bookshelves thematically in order to facilitate customers in finding the
books of their interest‘). These explanations, in which the effect seems to serve a particular
goal or purpose (telos), are called teleological explanations. In teleological explanations
something happens ‗for the sake of‘ something else; something takes place ‗in order to‘ make
something else possible. Hence, at first sight in the teleological explanations mentioned above
causes seem to be found in the future. However, this is not the case because one also needs to
take into account the evolutionary and developmental history of the human circulatory system
as well as the intention of the bookshop owner (for relevant analyses see Ruse 2003; Lewens
2004; Ariew 2007).
Teleological explanations, within the Western intellectual tradition, have their roots in the
philosophies of Plato (427-347 B.C.) and of his student Aristotle (384-322 B.C.) (Lennox
1992a; Ariew 2002). These two philosophers built the foundations for the study of natural
philosophy and natural history, and their views had an enormous influence on scientific
thought until the 19th century. In this chapter the major aspects of Plato‘s and Aristotle‘s
teleological reasoning are presented, and their relation to the teleological reasoning of
William Paley (1743-1805) and Georges Cuvier (1773–1838), respectively, is also discussed.
Paley‘s and Cuvier‘s were the most important (teleological) approaches to the study of nature
at the beginning of the 19th century and were actually those that Charles Darwin‘s (1809-
1882) theory had to confront. However, even Darwin‘s explanations can be considered in a
particular, limited sense as teleological.
Teleological explanations have also been found to exist among students‘ intuitive
explanations of biological phenomena (Southerland et al. 2001; Shtulman 2006; Kampourakis
and Zogza 2008). Two explanations have been proposed for the origin of this reasoning.
According to one view young children prefer teleological explanations mostly for biological
kinds. They view artifacts, such as chairs, and biological traits, such as eyes, as existing to
perform particular functions and tend to differentiate them from non-living natural objects,
such as mountains, which are not considered to perform particular functions (Keil 1992;
1994; 1995). According to another view, teleological explanations result from young
children‘s understanding of intentionality and are not limited to any particular type of objects
until later in development. Hence, children think that both artifacts and natural objects of any
type (both living and non-living) perform a particular function (Kelemen 1999a; 1999b;
1999c). Despite these differences, both proposals agree that children adopt design-based
teleological explanations for living kinds early in childhood which are maintained until the
Teleology and Evolution Education 109
age of 10 years old. Moreover, a separate body of research suggests that teleological
explanations seem to be independent of the religiosity of students‘ families (Evans, 2001;
Kelemen, 2003). This body of research suggests that, besides students‘ religious views, a
major obstacle for understanding evolution might be this intuitive teleological reasoning.
Several studies in conceptual development research have documented that resistance to
scientific ideas may derive from assumptions and biases that originate early in childhood and
that may persist into adulthood. Both adults and children have been found to resist acquiring
scientific information when it clashes with their intuitions about the world. Moreover, it
seems that the main source of resistance depends on what children know before they are
taught about science, and as far as evolution is concerned it is their teleological intuitions
about agency and design that make it difficult to accept (Bloom and Weisberg, 2007). In
addition, patients with Alzheimer disease were found to have a tendency to provide
teleological explanations similar to that of pre-school children, as they explained that rain
exists in order to provide water for plants and animals and that trees existed in order to
provide shade. These findings suggest that the preference for teleological explanations may
persist throughout life and reemerge when other beliefs are limited or compromised
(Lombrozo et al. 2007). Hence, analyzing and understanding students‘ teleological
explanations may be useful for evolution education. For this reason, these explanations could
be the target of any instruction aiming at conceptual change, as a prerequisite to achieve
learning.
The process of conceptual change is the process that leads to the rejection of old concepts
and to the accommodation of new ones. This requires a conceptual conflict situation during
which students find their current concepts inadequate for grasping new phenomena
successfully and, as a result, replace or reorganize their concepts. For conceptual change to
occur there must be dissatisfaction with existing concepts, and the new concepts must be
intelligible, must initially appear plausible, and must suggest the possibility of a fruitful
research program (Posner et al. 1982). Conceptual change can be not only the replacement of
old concepts with new ones but also the replacement of an old pattern of constructing
explanations with a new one. Hence, conceptual change from intuitive to evolutionary
explanations can be achieved if students have found their explanations insufficient to explain,
if they have found the new explanations easy to understand and to apply to new cases as well
as sufficient to explain phenomena they have been previously unable to explain, and if the
new explanations have the potential to open new areas of inquiry. A conceptual change
process from intuitive to evolutionary explanations should include: a) documentation of
students‘ intuitive explanations, b) a conceptual conflict situation, where students‘ intuitive
explanations would be challenged, and c) instruction on how to apply the new explanatory
framework to novel cases. The more critical of these stages is the stage of conceptual conflict.
Three conditions need to be fulfilled in order that students experience conceptual conflict
between two conceptions: a) both conceptions have to be intelligible b) both conceptions have
to be comparable and c) only one of the two conceptions has to be plausible (Hewson and
Hewson 1984). Hence, for conceptual conflict to occur between explanations it is necessary
that students can understand both of them, that they can compare them to realize that they are
in conflict and that they can apply both of them to new cases and realize that only one of them
is plausible. If this process is successful, conceptual change will probably occur; students will
reject their intuitive explanations and they will accommodate the new, more plausible ones
introduced to them during instruction.
110 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
In this chapter, the major types of teleology that Darwin‘s theory had to confront are
described and it is shown that it differs significantly from the theories of Paley and Cuvier.
Moreover, the sense in which Darwin‘s theory can be regarded as teleological is explained; in
his case the explanation for the presence of traits is based on their function without any
reference to final ends or purposes that drive the development of the organism. Hence, it is
argued that Darwin‘s explanations are a type of teleological explanation (that is based on
consequence etiologies) which is essentially different from the teleological explanations of
Paley and Cuvier in which final ends predominate (that is based on design etiologies). In
addition, studies in conceptual development research focusing on students‘ intuitive
teleological explanations are reviewed and it is shown that students exhibit a preference for
design etiologies from very early in childhood. We conclude that evolution instruction should
focus on conceptual change from design etiologies, which predominate in students‘ intuitive
reasoning, to consequence etiologies which are compatible with current evolutionary biology.
Plato had addressed in his dialogues some important issues that would later become
central questions both in philosophy and in science. In Timaeus, Plato attempted to explain
the creation of the universe as the artifact of a Divine Craftsman or Demiurge. The motive to
develop this idea seems to have been the wide spread of the view of mechanistic materialism
in the beginnings of the 4th century B.C., according to which there was no design and order in
the structure of the universe. Plato considered the universe as a logical, living entity
possessing a soul which was the primary cause of any transformation. As the dominant entity,
the soul controlled everything through the final causes which determined every action in
which the soul was involved, managing to impose control on any chance events (Κάλφας
1995, p. 85-90). For Plato, the final cause of the creation of the universe was the transfusion
of the soul of the Demiurge in his artifact, something that could be achieved by the imposition
of order over disorder. Order in the perceived universe was equal to the existence and
domination of the mind. The universe should have a body, but a mind and a soul as well in
order to be a perfect artifact. Hence, the major aim of the Demiurge was to form the universe
and to create its soul (Κάλφας 1995, p. 69-70).
This process had to take into account the actions of Need that seemed to impose
constraints to the work of the Demiurge. Need was a second force, after the Demiurge, and
was the mythical equivalent of the action of the co-causes, meaning the properties that had to
do with the structure of matter. Thus, Plato recognized two types of causes: the divine (final)
and the necessary (mechanistic), causes and co-causes respectively, and thought that they
were interdependent and not in conflict (Κάλφας 1995, p. 283). Consequently, the universe
was an artifact that resulted from the purposeful and rational action of the Demiurge that had
finally dominated over the irrational Need (Κάλφας 1995, p. 92). Teleological explanations
predominate in Timaeus, and its actual contribution in natural philosophy is the perception of
the universe as an artifact as well as the domination of teleology over mechanistic causality
(Κάλφας 1995, p. 107-109). This idea eventually regards the natural world as ―unnatural‖,
Teleology and Evolution Education 111
as it is not the product of natural processes but the product of art and of a benevolent and wise
craftsman (Lennox, 2001, p. 281).
Aristotle, a student of Plato, was influenced by his teacher‘s views. However, his
philosophy differs significantly from Plato‘s. The differences between these two philosophers
can be summarized in the statement that Aristotle viewed theology in natural terms whereas
Plato viewed nature in theological terms. Aristotle inherited the teleological argumentation
against mechanistic materialism from Plato (Κάλφας 1999, p. 53). But contrary to Plato,
Aristotle posed important questions relevant to biological phenomena which he attempted to
answer through their deep understanding; he tried to provide answers by identifying natural
causes within the organisms, rather than by looking for causes beyond the organism (Moore
1993, p. 41-42). Aristotle thought that there were four causes acting within nature and that
knowledge could be gained through their understanding. These causes were the Efficient
cause, the Material cause or matter, the Formal cause or form and the Final cause and
Aristotle considered all four of them as necessary for explanations (Κάλφας 1999, p. 116-
117). Matter and form were not causes in the current sense of the term; the former referred
not only to the material a body was made of but also to any circumstance required to make
this happen, whereas the latter referred to the internal structure and not only to shape of the
body (Κάλφας 1999, p. 201).
Aristotle thought that final causes served the maintenance of the organism. In particular,
the final cause for the existence of an organ would be its usefulness to the organism that
possessed it when it would use it (Ruse 2003, p.17-19). Contrary to Plato who accepted
teleological explanations as long as the explanandum was the outcome of rational design,
Aristotle thought that organisms acquired some features because they were functionally
useful to their life without any requirement of a preceding intention or design (Lennox 1992a;
1992b). For Aristotle the teleological approach was the main approach to understanding
biological phenomena and it seems that in many cases it actually made him identify functions
that would not have been noticed in a solely descriptive approach. In Timaeus, Plato had
described the possible role of invisible structures in the organisms‘ functions, thus failing to
identify the exact structures that were involved in particular functions. On the contrary,
Aristotle, having studied systematically the anatomy of organisms, succeeded in explaining in
detail which structures were related to particular functions (Cosans 1998).
Before Darwin the teleological approach was widely used to explain the origin of the
complex structures that could be found in nature. The world was considered to exhibit so
much complexity that could only be explained as the result of the work of a Designer. This
argument is briefly described as the argument from design. An important criticism of this
worldview came by David Hume (1711-1776) who made the following objections: a) evil,
imperfections and wasted material could be found in the world, b) more than one designers
could be perceived to exist, and c) the argument as originally stated could explain some but
not all types of complexity found in the world (Oppy 1996). Hume‘s criticism resulted from
his empiricist approach in the study of nature and from his intention to protect philosophy
from the anti-empiricist approach of theology: ―…that the universe, sometime, arose from
112 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
something like design: But beyond that position he cannot ascertain one single circumstance,
and is left afterwards to fix every point of his theology by the utmost license of fancy and
hypothesis. This world for aught he knows, is very faulty and imperfect, compared to a
superior standard…‖ (Hume 1779/1993, p.71). However, in pre-Darwinian times and despite
Hume‘s criticism, the Platonic and the Aristotelian teleological approaches in the study of
nature found two important proponents: William Paley and Georges Cuvier, respectively.
Paley was the major proponent of natural theology and believed that the complexity and
perfection of the natural world, documented by its empirical study, were the most powerful
argument for the existence of God (Ruse 2003, p.41). According to this view, all patterns,
symmetries and laws found in nature reflected the thoughts of God. Consequently, the study
of nature was a way to prove His existence. This approach, to prove something that should
not demand proof, could have been considered blasphemous in earlier times. But Paley lived
at the end of a long period of reformations, of production of new knowledge and of
expression of heretical ideas. Hence, as philosophers like Hume had injured the authoritarian
status of religion, through the argument that it was based only on faith and not on factual data,
Paley tried to confront this skepticism with rational arguments and data taken from the study
of nature (Thomson 2005, p. 6-10). In particular, Paley used the metaphor of the organism as
a watch and of God as a watchmaker, according to which a complex structure, like the watch,
could not have emerged accidentally but required the existence of a designer, in this case of a
watchmaker (Paley 2006/1802, p.7-8). By comparing a stone and a watch in terms of
complexity and by putting emphasis on the idea of a complex as well as of a self-replicating
watch, Paley came to two major conclusions: 1) the more complex the parts of a structure
were, the more powerful was the evidence of the existence of a designer and 2) the more
complex the design was, the more intelligent and capable was the designer. Then, based on
these conclusions, he attempted to support that tissues, organs, organisms and ecosystems
were much more complex than a self-replicating watch and hence they demanded an even
more capable designer: God (Ariew 2007). The analogy between Paley‘s designer and Plato‘s
Demiurge is obvious; however it is important to note that not everybody in the mainstream
Church approved of attempts to prove God through science. Contrary to what natural
theology suggested, the traditional view was that the existence of God could be documented
through the study of the Bible (Thomson 2005, p.8).
On the other hand, the most important proponent of Aristotelian teleology in the time
before Darwin was Cuvier, who had been influenced by the works of Aristotle (Taquet 2006,
p.179-184). One of his major contributions was the synthesis of paleontology, classification
and comparative anatomy on the basis of a teleological view of nature according to which
function determined form. The founding principle of Cuvier‘s biology were the ―conditions
for existence‖ (conditions d‘ existence), which were a synonym for the final causes.
According to this principle the Creator, having taken into account the conditions that were
necessary for the survival and the reproduction of an organism, had created those organs that
were needed to achieve this goal (Ruse 2003, p.60-64). Cuvier‘s teleology formed the
foundations of biology before the publication of Darwin‘s Origin of Species (1859). It was
accommodated by naturalists both within France and abroad, especially in Great Britain
because it was in accordance with the widespread view of natural theology (Appel 1987,
p.41). It should be noted that Cuvier was a Protestant; natural theology was a mostly British
phenomenon and was not widely accepted in Catholic France (Appel 1987, p.55-57). The
principle of the conditions for existence was very different from Paley‘s approach. Although
Teleology and Evolution Education 113
Cuvier accepted the existence of a Creator, he did not put emphasis on the confirmation of
His existence but on the explanation of the features that organisms possessed on the basis of
their function. An organism could not exist if it did not fulfill the conditions necessary for its
existence; hence the several parts of the organism should be coordinated in such a way so as
to make its existence possible. It should be noted that the conditions that Cuvier referred to
had to do with the organism exclusively and not with the conditions of the environment in
which he lived (Reiss 2005). A crucial consequence of Cuvier‘s teleological approach was
that evolution was not only empirically false, but also conceptually impossible. If the
structure of the organisms was arranged in teleological terms, then the transition from one
form to another would be precluded. Transitional forms would not be suited, in teleological
terms, neither for the way of life of the ancestors, not for that of the descendants because their
parts would fail to serve the purpose of the organisms overall well-being. Consequently, there
could be no link between different groups, no transitional forms and hence no evolutionary
transition through time (Ruse 2000).
The relation of Darwin‘s theory to teleology has been viewed in many different ways by
philosophers of science. Karl Popper (1902-1994) initially suggested that Darwin‘s theory
was not a scientific theory but rather a ―metaphysical research program‖, which was not much
better than the theistic (teleological) view of adaptation, although it had some practical value
(Popper 1974, p. 134-138). Contrary to this view, Thomas Kuhn (1922-1996) considered
Darwin‘s theory as the first of the evolutionary theories that not only rejected the notion of
evolution as a goal-directed process but also provided the arguments for the abolition of that
teleological kind of evolution (Kuhn 1996, p. 171-172). Finally, some later philosophers think
that Darwin‘s theory relieved teleological explanation from supernatural elements and placed
it in the context of nature. The view that evolutionary explanations which appeal to natural
selection are in fact teleological explanations results from the fact that the explanation for the
presence of a feature is based on its function and to its contribution to the survival of its
possessors (Ayala 1970; Brandon 1981; Lennox 1993; Ruse 2002; Depew 2008).
The presence of an organ that performs a particular function may be explained through
this function, by invoking natural selection: if an organ has been maintained through the
survival of its possessors during the process of natural selection as a consequence of its
function, it can be suggested that the cause of the presence of this organ is the fact that it
performs this function. In other words, the explanation for the presence of the organ is based
on the consequence of its existence, which is its function (Wright, 1973). This is described as
a consequence etiological approach to function, as it is based on processes that presuppose
consequence etiologies. The Darwinian explanation based on natural selection is unlike any
of the standard forms of teleology of the 19th century (Paley, Cuvier); however, it is
inherently teleological as the advantage that a trait provides to its possessors explains its
increase, or its presence, in a population. When one examines the Darwinian explanation
carefully, a robust form of teleology is revealed. The structure of the Darwinian explanation
can be described as follows:
114 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
Several objections have been raised on the relation between Darwin‘s theory of natural
selection and teleology. These are based on the argument that Darwin‘s relation to teleology
cannot be documented only on the basis of the structure of his arguments, while ignoring the
epistemological aspects of teleology in the study of nature. According to this view Darwin‘s
approach was not teleological, he considered teleology and its supporters as the major
opponents his theory had to confront and when it seems that teleological language is used in
his writings it is due to his being careless in the use of language (Ghiselin 1994; Ghiselin
2003/1969, p. 134-159; Mayr 2004, p. 42). In addition, these authors have suggested that the
word teleonomy might be used in order to describe ―systems operating on the basis of a
program, a code of information‖ and to designate ―the apparent purposefulness of organisms
and their characteristics‖ (Mayr 1961, p.1504) as well as ―to designate the sort of
adaptationist thinking that ought not to be confused with teleology‖ (Ghiselin 1994, p. 489-
490). However, an objection has been raised to the use of this term because the etymological
roots of the term teleonomy would suggest that it means ‗laws which refer to ends‘, which is
essentially what Kant and his followers intended to describe by the term teleology (Lennox
1994, p.493). A more recent definition suggests that ‗a teleonomic process or behavior is one
that owes its goal-directedness to the influence of an evolved program‘ (Mayr 2004, p. 51).
According to this, the term teleonomic is not a substitute for the term teleological in general
but only for one of its different meanings; teleonomic processes are characterized by two
components: they are guided by a genetic program and they depend on the existence of some
endpoint, goal or terminus that is ‗foreseen‘ in the program that regulates the behavior or
process (p.52).
It should be noted that Darwin‘s relation to teleology was not clear even to his
contemporaries, as he was praised and blamed both for promoting and for undermining
teleology (Beatty 1990, p. 124). As Peter Bowler has stated ―…this view [that any mechanism
for maintaining adaptation is compatible with design] is difficult to sustain in the case of
natural selection, where the element of chance variation seems to make the outcome
unpredictable and where death is the driving force of change‖ (Bowler 2003, p.205). In this
sense Darwin‘s theory differs significantly from the teleological approaches of the 19th
century. Consequently, it is important to make clear what kind of teleology his theory entails,
as the explanation for the presence of traits is based on their function without any reference to
final ends or purposes that drive the development of the organism. According to Darwin, the
explanation that e.g. birds have wings because wings are for flying is understood in terms of
an evolutionary argument in which it is explained how possessing wings provided a fitness
advantage to individual birds, resulting to wings being selected and eventually becoming
prevalent in bird populations. This is a teleological explanation which is not based on a final
end or a purpose but on a natural, selective process (Ariew 2007). The major features of the
teleological explanations of Paley, Cuvier and Darwin are presented in Table 1.
Teleology and Evolution Education 115
Table 1. The major features of the teleological explanations of Paley, Cuvier and Darwin
Relation
Factor that
Type of Final ends Cause of between Type of
determines
teleology served adaptations form and explanation
the final ends
function
Structure Α
Final end: survival
External External function exists in order
Yes of the organism in a
(Paley) (God) determines to perform
given environment
form function B
Internal (the Final end: survival Structure Α
function
Internal preservation of the organism exists in order
Yes determines
(Cuvier) of the (independently of to perform
form
organism) the environment) function B
Structure Α
Differential survival form exists because
Selective
No - of organisms in a constraints it has
(Darwin)
given environment function performed
function B
Another objection to the etiological account has to do more generally with the existence
of teleological explanations in biology. This is based on an analysis described as functional
analysis (Cummins 1975), and is actually an attack on the consequence etiological approach
to functions presented above (Wright 1973). The etiological approach to functions has been
defended by several philosophers (Neander 1991; Griffiths 1993; Kitcher 1993; Godfrey-
Smith 1994; Allen and Beckoff 1995). However, it has been argued that to explain the
existence of a trait in terms of the function it has performed and still performs provides an
inadequate view of the evolutionary process (Cummins 2002). Cummins argues that
biological traits exist not because of their functions but because of their developmental
histories. Whether or not a trait has a function and what that function happens to be is
independent of whether it was selected for it. To explain selection, Cummins suggests, one
must look not at the function of a trait but on how well the several varieties of a trait are
functioning, because selection presupposes the existence of such variation. Hence, the
explanation of the existence of traits in terms of their functions can be misleading. Such
explanations ―either run into the fact, fatal to classical teleology, that the crucial details of
evolutionary (or ontogenic) development predate anything with the function that is supposed
to do the explaining, or they founder on the fact that competing traits in selection scenarios
typically have the same function. Things don‘t evolve because of their functions any more
than they develop because of their functions‖ (Cummins 2002, p. 169).
Although this account seems to be entirely opposed to the etiological analysis, it has been
suggested that these ideas should be integrated with each other. On one hand functional
analysis focuses on the identification of current causal contributions in complex processes, a
very prominent activity in physiological studies. On the other hand, etiological analysis
116 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
focuses on the origin of functions through selective processes, with functions making causal
contributions to responses to older selection pressures. Functional analysis on its own can be
quite liberal as it might explain any complex system as performing a function e.g. that a
function of a particular arrangement of rocks is to contribute to the widening of a river delta
or that the function of particular mutations is to promote the formation of tumors. On the
other hand, the etiological analysis may not be always applicable as there are traits that
perform functions although they have not been selected for these functions. However, when
this is the case the etiological analysis can provide useful information for the origin of a trait
that cannot be obtained through the functional analysis (Kitcher 1993). Even if the unification
of these two approaches is not accepted, it is still clear that the etiological and the functional
analyses are both real, important and distinct (Godfrey-Smith 1993). This seems to be like a
pluralistic approach to the study of functions that may provide two distinct definitions: a
historical one which is based on Wright‘s etiological analysis of functions and explains how a
trait has been selected for the function it currently performs and a non-historical one that
which is based on Cummins‘ functional analysis that explains the function itself. Interestingly
enough, historical and non-historical definitions exist for adaptation as well (Lewens 2007).
Evidence from cognitive development research suggests that children naturally construe
the biological world in teleological terms. This evidence comes mainly from the research of
Frank Keil (1992; 1994; 1995) and Deborah Kelemen (1999a; 1999b; 1999c) who have
provided two distinct explanations for this. Keil has argued that children naturally construe
the biological world in teleological terms because of the particular properties of biological
kinds, whereas Kelemen has argued that this happens because children tend to construe
everything in these terms.
In particular, according to Keil teleological explanations are mainly used in biological
thought which is an autonomous mode of construal. As a result, students distinguish
organisms from artifacts, although they can be given teleological explanations as well. In
Keil‘s view this is due to two main differences between organisms and artifacts: a) although
both organisms and artifacts exhibit properties that could be explained in teleological terms,
on the basis of the purposes they seem to serve, there is a major difference in that these
purposes are usually more self-serving for the organisms (for example roses have thorns to
keep animals from getting at them whereas barbed wire has barbs to keep animals from
getting at something valuable to humans) and b) organisms have clear essences and causally
more potent ones compared to artifacts (Keil 1994, p.248-249). Keil has argued that young
children are sensitive to the advantages of the teleological-functional mode of construal for
organisms, tending to associate it with organisms than with other sorts of things. Hence, he
suggested that in addition to the intentional and physical stances a design stance might also
exist that would look at things as if their properties were designed for purposes and that this
seems to be the preferred mode of explanation for properties of organisms (Keil 1992, p.127-
128).
Teleology and Evolution Education 117
According to Keil the last property is perhaps the most distinctive one between organisms
and artifacts. Organisms seem to have mutually supportive networks of causal relations
among their properties, which are highly interrelated because they tend to support, either
directly or indirectly, the presence of each other. Consequently, the presence of a property
will help increase the likelihood of other properties being present: if each property increases
the odds of the survival of the organism, it will increase the likelihood of all other properties
through survival (Keil 1995, p.236).
On the other hand, according to Kelemen the bias to view objects as made for some
purpose derives from an early sensitivity to intentional agents and their behavior as object
makers and users. The teleological bias to explain all kinds of phenomena in terms of
purposes might occur due to children‘s drawing on their early knowledge of intentional
behavior. Thus, in the absence of other explanations they treat objects of all kinds as artifacts
that have been intentionally made to serve a purpose (Kelemen 1999a). In one study,
preschool children were asked what they thought living things, artifacts, non-living natural
objects and their physical parts were for. Despite the fact that they were explicitly given the
option to answer that these were not ‗for‘ anything, four and five year old children provided
answers in which they considered almost every type of object and part as having a particular
function (not only eyes were for seeing and noses for smelling but also mountains were for
climbing and clouds for raining). In a second study, it was investigated if children really
considered these activities as functions or as things that the objects simply could do or be
used to do. As in the previous case, the study found that preschool children explained the
origin of objects or parts of all types in teleological terms, as having been intentionally made
for a purpose (Kelemen 1999b). Finally, in a third study it was found that seven and eight
year old children provided teleological explanations for both living and non- living natural
objects. They did not only explain animal properties, such as long necks, in terms of a
purpose, but also used this kind of explanation for non-biological natural properties, such as
pointy rocks, even after they had heard a description of the non-teleological physical process
by which natural objects are formed (Kelemen 1999c). These findings suggest that, rather
than being selectively teleological, preschool and elementary school children possess a
118 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
general teleological bias, a promiscuous teleology, unlike older children and adults who
usually restrict their teleological view to biological properties. According to this proposal,
people find purpose-based explanations compelling because teleological explanations derive
from a mode of thought that is familiar: intentional reasoning. People‘s tendency to attribute
purpose to objects might derive from our ability to attribute purpose to the minds of agents.
Moreover, people seem to have an early awareness of intentional object use as, during
infancy, most of the objects that children encounter are artifacts, whose presence in their
environment is explained by the way agents use them to achieve their own goals (Kelemen
1999a, p.466).
Despite their different views on the actual developmental process, both Keil and Kelemen
suggest that children with no particular biological knowledge intuitively provide teleological
explanations to explain biological properties and functions. However, although Keil and
Kelemen agree about the role of teleological explanations in explaining biological parts (e.g.
why do giraffe's have long necks?), they disagree about their role in explaining entire animals
(e.g. why are there giraffes?). Kelemen has argued that young children overextend
attributions of function, purpose, and design to all types of objects. Hence, pre-school
children tend to ask ‗‗What is it for?‘‘ questions and to look for purpose or function for all
objects, both organisms and artifacts (Kelemen 1999b). Moreover, in order to provide
explanations for the existence of particular properties children think of organisms as artifacts
(Kelemen 1999c). On the other hand, Keil suggests that preschool children look for different
information about animals and artifacts, and although they ask about the functions of artifacts,
they do not impute design and function to organisms. Hence, they are more likely to ask
questions about functions and behaviors of artifacts than to ask similar questions for animals.
Thus, children are able to clearly distinguish between organisms and artifacts from very early
in childhood (Greif et al. 2006).
exhibit a bias for endorsing intentional accounts of how species originated, regardless of the
religiosity of their background.
Similar findings were reported by another study that involved American and British
students. Based on the assumption that America and Britain share many cultural
characteristics but differ in religiosity, with the British being less religious than the
Americans, the study aimed at investigating if there was a difference in American and British
elementary, aged 7-10 years old, students‘ preferences for teleological explanations (Kelemen
2003). What was novel in this study was data on British students, as data form Americans
was obtained from an older study (Kelemen 1999c). Despite some differences in details, the
explanations of students of both groups were quite similar, as they generally preferred
teleological explanations both for organisms and for natural objects. Although British
children were considered less likely to be exposed to statements reinforcing the notions of
intention or design in nature, the amount of exposure that they might have received was
sufficient. In other words, it could be argued that children only require a minor or no
environmental support to establish a bias to apply teleological explanation promiscuously.
The reason for this could be that the tendency to attribute purpose to objects may easily be
established, having as its primary source the cognitive predisposition to attribute purpose to
others‘ actions (Kelemen 2003).
These studies suggest that the teleological stance is not necessarily the result of the
religiosity of students‘ social background. Then, perhaps children are naturally inclined to
privilege intentional explanation and are oriented toward explanations characterizing nature
as an intentionally designed artifact. A bias to explain, plus a preference for intentional
explanation, may then be what leads children, in the absence of other knowledge, to a
generalized view of objects as intentionally created by someone for a purpose. Then, if
children have beliefs about purpose and about the intentional origins of nature, it might be
possible that they are intuitive theists, insofar as they are predisposed to develop a view of
nature as an artifact of nonhuman design (Kelemen 2004). To accept this characterization, it
was necessary to show that children‘s tendency to reason about natural phenomena in terms
of a purpose (Kelemen, 1999b) and their intuitions about intelligent design in nature, whether
or not they come from fundamentalist religious backgrounds (Evans 2001), were related in a
systematic fashion.
To examine if such a relation exists, British elementary school children (aged 6-10 years
old) were given tasks probing their intuitions about purpose and intelligent design in the
context of their explanations about the origins of natural phenomena. In particular, they were
shown photographs of two artifacts (a hat and a boat), two animals (a bird and a monkey),
two natural events (a thunderstorm and a flood) and two nonliving natural objects (a mountain
and a river). Children were told that they would be asked questions about these objects for
which nobody might know the answer and that they should give their best ideas. Then they
were asked to answer: a) open-ended origins questions b) closed ended origins-teleology
questions and c) closed-ended intelligent-design questions. It was found that children were
most likely to provide teleo-functional explanations for artifacts as well as for artifact-like
natural objects and animals, but not for natural events, to open-ended questions. The same
pattern of results was documented in younger children‘s responses in the closed-ended
origins-teleology task and closed-ended intelligent-design task, in which artifacts and animals
were particularly identified as being the products of intentional creation. Moreover, these
teleological and intelligent design intuitions were found to be interconnected. The results
120 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
difference-making factors. Hence, the explanations can have the following structure: in
explaining a forest fire by appeal to lightning, one indicates a cause (i.e. the lightning),
presupposes a broader regularity of which the explanandum is an instance (i.e. that lightning
can cause fires under certain conditions) and determines which aspects of the complex causal
etiology of the explanandum are explanatorily relevant (i.e. the lightning but not the presence
of oxygen or the sound of accompanying thunder) (Lombrozo 2006).
Many studies have documented that secondary students, and undergraduates, tend to
provide teleological explanations for evolution. However, it should be noted that researchers
have not always explicitly distinguished teleological explanation from other types of
explanation. On the contrary, in several studies although it was found that students tended to
attribute the origin of traits to predetermined plans or to the achievement of desired goals,
their preconceptions about evolution were characterized as Lamarckian, a characterization
that actually masks their teleological nature (Kampourakis and Zogza 2007). For example
during the pre-test of one study 35 out of 50 secondary students provided teleological
explanations for evolution. In particular, these students explained the appearance of traits as
the consequence of the animals‘ requirements for survival, while often the explanations for
the evolution of a trait contained the words need or necessary (Settlage 1994). In a study with
college students, it was found that most of them understood evolution as a process in which
species respond to environmental conditions by changing gradually over time and they
attributed changes in traits to need-driven adaptive processes (Bishop and Anderson, 1990).
In another study with college students it was found that their knowledge about evolution be
fore instruction was limited and mixed. The most common alternative conceptions were
related to teleology and ranged between 19.36% and 26.12% of the total number of responses
(Jensen and Finley, 1996). In an interesting study with students of various ages the patterns
that characterize students‘ explanations of biological phenomena were explored. Teleological
explanations about biological phenomena were the most prevalent category in all grade
levels. However, it seemed that there was some improvement in students‘ ability to reason
scientifically within biological domains as grade levels increased (Southerland, Abrams,
Cummins and Anselmo, 2001). Finally, in a recent study of 14-15 year old students‘ intuitive
explanations of evolution, it was found that in most cases teleological explanations
predominated, with the end or goal being the survival of the species. It was found that in
general students had the tendency to look for purpose or plan when they did not have
adequate information (Kampourakis and Zogza, 2008).
Teleological explanations seem to prevail in biology because: a) Organisms seem goal-
oriented because their structure is often adapted to their survival, b) People tend to project
from their own goals and intentions to natural phenomena, c) Teleological explanations have
apparent explanatory value because they tend to make us feel that we really understand the
phenomenon in question, as it is accounted for in terms of purposes, with which we are
familiar from our own experience of purposive behaviour, and d) There is heuristic value to
teleological approaches, as in the history of science biological research that was motivated by
a teleological orientation often resulted in important scientific discoveries (Zohar and
Ginossar, 1998). Teleological explanations are compatible with religious beliefs about
122 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
Creation and Intelligent Design and perhaps make evolution seemingly easier to understand.
If it is explained that it is consequence and not design etiologies which should be applied to
explain evolution by natural selection there are two possible outcomes: a) one may fail to
understand evolution, as it is more easily understood in terms of design rather than in terms of
natural selection, and b) one may not accept evolution because the elimination of design is not
compatible with his religious beliefs. These two outcomes may not presuppose each other, as
the relation between understanding and accepting evolution is not clear and diverse findings
stem from research in post-secondary settings. One body of research suggests that
understanding evolution may influence its acceptance and that the misunderstanding of the
nature of evolutionary theory may be motivated by factors shaped by students‘ beliefs
(Dagher and Boujaoude 2005). Moreover, teaching about the nature of science can enhance
understanding of evolutionary theory if students discuss their beliefs in relation to scientific
knowledge (Dagher and BouJaoude 1997). On the other hand, students may also perceive a
negative impact of evolutionary theory on the social and the personal aspects of life, and
regard the consequences of accepting evolution more negative when more time was given to
its teaching (Brem et al. 2003). However, another body of research suggests that no
relationship may exist between the understanding and the acceptance of evolution. Students‘
initial acceptance of evolution may not influence their subsequent learning of the subject
(Ingram and Nelson, 2005). More generally, students may have an understanding of
evolutionary theory without accepting its validity, or alternatively, they may accept the
validity of the theory based upon a poor understanding of it (Sinatra et al. 2003). These
findings do not provide a definite answer, but suggest that beliefs about evolution cannot be
regarded in isolation. A proper understanding of evolutionary theory and its consequences
may require lessons from the philosophy of science about what empirical claims are, as well
as lessons from moral philosophy about the difference between empirical claims and moral
claims (Lombrozo et al. 2006).
CONCLUSION
The main aim of this chapter was to integrate aspects from the history and philosophy of
science with newer findings from cognitive developmental psychology, in order to suggest
what the history and philosophy of science can contribute to the analysis of students‘ intuitive
explanations and to the development of a conceptual conflict situation that would facilitate
conceptual change. To summarize, the term teleology has been used to refer to two distinct
processes: a) those processes in which a function serves an ultimate goal such as the survival
of the organism, where the explanation for the presence of an organ is based on this particular
ultimate goal (a design etiology) and b) those processes in which no ultimate goals exist and
where the explanation for the presence of an organ is based on the function it performs, a
consequence of which has been the survival of the organism (a consequence etiology). Such
explanations are teleological because the presence of an organ is eventually explained on the
basis of the proximate goal that it serves: its function. However, there is a major difference as
well: in the first case the proximate goal serves an ultimate goal, the survival of the organism,
whereas in the second case no ultimate goals are served and it is the proximate goal that is the
cause of the survival of the organism and of the presence of the organ. In other words, in the
Teleology and Evolution Education 123
first case the explanation for the presence of an organ is based on the function it performs in
order to fulfill the ultimate goal it was intended to serve, whereas in the second case the
explanation for the presence of the organ is based on the function it has performed in the past
and still performs, as well as on the fact that this function has been and still is beneficial for
the organisms that possessed it. A misunderstanding of evolution may exist because the
distinction between explanations based on design or consequence etiologies is not always
made clear. While evolution could theoretically be explained in both ways, and perhaps
people intuitively tend to explain it in terms of design and purpose, only those explanations
which presuppose processes with consequence etiologies are acceptable. The research
reviewed in this chapter suggests that teleological explanations predominate in ages 8-10
years old and that they are independent of the religiosity of the family. However, religiosity
seemed to have an influence in older ages as students coming from more religious
backgrounds gave more teleological/creationistic explanations than children from less
religious families who tended to give more evolutionary explanations. Hence, students‘
religious views may not be the only obstacle in understanding evolution. What may be
another major obstacle, especially in younger ages, are their teleological intuitions. If this is
the case, poor understanding and acceptance of evolution might be due to the fact that
students do not experience a conceptual conflict at the ages when teleological intuitions
predominate.
When students come across a peculiar organism, with which they are not familiar, they
will probably wonder what some of his characteristic body structures are for. For example, if
they saw an elephant or a giraffe for the first time, some very peculiar animals with which we
are familiar, they might wonder why they need such a long trunk or such a long neck,
respectively. If the students were aware of the habits of these animals, they would probably
provide the following answer: ―the elephant has a long trunk in order to collect food and bring
it into its mouth‖ or that ―the giraffe has a long neck in order to browse on the leaves of trees
and feed itself, when the food on the ground is not adequate‖. In the same sense, the students
would say that fishes have particular (hydrodynamic) shapes in order to move easily and
quickly in the water, that polar bears have thick furs in order to stay warm in the cold
environment in which they live or that many animals have a body color that resembles their
environment in order to conceal themselves and escape from their predators. These
explanations seem quite sound. Many adults would provide these particular explanations and
they might also be found in biology textbooks. The answers to ―Why?‖ questions often have
the form ―In order to‖ and explain the presence of particular body structures on the basis of
the function they seem to perform. Thus, we might say in brief that the usual answer to the
question ―Why do individuals of species O have structure A?‖ is that ―Individuals of species
O have structure A in order to perform function Β‖. This answer is logically sound but
incorrect from a biological point of view. Biological research has not yet documented any
single instance of purposive acquisition of body structures that perform particular functions.
In other words, there is no evidence that would support the answer that ―Individuals of
species O have structure A in order to perform function Β‖. Of course, in all these cases it
certainly is a fact that ―In individuals of species O function B is performed by structure A‖.
But these two sentences are not the same. Structure has a causal role in the production of
function, but not vice versa. The function depends on the structure but the structure does not
depend on the function. This is due to the fact that in the first place body structures have
arisen through a process that does not take into account the functions (purpose) they may
124 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
serve. In other words, the emergence of a body structure in a species is not explained by the
function it serves; it is only the spread of a body structure within a species that can be
explained by the function it performs. This is a crucial point as the emergence and the spread
of a trait are the outcomes of entirely independent processes.
The answer to the question ―Why do individuals of species O have structure A?‖ consists
of two parts. The first part suggests that the emergence of structure Α was unpredictable, in
the sense that it was the outcome of a natural process that might have had several possible
outcomes of which one eventually arose, independently of any goal or purpose. The second
part suggests that structure A exists in individuals of species O because it contributed to the
survival of their ancestors in a particular environment, while those individuals that did not
possess this structure vanished. But is it possible that the spread of the structure in a
population is not related to the survival of the individuals which possess it and that its spread
is not an outcome of a natural selection process? Of course; not all traits are adaptations.
Consequently, the answer to the question ―Why do individuals of species O have structure
A?‖ can be the following: ―Individuals of species O have structure A because it happened to
emerge in their ancestors and because, possibly but not necessarily, happened to contribute to
theirs and their descendents‘ survival in a particular environment‖. An answer like
―Individuals of species O have structure A in order to perform function Β‖ is mistaken
because no biological phenomenon could have ensured the emergence of structure A and its
usefulness. In other words, structure A might have never emerged or it might have emerged
but eventually prove to have been useless if the individuals of the species lived in another
environment. In any case, the existence of a structure in the individuals of a particular species
can be explained by a historical process through which it emerged and spread and not by the
function it performs.
A requirement for effective evolution instruction is putting emphasis on the fact that
causes are not to be found in the future. It is necessary that students realise that their intuitive
teleological explanations reverse the course of nature, as the emergence of a feature seems to
precede its cause, and that causes are to be found in the past. Evolution is a historical process;
as a result there is a causal dependence on particular antecedent conditions or events; had
these never existed, there would have been a different evolutionary outcome (Beatty 2006).
To achieve this, it is important that teachers note the distinction between design etiologies,
which are related to the intention (of factors internal or external to the organism) to achieve a
preconceived goal, and non-intentional consequence etiologies, which are related to the
Darwinian theory of evolution through natural selection. Such a distinction requires
carefulness in how evolutionary explanations are described by teachers or are presented in
textbooks. Hence, students should not learn that adaptation results from a change in the
structure, the functions or the behavior of an organism that took place in order to provide it
the ability to survive in some environmental conditions. This is what is implied when it is
stated that, for example, bacteria were adapted to antibiotics or that bacteria developed
resistance to antibiotics. Students should understand that such an explanation is wrong as
bacteria cannot intentionally develop resistance to antibiotics. Students should be given more
accurate explanations such as that adaptation results from a change in the structure, the
functions or the behavior of an organism that accidentally took place and happened to provide
it the ability to survive in particular environmental conditions, and was preserved through
natural selection. Hence, bacteria did not adapt to antibiotics; within their population some
individuals happened to have resistance to antibiotics. These individuals survived whereas
Teleology and Evolution Education 125
non-resistant individuals vanished. In other words, it was the population and not the
individuals that eventually adapted to antibiotics. A change of explanatory framework from
design etiological explanations, which characterize students‘ intuitively explanations, to
consequence etiological explanations could be a major aim of the conceptual change process
in evolution. Recent research also suggests that this kind of conceptual change can take place
with a quite satisfactory outcome (Kampourakis and Zogza, 2009).
An aim of science education should be making students able to distinguish scientific
explanations from non-scientific ones. Especially in the case of evolution students should also
be able to identify the limits between scientific content knowledge about the history and
evolution of life on the earth and philosophical or religious views about life‘s purposes and
meanings. No matter how important these are, they fall outside the realm of science. Hence,
science education should make clear that although all organisms have an inherent program
coded in their genes, it determines their development but not their future; that science cannot
prove or even support the existence of destiny or fate and that such questions fall outside its
realm. On the contrary, life is, at least in part, the outcome of contingent events that guide the
interactions between organisms and their environment in an unpredictable way. No-one‘s life
is predetermined; its course depends in part on the organisms themselves and in part on
events that can be neither predicted, nor controlled. If students accept this view of life and
liberate themselves from dogmatic and fatalistic views, a major contribution to scientific
literacy will have been made.
REFERENCES
Allen, C., Beckoff, M. (1995). Biological function, adaptation and natural design. Philosophy
of Science, 62, 609-622
Appel, T.A. (1987). The Cuvier-Geoffroy debate: French biology in the decades before
Darwin. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Ariew, A. (2002). Platonic and Aristotelian roots of teleological arguments in cosmology and
biology. In: Ariew A, Cummins R, Perlman M (eds.) Functions: new essays in the
philosophy of psychology and biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 7-32
Ariew, A. (2007). Teleology. In: Hull D, Ruse M (eds.) Cambridge Companion to the
Philosophy of Biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 160-181
Ayala, F.J. (1970). Teleological explanations in evolutionary biology. Philosophy of Science,
37, 1-15
Balashov, Y., Rosenberg, A. (eds) (2002). Philosophy of science: contemporary readings.
London and New York: Routledge.
Beatty, J. (1990). Teleology and the relationship between biology and the physical sciences in
the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. In: Durham F, Purrington RD (eds) Some truer
method: reflections on the heritage of Newton. New York: Columbia University Press,
pp. 113-144
Beatty, J. (2006). Replaying life‘s tape. Journal of Philosophy, CIII(7), 336-362
Bishop, B.A., Anderson, C.W. (1990). Student conceptions of natural selection and its role in
evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27, 415–427.
126 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
Bloom, P., Weisberg, D.S. (2007). Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science,
316, 996-997
Bowler, P.J. (2003). Evolution: the history of an idea, 3rd edn. Berkeley and Los Angeles:
University of California Press
Brandon, R.N. (1981). Biological teleology: questions and explanations. Studies in the
History and Philosophy of Science, 12(2): 91-105
Brem, S.K., Ranney, M., Schindel, J. (2003). Perceived consequences of evolution: college
students perceive negative personal and social impact in evolutionary theory. Science
Education, 87(2), 181-206
Cosans, C.E. (1998). Aristotle‘s anatomical philosophy of nature. Biology and Philosophy,
13, 311-339
Cummins, R. (1975). Functional analysis. Journal of Philosophy, 72(20), 741-765
Cummins, R. (2002). Neo-teleology. In: Ariew A, Cummins R, Perlman M (eds.) Functions:
new essays in the philosophy of psychology and biology. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, pp. 157-172
Dagher, Z.R., Boujaoude, S. (1997). Scientific views and religious beliefs of college students:
the case of biological evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34(5): 429–
445
Dagher, Z.R., Boujaoude, S. (2005). Students‘ perceptions of the nature of evolutionary
theory. Science Education, 89, 378– 391
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John
Murray
Depew, D. (2008). Conequence etiology and biological teleology in Aristotle and Darwin.
Studies in the History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 39, 379-
390
Evans, E.M. (2001). Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief
systems: creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 217–266
Ghiselin, M.T. (1994). Darwin‘s language may seem teleological, but his thinking is another
matter. Biology and Philosophy, 9, 489-492
Ghiselin, M.T. (2003) [1969]. The triumph of the Darwinian method. New York: Dover
Publication
Godfrey-Smith, P. (1993). Functions: consensus without unity. Pacific Philosophical
Quarterly, 74, 196-208 (reprinted in Hull and Ruse, 1998, pp. 280-292)
Godfrey-Smith, P. (1994). A modern history theory of functions. Noûs, 28, 344-362.
Greif, M., Kemler-Nelson, D., Keil, F.C., Guiterrez, F. (2006). What do children want to
know about animals and artifacts?: Domain-specific requests for information.
Psychological Science, 17(6), 455-459
Griffiths, P. (1993). Functional analysis and proper functions. British Journal for the
Philosophy of Science, 44, 409-422
Hewson, P.W., Hewson, M.G.A. (1984). The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change
and the design of science instruction. Instructional Science, 13, 1-13
Hull, D.L., Ruse, M. (Eds) (1998). The philosophy of biology. Oxford: Oxford University
Press
Hume, D. (1993) [1779/1777]. Dialogues concerning natural religion and natural history of
religion (Gaskin JCA, ed.). Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.
Teleology and Evolution Education 127
Ingram, E.L., Nelson, C.E. (2006). Relationship between achievement and students‘
acceptance of evolution or creation in an upper-level evolution course. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 43(1), 7-24
Jensen, M.S., Finley, F.N. (1996). Changes in students‘ understanding of evolution resulting
from different curricular and instructional strategies. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 33(8), 879–900.
Kampourakis, K., Zogza, V. (2007). Students‘ preconceptions about evolution: how accurate
is the characterization as ‗‗Lamarckian‘‘ when considering the history of evolutionary
thought? Science and Education, 16(3–5), 393–422.
Kampourakis, K., Zogza, V. (2008). Students‘ intuitive explanations of the causes of
homologies and adaptations. Science and Education, 17(1)I, 27–47.
Kampourakis, K., Zogza, V. (2009). Preliminary evolutionary explanations: a basic
framework for conceptual change and explanatory coherence in evolution. Science and
Education, (online first article).
Keil, F.C. (1992). The origins of an autonomous biology. In: Gunnar MR, Maratsos M (eds),
Modularity and constraints in language and cognition. Minnesota Symposium on Child
Psychology (Vol. 25), Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum, pp. 103–138
Keil, F.C. (1994). The birth and nurturance concepts by domains: the origins of concepts of
living things. In: Hirschfeld LA, Gelman S (eds.), Mapping the mind: domain specificity
in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 234–254
Keil, F.C. (1995). The growth of causal understanding of natural kinds. In: Sperber D,
Premack D, Premack AJ (eds.), Causal cognition: a multi-disciplinary debate, Oxford:
Clarendon Press, pp. 234–262
Kelemen, D. (1999a). Function, goals and intention: children‘s teleological reasoning about
objects. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3(12), 461-468
Kelemen, D. (1999b). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70,
241–272
Kelemen, D. (1999c) Why are rocks pointy?: children‘s preference for teleological
explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1440-1452
Kelemen, D. (2003) British and American children‘s preferences for teleo-functional
explanations of the natural world. Cognition, 88, 201-221
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children ‗‗intuitive theists‘‘?: reasoning about purpose and design in
nature, Psychological Science, 15(5), 295-301
Kelemen, D., DiYanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: purpose and intelligent design in
children‘s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(1), 3–31
Kitcher, P. (1993) Function and design. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 18, 379-397
(reprinted in Hull and Ruse, 1998, pp. 258-279)
Kuhn, T.S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions, 3rd ed. Chicago and London:
University of Chicago Press.
Kάλφας, B. (1995). Πλάτων Τίμαιος. Εκδόζεις Πόλις, Αθήνα [Kalfas, B. (1995). Plato
Timaeus. Polis Editions, Athens]
Kάλφας, B. (1999). Αριστοτέλης Περί Φύσεως. Εκδόζεις Πόλις, Αθήνα [Kalfas, B. (1999)
Aristotle On Nature: Physics‘ Book II. Polis Editions, Athens]
Lennox, J.G. (1992a). Teleology. In: Keller EF, Lloyd EA (eds), Keywords in Evolutionary
Biology. Cambridge, Massachussetts and London, England: Harvard University Press, pp.
324-333
128 Kostas Kampourakis and Vasso Zogza
Lennox, J.G. (1992b). Philosophy of Biology. In: Salmon M, Earman J, Glymour C, Lennox
J, Machamer P, McGuire J, Norton J, Salmon W, Schaffner K (eds), Introduction to the
philosophy of science. New Jersey: Prentice Hall, pp. 269-309
Lennox, J.G. (1993). Darwin was a teleologist. Biology and Philosophy, 8, 409-421
Lennox, J.G. (1994). Teleology by another name: a reply to Ghiselin. Biology and
Philosophy, 9, 493-495
Lennox, J.G. (2001). Aristotle‘s philosophy of biology: studies in the origins of life science.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lewens, T. (2004). Organisms and artifacts: design in nature and elsewhere. Cambridge
Massachusetts and London, England: MIT Press.
Lewens, T. (2007). Adaptation. In D.L. Hull and M. Ruse (Eds), Cambridge Companion to
the Philosophy of Biology, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1-21
Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. Trends in Cognitive
Science, 10(10), 464-470
Lombrozo, T., Carey, S. (2006). Functional explanation and the function of explanation.
Cognition, 99, 167–204
Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D., Zaitchik, D. (2007). Inferring design: Evidence of a preference
for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer's Disease. Psychological Science,
18(11), 999-1006
Lombrozo, T., Shtulman, A., Weisberg, M. (2006). The Intelligent Design controversy:
lessons from psychology and education. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(2), 56-57
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 131, 1501-1506
Mayr, E. (2004). What makes biology unique? : considerations on the autonomy of a
scientific discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
Moore, J.A. (1993). Science as a way of knowing: the foundations of modern biology.
Cambridge, Massachusetts and London, England: Harvard University Press
Neander, K. (1991). The teleological notion of ‗function‘. Australasian Journal of
Philosophy, 69, 454-468
Oppy, G. (1996). Hume and the argument for biological design. Biology and Philosophy, 11,
519-534
Paley, W. (2006) [1802]. Natural theology or evidence of the existence and attributes of the
Deity, collected from the appearances of nature, (Eddy MD, Knight D, eds). Oxford and
New York: Oxford University Press.
Popper, K. (1974). Darwinism as a metaphysical research programme, In: P.A. Schilpp (ed)
The philosophy of Karl Popper, volume 1, Open Court, LaSalle, IL, pp. 134-138
(reprinted in Balashov Y, Rosenberg A, 2002, pp. 302-304)
Posner, G.J., Strike, K.A., Hewson, P.W., Gertzog, W.A. (1982). Accommodation of a
scientific conception: toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211-
227
Reiss, J.O. (2005). Natural selection and the conditions for existence: representational vs.
conditional teleology in biological explanation. History and Philosophy of the Life
Sciences, 27, 249-280
Ruse, M. (2000). Teleology: yesterday, today, and tomorrow? Studies in the History and
Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 31(1), 213–232
Teleology and Evolution Education 129
Ruse, M. (2002). Evolutionary biology and teleological thinking. In: Ariew A, Cummins R,
Perlman M (eds.) Functions: new essays in the philosophy of psychology and biology.
Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 33-59
Ruse, M. (2003). Darwin and design: does evolution have a purpose? Cambridge,
Massachussetts and London, England: Harvard University Press,
Settlage, J. (1994). Conceptions of natural selection: a snapshot of the sense-making process.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching 31 (5), 449-457.
Shtulman, A. (2006). Qualitative differences between naive and scientific theories of
evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 52(2), 170-194
Sinatra, G.M., Southerland, S.A., McConaughy, F., Demastes, W. (2003). Intentions and
beliefs in students‘ understanding and acceptance of biological evolution. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 40(5), 510–528
Southerland, S. A., Abrams, E., Cummins, C. L. and Anselmo, J. (2001). Understanding.
students‘ explanations of biological phenomena: Conceptual frameworks or p-prims?
Science Education, 85, 328-348.
Taquet, P. (2006).Georges Cuvier: naissance d‘un genie. Paris: Odile Jacob
Thomson, K. (2005). Before Darwin: reconciling God and nature. New Haven and London:
Yale University Press
Wright L (1973) Functions. Philosophical Review, 82(2), 139-168
Zohar, A. and Ginossar, S. (1998). Lifting the taboo regarding teleology and
anthropomorphism in biology education-heretical suggestions. Science Education, 82,
679-697.
In: Recent Trends in Education ISBN 978-1-60741-795-8
Editor: Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas © 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Chapter 5
ABSTRACT
Writing is a complex and cognitively demanding activity. It cannot be performed as
a sequence of discrete steps; it requires the simultaneous combination of several
strategies and the application of various mental resources. Writing is, therefore, both a
recursive and a dynamic process. To be successful, writers need an understanding of the
components of a quality text as well as knowledge of writing strategies that can be used
to shape and organize the writing process. In particular, writing competence requires
appropriate and self-regulated knowledge of strategies for planning what to write, and
then revising what has been written.
In this chapter, we first present a review of the recent research on the planning and
revision processes in writing in order to show the importance that these have in the
development of writing competence. Then, we describe the existing research, evaluating
strategy-focused intervention studies, to provide an overview of the nature of the
interventions programs and an indication of which have been most successful. In the
second part of the chapter, we describe and summarize findings from our own studies
(Torrance, Fidalgo, and García, 2007; and Fidalgo, Torrance, and García, 2008). These
studies move beyond existing research by (a) exploring the effects of strategy focused
instruction on students‘ writing processes as well as on their written products and (b)
demonstrating the long-term effects of this kind of intervention. In a final section, we
discuss the practical implications of this body of research (both ours and others) and
*
Correspondence should be directed to Raquel Fidalgo e-mail: [email protected]
†
Or to Mark Torrance: [email protected].
132 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundin, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
make suggestions for how lessons learned from this research might be applied in the
classroom.
INTRODUCTION
The impact of the cognitive approach to writing has been remarkable. According to this
approach writing is understood as much more than just a motor activity. It demands reflective
thinking processes to be used when planning text, whilst drafting, and when the text is revised
process (Tynjälä, Mason, and Lonka, 2001). Students tend to have difficulty in implementing
these writing processes and this problem persists in teaching writing at school.
Contemporary research is focused on viewing writing as a process rather than as a
product (Boscolo, 1995). In general, the focus of investigation is on the processes that shape
writing, thus the writer must pay attention to the generation of ideas and when revising the
text. Writing is a complex task that requires the coordination of several mental activities.
These do not necessarily occur in a set sequence. Rather, writers simultaneously and
dynamically combine various strategies and resources. Writing is also a communicative act
and a social event between the writer and audience. The writer has to clearly establish the
communicative intention, the goal and the type of text (Beal, 1996; McCutchen, 2006). The
writer also has to consider the audience, in terms of the reader‘s characteristics and
expectations to determine what they can or must write (Boscolo and Ascorti, 2004).
Moreover, the writer has to master the topic that they are writing about as well as generating
relevant ideas that will be expressed in the text (McCutchen, Francis, and Kerr, 1997; Perez,
2001). The writer must also control the coherence of the entire text by clarifying the message,
reorganising and/or modifying ideas. Throughout the writing process it is necessary to
translate from ideas into words. This activity does not mean writing isolated words or
sentences; they must be organized so that they cohere. It is rare that the first draft that a writer
completes successfully communicates their message. It is necessary to create one or more
versions. The writer makes both surface modifications, such as orthographic and grammatical
corrections, as well as modifications at a deeper level, such as the reorganisation of the text
over the various drafts (Alamargot and Chanquoy, 2001, p. 1).
The seminal and original cognitive model of writing processes proposed by Hayes and
Flower (1980) describes three processes used in writing: planning, translating, and revising.
These are retained but reconceptualized more recent papers (Hayes, 1996, 2004). Planning is
a reflective process by which content for the text is generated and organised. It combines low-
level memory retrieval processes with more strategic problem solving, decision-making and
inferencing. Translation is the process by which planned content is transformed into full,
coherent text. The revision process is again reflective, involving reading, interpretation, and
editing of the text-produced-so-far. Planning, translating and revising are under executive
control and are driven by specific task schemas (Hayes, 2004). Arguably, a core component
of writing expertise development is the learning of appropriate schemas.
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 133
Planning
Revision
Like planning, revision is a high level composite process that weighs heavily on the
writer‘s limited attentional capacities (Beal, 1996; McCutchen, 1996). A distinction is often
made between effective revision on paper and mental revision – rehearsal of content before it
is committed to paper. Alamargot and Chanquoy (2001, p. 100) proposed the following
understanding of revision: Revision occurs when something (e.g., a word) is altered (added,
deleted, substituted) to reach a certain goal (improving style, developing content). This
change can be made to text that had already been transcribed, or to text in the head and has a
particular effect (either enhancing or detracting from the quality of the text), and a particular
cognitive cost.
Hayes (1996, 2004) argued that revision involves a combination of critical reading,
problem solving and text production and that these are coordinated by an overall task schema.
Accordingly they provided evidence supporting the existence of a revision task schema.
Generally, novice writers seem to operate under a revision schema which dedicates most
effort to revision of the surface aspects of the text at the expense of the changes to message or
134 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundin, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
rhetoric, although there is sometimes a shift to deeper level revision in older writers
(Butterfield, Hacker and Plum, 1996). McCutchen et al. (1997) argued that the development
of deeper level revision is related to the writers‘ ability to read texts critically. Wallace,
Hayes, Hatch, Miller, Moser and Silk (1996) report an intervention that improves writers‘
revision by helping them to develop a revision schema that directs attention to the deep rather
than to the surface aspects of texts. Similarly, Beal, Garrod, and Bonitatibus (1990) found that
young writers revised more effectively when instructed to monitor their text comprehension
during revision.
When it is necessary to solve a problem within an existing text, the writer must recognize
the problem and then take appropriate steps to correct it. Such problem-solving involves
comparing a representation of the actual text to a representation of the intended text. Several
processes in this sequence can be problematic for novice writers (McCutchen, 2006). First,
novice writers are less likely to engage in much planning and their memory representations of
the intended text are often vague. Second, writers can have difficulty in forming an accurate
representation of the text that they have already written and this can inhibit their ability to
revise effectively. Novice writers have difficulty distinguishing between information that is
presented directly in the text, or might reasonably be inferred by the text‘s intended reader,
and the background knowledge that they themselves bring to their re-reading. They therefore
miss errors in their own texts that they readily see in texts written by others. One effective
strategy for helping students in this respect is to introduce peer interaction into the revision
process (Boscolo and Ascorti, 2004). Third, writers may have difficulty in generating
alternative language to correct a problem even though they have detected it. Fourth, revision
processes are likely to place substantial demands on cognitive capacity. This will be
particularly the case with younger writers for whom low level writing and reading processes
more demanding, and who are likely to have poorer strategies for managing cognitive load
(Piolat, Rosoussey, Olive, and Amada, 2004). As a result of these issues, novice writers
encounter more difficulties than expert writers in diagnosing and correcting text problems.
If the goal of writing instruction is to help novice writers to develop the skills and
strategies that are associated with writing expertise, then it is helpful first to define writing
expertise. Boscolo (1995) suggested that an expert writer is a thoughtful planner, a coherent
organizer, a careful reviser, and an audience-sensitive message-sender. In order to reach such
a level of competence it is necessary to develop writing strategies that make novice writers
become sensitive to these different aspects of writing. Such strategies would address each
writing process and sub-process, from the planning through to the revision stage (Alamargot
and Chanquoy, 2001).
There has been recent growth in the number of studies evaluating the effects of different
methods of writing instruction (Pritchard and Honeycutt, 2006). Whereas research in the
1970s and 1980s tended to concentrate on how the components of the writing process are
related to writer and text variables, there has been a more recent focus on research specifically
designed to measure improvements in the quality of the written product as a result of using
different teaching strategies. There has been a particular focus on the effects of teaching
particular cognitive strategies for managing the writing process. Learners are seen as being
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 135
actively engaged in all aspects of writing: planning, composing, and revising the multiple
drafts of their text (Allal, 2004).
A number of studies have demonstrated benefits for teaching planning and revision
strategies (e.g., Bryson and Scardamalia, 1996; Danoff, Harris, and Graham, 1993; de la Paz,
2005; de la Paz and Graham, 1997, 2002; Englert, et al., 1991; Simons, et al., 1994; Yeh,
1998). A recent meta-analysis of studies evaluating a broad range of different forms of
writing instruction identified strategy-focused interventions as the most effective, with a large
mean effect size (Graham and Perin, 2007). These include studies of both learning disabled
and typically-developing writers of ages ranging from 4th to 10th grade. Studies have explored
writing in both expository and narrative genres, and several have demonstrated that training in
one strategy generalizes to performance in another. For example Bryson and Scardamalia
gave typically-developing 10th-grade writers instruction in both planning and revision
strategies. They found that on a persuasive writing task students who had experienced the
intervention produced better argued, more reflective, and better quality texts, but found no
difference in spelling errors or text length.
Several studies have explored teaching revision strategies independently of planning
(e.g., Arias-Gundín and García, 2006; Cameron, Edmund, Wigmore, Hunt, and Linton, 1997;
Fitzgerald and Markham, 1987; García and Arias-Gundín, 2004), and with appropriate
teaching methods this can have a positive effect on the quality of students‘ texts. Effective
instruction appears to require some sort of scaffolding for the revision process by, for
example, providing learners with revision guides (Chanquoy, 1997) and that students are
given feedback on the effectiveness of their revisions (Matsumura, Petthey-Chavez, Valdes,
and Garnier, 2002; Perez, 2001). Sharing revision activities with peers seems particularly
effective (Boscolo and Ascorti, 2004; McCutchen, Francis, and Kerr, 1997; Rouiller, 2004).
In the remainder of this chapter we first describe a strategy focused intervention that we
call Cognitive Self-regulation Instruction (CSRI) aimed at developing the quality of writers‘
texts by developing effective strategic writing processes. We then summarize an evaluation of
this approach (García and Fidalgo, 2006; Torrance, Fidalgo, and García, 2007 and Fidalgo,
Torrance, and García, 2008). Finally, we draw some general conclusions for teaching
practice.
writer has the ability and motivation to spontaneously apply appropriate strategies to their
own writing. They also all emphasize dialogue between teacher and students, recognize the
social origins of self-regulation, and promote the use of self-dialogue to regulate students‘
own behavior.
These principles are brought together in an instructional approach that emphasizes
teacher modeling and student emulation. Students observe the teacher while he or she models
the particular strategy to be taught – planning an expository text, for example – and then
emulates what they have seen. The intention is that for each strategy students move through a
sequence of observation, then emulation, then self-control and finally independent self-
regulation (Schunk and Zimmerman, 1997; Zimmerman, 2000). Students‘ learning is
scaffolded by the use of a range of support materials. These include mnemonics to aid
memory about, for example, the different functions of planning, and blank tables or grids
which students can used to structure their thinking, and procedural prompts. These can
steadily be removed over the course of an instructional program as students develop fluency
and independence in the writing strategies that are being taught.
The instructional pattern of the CSRI model consists of four sequential instructional
stages. Through this instructional sequence students are guided towards a self-regulated
competence through the guidance provided by materials or the teacher. This scaffolding is
faded out through the instruction program. The first instructional stage aims at developing in
students a mental framework and terminology through which to understand subsequent
process-focused instruction. The aim is to broaden students attention to include not just
mechanical aspects of writing (spelling, sentence structure, and so forth) but substantive
features of both the text (the need for well structured and audience-focused content) and
process (the importance of planning and revision). In CSRI students are taught about
generation of ideas, planning, or organization of ideas, thinking about audience, revising, and
other processes, drawing heavily on established cognitive accounts of wiring (e.g., Hayes and
Flower, 1980). In this way students develop specialized cognitive schemas for understanding
the kinds of planning, translating and revising skills and sub-processes that are characteristic
of more competent writers. The intention is that this framework will allow students to manage
and regulate the complex higher level set of cognitive processes associated with the planning
or the revision processes taught in later stages.
Knowledge about planning is taught using the mnemonics ―POD‖ and ―OAIUE‖ (or just
―the vowels‖). POD is drawn from previously evaluated interventions (e.g., Mason, Harris,
and Graham, 2002) and represents three steps in the development and use of writing plans:
Picking ideas - generating ideas related to the theme of the text, Organizing ideas, and
Developing text. Students are encouraged to plan in advance of writing full text and to make
use of these plans while writing, but also to be open to the possibility of further developing
these plans once writing has started. The Vowels expand the Organizing component of POD.
O (Objetivo) encourages students to develop high level goals for their text. A (Audiencia)
indicates the need to identify and accommodate reader needs. I (Ideas) points to the need to
deliberately generate and list possible ideas for the text. U (Unir ideas) stands for ―link ideas‖
and encourages students to explicitly think about and decide how these ideas will be
organized. Finally E (Esquema / Schema) encourages students to think about standard
structures and genre conventions for the kind of text that they were producing (García and
Fidalgo, 2006).
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 137
Revision is support by the mnemonic LEA (RED in English), which highlights the three
main steps of the revision process. Students Read (Leer) the text several times and in different
ways. First, they must read the text closely paying attention to the structure, paragraphs, and
inter-paragraphs links. Secondly, they must quickly reread their text paying attention to lower
level features (spelling, punctuation, sentence syntax). Students are encouraged to Evaluate
(Evaluar) what they read and, lastly, to make (Actuar, Do) the necessary changes.
The second instructional stage involves modeling the use of mature planning and
revising strategies. During this stage the teacher or instructor thinks aloud in front of the
students while planning, drafting, and revising text. This demonstrate how the framework and
mnemonics introduced in first stage can be used to regulate writing processes. Think aloud is
partly spontaneous and partly pre-prepared. The teacher asks themselves questions like ―what
should I do before the writing task?‖, ―What do should I do first…, second…, later?, ―what
should I do during the drafting phase?‖ and so forth. In each case the teacher would then
provide a response based round the mnemonics introduced above, and then relate these to the
particular writing task in which they are engaged. Also included in the self-regulation
processes are the questions about self-efficacy, attributions and motivation.
After the modeling, the students and the teacher collectively analyze and reflect on the
most important actions of the writing process, the necessary steps in writing and their
importance. Finally, they should summarize the most relevant conclusions about the writing
process performed and the self-regulation strategies and procedures that were used and their
importance in achieving good quality text. In the context of writing instruction, as is the case
for teaching other procedural skills, modeling and observation has been shown to be more
effective than direct instruction (e.g., Couzijn and Rijlaarsdam, 1996). Students who learn by
observation ―step back‖ from the writing task and can focus on the learning task, creating a
learning opportunity to broaden their knowledge about writing (Braaksma, van den Bergh,
Rijlaarsdam and Couzijn, 2001; Braaksma et al., 2004; Couzijn, 1999).
The teacher engages offers two forms of this metacognitive modeling: a coping model
and a mastery model. Coping modeling involves the teacher occasionally making errors or
adopting sub-optimal strategies, but them immediately correcting these mistakes. Mastery
modeling involves writing without explicitly making and correcting mistakes. Some
researchers have shown that a coping model is more effective than a mastery model
(Kitsantas, Zimmerman, and Clearly, 2000; Zimmerman and Kitsantas, 2002). Arguably,
though, mastery models are also valuable as they serve to provide a benchmark against which
students can evaluate their own performance.
In the third instructional stage students learn to emulate the strategies that were modeled
in the previous stage. Like the teacher that they were observing, the students thought aloud.
Thinking aloud is an excellent technique for developing metacognitive knowledge and self-
regulation strategies. In general, it helps students develop greater control and awareness about
the cognitive writing processes and the self-regulation skills in writing. It helps them to guide
their thoughts during the writing process. It increases their self-control as writers and can also
improve their writing performance. Students think more precisely, carefully and
systematically when they are thinking aloud. Besides, the teacher or peers can identify and
diagnose any misunderstood or misused concepts, rules, facts, important omissions and
inadequate or incomplete knowledge, approaches or skills in writing if students are thinking
aloud (Hartman, 2001). Students are first trained to think aloud. Later, working in pairs, the
first student models the specific writing process while the second student carefully and
138 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundin, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
analytically assesses the student‘s writing processes and strategies. The second student must
have an active role, analyze the strengths and weaknesses of the writing process that they are
observing, and provide guidance when necessary. The students then swap roles. Working in
pairs in this way, in addition to observing modeling by the teacher, is particularly helpful
because observers are more likely to take on the behaviors they are observing when they
perceive similarity between the person modeling and themselves (Bandura, 1986). Of course
when the model is a peer there is no guarantee that the modeled behavior will be effective.
However safeguards are provided both by telling observers that they should be critical, and by
close monitoring from the teacher.
When the writing process is finished, all the students along with the teacher, initially in
pairs and later in a big group, analyze and reflect on the most important actions of the writing
process, their need for the self-regulation strategies and procedures that had been used and
their importance in achieving a quality text.
Finally, in the fourth instructional stage students work individually, again thinking aloud,
observed by the teacher. In this stage all procedural facilitations are eliminated although
teachers provide feedback about their performance. During this stage, students should begin
to shift their attention from the modeled processes to the performance outcomes (Zimmerman
and Kitsantas, 2002). The intention is that they also move from thinking of the strategies as
the teachers, and therefore externally imposed, to being their own strategies and adapted to
their own personal needs. Progressively, the students will learn to apply strategies flexibly to
different writing tasks. This process of personalization is aided by asking students to make
their own list of writing strategies that they feel that they can use in their future writing.
Evaluation of the CSRI approach to teaching writing suggest that it is capable of
delivering substantial and enduring improvements in the writing competence of typically-
developing sixth-grade students. This research is described in detail in Torrance, Fidalgo, and
García, 2007 and Fidalgo, Torrance, and García, 2008, and is summarized next.
Our study sampled 95 Spanish sixth-grade primary school students (39 girls and 56 boys)
with a mean age of 11 years and 7 months. These students were taken from four different
classes. Three of these classes were within the same school (N = 22, 24 and 25, respectively)
these students received the CSRI instructional program. The fourth class attended a different
but similar school. This class formed the control group (N= 24). Writing performance in these
students was assessed prior to intervention, immediately post-intervention, and twelve weeks
after intervention (or at equivalent times in the control). We also tested 56 of the original
intervention sample two years after they had completed CSRI and returned to the normal
literacy curriculum (20 girls and 36 boys with mean age of 14 years and 1 month). These
were compared with a normal-curriculum control group formed of 21 students of the same
grade (7 girls and 14 boys) with a mean age of 14 years and 4 months, and who had not
participated in the CSRI intervention but who had for the previous two years been studying at
the same school and attended the same classes as the intervention sample. This control group
had very similar curriculum performance and Standardized Ability Test scores to the
intervention sample.
Writing performance was assessed with tasks that involved students composing short
expository essays about topics related to previous curriculum content whilst logging their
writing activities at frequent, random intervals. At baseline and post-test, all the participants
wrote essays in the same genre, using the compare-contrast genre which had been trained in
CSRI condition. To determine whether the effects of CSRI generalized to other kinds of
expository task at delayed post-test (12 weeks after the intervention) the three different
classes that comprised the CSRI group completed tasks in different genres. Class A
completed a compare-contrast task (the same task as the ordinary curriculum group), Class B
completed a task that involved expression of an opinion, and Class C completed a task
involving the description of a causal relationship. At long-term follow up (two years after the
intervention) both groups wrote compare-contrast essays. In each case, the themes of the
writing tasks were supported by additional topic-related material.
The instructional program developed with the experimental groups was based on the
CSRI model described previously. According to this model, the instructional program
followed the four general stages of training, over 10 weekly sessions lasting between 60 and
75 minutes each. Table 1 summarizes this instructional program. It includes the instructional
stage, the number of sessions, the contents, strategies, techniques, and supportive materials.
The program was delivered by the students‘ normal literacy teacher, who received specific
training from the researchers.
140 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundin, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
O = Object
A = Audience
I = Ideas
U = Unite ideas
E = Draft Essay
O = Object
A = Audience
I = Ideas
U = Unite ideas
E = draft Essay
6th Knowledge of the revision Direct and explicit Writing process matrix
process instruction Matrix of the revision process
Mechanical and Previous knowledge Organizer
Substantive revision Interactive explanation Mnemonic chart RED revising
Revising strategy: RED Functional examples of strategy
R = Read text mechanical and Matrix of the type of revisions:
E = Evaluate text substantive revision mechanical vs substantial
D = Do necessary changes
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 141
Table 1. (Continued)
Table 1. (Continued)
Assessment
These activities were labeled and defined as follows: reading references (I am reading
the reference materials), thinking about content (I am thinking of things to say in my text);
writing outline (I am writing a plan of what I‘m going to write in the text or I am using my
notes to make a detailed outline); writing text (I am drafting full text rather than making
notes); reading text (I am reading though all or part of my text); changing text (I am making
changes to my text, for example correcting orthographic mistakes, changing words, adding
144 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias-Gundin, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
words, eliminating words, etc.); unrelated (I am doing or thinking something unrelated to the
text, for example: looking for a pen, looking through the window, etc.).
Measure Criteria
Structure The rater considered the presence and development of six characteristics:
Background information to present the text.
Structural cues
An introduction: a topic or thesis sentence to establish the general comparison-
contrast.
Clearly developed organization either whole by whole or part by part or likeness-
differences.
Unity within individual paragraphs and in the case of a theme within the entire
paper.
A conclusion which reiterates the purpose of the paper, to show comparisons or
contrasts or both
Coherence The rater considered the presence and development of seven characteristics:
Topic or theme identified
Topic or theme extended without digressions
A context which oriented the reader
Details which were organized in a discernible plan which was sustained
throughout the text.
Cohesive ties linking sentences and / or paragraphs
Discourse which flowed smoothly
Conclusion statement creating a sense of closure.
Quality The rater considered the presence and development of seven characteristics:
Clear sequence of ideas
Text development with little or no irrelevant ideas.
Good organization
Fresh, vigorous word choice.
Variety of interesting details
Correct sentence structure
Correct punctuation, capitalization and spelling.
Time spent in each of seven activities through writing process was estimated by
multiplying the number of times that a participant indicated a particular activity in their
writing log by the mean inter-beep interval.
These activities were collected in a blank writing log divided into multiple sections each
listing the seven possible writing activities. Each activity was indicated with a specific
symbol or picture which, after training, became familiar to the students. This helped to
minimize the extent to which completing the log diverted attention from the writing task.
Students were trained in using this method prior to completing the baseline assessment. We
then determined the students‘ accuracy in using the categorization scheme by indicating the
activities of a writer at 25 different examples of activities during their writing process. The
comparison of the students‘ categorization with that of an expert judge showed good
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 145
agreement (kappa = .87 for both the initial study when assessed again for the long-term
follow up).
Results
We first describe effects of CSRI on the students‘ texts, and then on writing processes. In
both cases we made separate comparisons for baseline vs. post-test (for immediate effects of
CSRI) and baseline vs. delayed post-test (for more persistent effects). If CSRI is effective we
would expect to find increases in quality and use of planning and revision that were greater in
the intervention sample than in the control. If this difference is found and has not occurred by
chance then trial (baseline vs. post-test or baseline vs. delayed post-test) by group
(intervention vs. control) interactions should, therefore, be statistically significant.
As Table 4 indicates, there were statistically significant improvements in the quality of
texts produced by students in the intervention group compared with controls, and this
improvement was sustained in the delayed-post-test assessment. We also found that after
attending CSRI students were more likely to use anaphoric, structural, and meta-structural
devices for making their text cohere. Arguably this demonstrates an increased tendency to
attend to reader needs rather than just expressing ideas as they occur.
CSRI also appeared to have a substantial effect on the extent to which participants made
use of introductory and concluding paragraphs. At post-test only 2 (8%) of the ordinary
curriculum group included introductions in their text, compared with 67 (94%) of CSRI
participants. Similarly, only one student in the ordinary curriculum group wrote a concluding
paragraph, whilst 61 (86%) of the CSRI students did so. This pattern was repeated at delayed
post-test. Of the 22 CSRI participants that performed the compare-contrast task, 21 (95%)
wrote introductions and 20 (91%) wrote concluding paragraphs, compared with two and one
students, respectively, in the ordinary curriculum condition. This outcome was generalized to
the opinion and cause-and-effect tasks with, 21 (87%) and 25 (100%) of students,
respectively, writing introductory paragraphs, and 19 (79%) and 24 (96%) writing
conclusions. There was also a substantial increase in the use of paragraphing by the CSRI
group at post- and delayed-post tests, effects that were absent in ordinary curriculum group.
Although the intervention focused exclusively on writing compare-contrast essays, the
effects generalized well to the opinion and cause-effect tasks that were completed by sub-
samples of the intervention group at delayed post test.
Two years after CSRI instruction most of these effects were maintained, albeit with rather
more modest effect sizes (Table 5). CSRI students produced better quality text, with more
sophisticated coherence ties, compared with controls. They were also more likely to include
introductory paragraphs (but not conclusions).
Table 4. Comparison on reader- and text-based quality measures taken at baseline, post-test and delayed post-test (12 week)
for CSRI and control students
Baseline Post-test 12-weeks Baseline vs. post-test Baseline vs. delayed post-test
Control CSRI Control CSRI Control CSRI F (1, 93) p η2 F (1,44) p η2
Reader-based measures
Quality 2.46 (.55) 2.4 (.60) 2.21 (.25) 5.29 (1.06) 2.17 (.50) 4.91 (1.11) 214.4 .001 .70 94.8 .001 .68
Coherence 2.4 (.47) 2.25 (.5) 2.31 (.44) 3.82 (.53) 2.52 (.45) 3.89 (.31) 146.9 .001 .61 94.4 .001 .68
Structure 1.94 (.76) 1.76 (.53) 1.71 (.66) 3.73 (.71) 1.94 (.61) 3.84 (.36) 158.7 .001 .63 91.3 .001 .67
Text-based measures
Word count 83.2 77.4 (22.5) 84.4 (42.0) 92.9 (26.4) 94.3 (31.2) 106 (23.5) 7.16 .01 .14
(29.4)
Paragraph count 1.4 (.6) 1.7 (1) 1.6 (.9) 3.6 (1.1) 1.8 (.5) 3.8 (.4) 26 .001 .22 44.9 .001
Anaphoric ties 1.51 1.61 (2.59) 1.44 (1.66) 3.9 (2.48) 2.39 (2.09) 3.98 (2.47) 12.2 .001 .12 7.3 .01 .14
(1.24)
Reformulation ties 0 .09 (.62) .10 (.34) 1.06 (.60) .30 (.97) 1.29 (.51) 22.4 .001 .19 4.3 .04 .09
Structural ties .90 (2.02) .37 (1.66) .40 (1.97) 1.51 (1.23) .32 (.52) 1.52 (.89) 9.1 .003 .09 6.8 .012 .13
Meta-structural ties .05 (.27) 0 0 1.1 (.55) .20 (.58) .94 (.33) 97.3 .001 .51 26.8 .001 .38
Note: Results are given for just those effects that were statistically significant. Means at 12 weeks are just for students who completed the compare-
contrast essay (n = 24). F ratios are reported for test by group interactions.
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 147
CSRI had a rather more mixed effect on students writing strategies. There was a much
greater tendency, relative to controls, for CSRI students to plan their texts. However, we found
no effect of the intervention on students‘ tendency to read and make changes to their text.
Revision was more or less absent for all writing tasks in both groups. As for the long term
follow up, the analysis of the differences between the CSRI and the control group concerning
the time spent on the different writing processes only showed a statistically significant higher
time spent on writing the outline process in the CSRI than the control group. The explicit
planning activity, in the form of outline-writing, was greater for the CSRI group compared to
the control group. Again, however, there was no evidence that CSRI students were more likely
to engage in revision.
CSRI does, therefore, appear to offer substantial benefits for sixth grade writers. Students
who experienced CSRI produced better quality text, even two years after intervention
Specifically, the texts of CSRI participants suggested greater consideration of audience and of
communicative and pragmatic goals. In the terms of Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987) this
finding suggests a shift in the students towards a more knowledge transforming approach to
writing. This effect was accompanied by, and perhaps resulted from, a substantially increased
tendency for students to systematically plan their texts.
These findings are consistent with previous research. As we discussed above, recent
systematic literature reviews focusing on instructional practices for teaching writing, indicate
that strategy-focused instruction has a strong impact on the quality of students‘ written
products (Graham, 2006; Graham and Harris, 2003; Graham and Perin, 2007). In particular,
the Self-Regulated Strategy Development instructional model (SRSD) has been proven
effective and yielded a large average effect size. This model shares key instructional
techniques and strategies with the CSRI approach to teaching writing. A feature of our
evaluation that is absent in the research reviewed by Graham and co-workers was the long-
term follow-up. Our findings suggested that the benefits of the CSRI were still present nearly
two years after the intervention was delivered. CSRI students continue to produce significantly
better quality texts and tended to spend more time planning their texts than their peers who
followed the normal curriculum in writing instruction. This suggests that CSRI, and by
implication other strategy-focused interventions, promote a more strategic and self-regulated
approach to writing, especially in relation to planning processes, which is enduring, and does
148 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
not developed spontaneously under more traditional writing instruction, at least until after the
eighth grade.
is a particular effective approach to modeling. However in practice we have found that ―think-
aloud‖ needs itself to be supported. Teachers need to rehearse particular self-questioning and
self-instructing devices in advance of modeling in front of the class. From the students‘
perspective, therefore, that teacher – as the expert model – is simply doing what she would
normally do when composing a text, and we think that this is important. However, to
maintaining this illusion requires that the teacher does some advance preparation. The CSRI
intervention made use of think-aloud not just when initially demonstrating strategies to
students but during subsequent emulation, both with when students worked in pairs and even
when they finally wrote alone. This both helps to make strategies salient and explicit, and
means that teachers and other students have at least some access to, and are therefore able to
comment on, the internal mental processes that are the focus of the intervention.
Self-regulation in writing can also be promoted by means of specific techniques, such as,
mnemonic devices for the cognitive strategies of writing, cognitive modeling and emulation
and the thinking aloud technique. The cognitive strategies developed in this way allow
students to manage and regulate the complex higher-level set of cognitive processes associated
with planning, drafting and revision. Teachers are likely to find that students require
considerable support – both through shared writing activities and supporting written materials.
This needs to be removed gradually in order to promote autonomous, self-regulatory use of the
strategy being taught. Our evaluation of CSRI suggests that gradual removal of scaffolding in
this way results in most students spontaneously adopting pre-planning strategies, even two
years after instruction with no subsequent reinforcement. However the same was not true for
revision strategies. Students appeared to understand the function and possible benefits of
revision, and could emulate teacher modeling, but they did not then spontaneously revise their
own texts. This may be for motivational reasons. Whereas deliberate planning necessarily
occurs primarily before drafting has starting, particularly when the writing task is short,
revision is predominantly a post-drafting activity. Motivation is generally likely to be lower at
this point. Students are also likely to feel that if they have carefully planned their text, then
followed this plan when writing, there is little to be gained from then reading through what
they have written. Unless goals change during writing, which for students completing
relatively short, teacher-provided tasks, is unlikely, or there is reader input that reveals that
they have not been as successful in communicating their ideas as they though they were,
students are unlikely to perceive any need to change their text. Developing an inner-critic –
internal processes that allow writers to distance themselves from their text and see it from the
perspective of intended readers – may be something that is beyond young writers.
In conclusion, therefore, strategy focused writing instruction in general, and the specific
methods used to implement this in the CSRI intervention in particular, appear to be effective in
helping students at the top end of elementary school develop into more mature writers.
Specifically this approach appears more effective, or at least to add substantial value to,
traditional approaches to writing instruction that focus solely on characteristics of the written
product. The very large effect sizes found for immediate and short-term effects of CSRI in our
evaluation (compared to more modest average effect sizes found by Graham and Perin, 2007)
can in part be explained by the almost total absence of process oriented teaching in the Spanish
literacy curriculum. In contexts where normal writing instruction is already more processes
focused, as is increasingly the case in the UK, for example, effects may be less marked.
However, we believe that the range of methods adopted by CSRI, and perhaps particularly the
150 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
extensive use of think-aloud-based modeling and emulation, are have the potential to make a
useful contribution to writing instruction across a broad range of contexts.
NOTE
During this research, we received competitive funds from Ministerio de Investigación,
Ciencia e Innovación --MICINN: Ministry of Research, Science and Innovation- from the
Spanish Government, project SEJ2007-66898-EDUC (2007-2010), with European Union
FEDER funds. And Excellence Research Group funds from Junta de Castilla y León (GR259),
and with European non FEDER funds for 2009-2010-2011 (BOCyL 27 on April 2009). Both
awarded to the third author.
REFERENCES
Alamargot, D., and Chanquoy, L. (2001). Through the models of writing. Dordrecht: Kluwer
Academic Press.
Alamargot, D., Favart, M., Coirier, P., Passerault, J. M., and Andriessen, J. (1999).
Argumentative text: planning and translating. In M. F. Crété, and E. Espéret (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 1998 European Writing Conference- Writing and learning to write at
the dawn of the 21st century (pp. 149-156). Poitiers: Editions du C. N. R. S.
Allal, L. (2004). Integrated writing instruction and the development of revision skills. In L.
Allal, L. Chanquoy, and P. Largy (Eds.), Revision cognitive and instructional processes
(pp. 139-155). New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Arias-Gundín, O., and García, J. N. (2006). Instrucción en el proceso de revisión textual
mediante el procedimiento CDO en estudiantes que presentan bajo rendimiento
[Instruction in textual revision process by the CDO procedure in students with low
achievement]. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 32 (145), 693-713.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Beal, C. R. (1996). The role of comprehension monitoring in children‘s revision. Educational
Psychology Review, 8 (3), 219-238.
Beal, C. R., Garrod, A. C., and Bonitatibus, G. J. (1990). Fostering children‘s revision skills
through training in comprehension monitoring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82,
275-280.
Bereiter, C., and Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale,
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Boscolo, P. (1995). The cognitive approach to writing and writing instruction: A contribution
to a critical appraisal. Current Psychology of Cognition, 14, 343-336.
Boscolo, P., and Ascorti, A. (2004). Effects of collaborative revision on children‘s ability to
write understandable narrative texts. in L. Allal, L. Chanquoy, and P. Largy (Eds.),
Revision cognitive and instructional processes (pp. 157-170). New York: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
The Cognitive Self-Regulation Approach to Pre-Secondary Writing Instruction 151
Braaksma, M. A. H., Bergh, H. van den, Rijlaarsdam, G. and Couzijn, M. (2001). Effective
learning activities in observation tasks when learning to write and read argumentative
texts. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 1, 33-48.
Braaksma, M. A. H., Rijlaarsdam, G., van den Bergh, H. and van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A.
(2004). Observational learning and its effects on the orchestration of writing processes.
Cognition and Instruction, 22(1), 1-36.
Bryson, M., and Scardamalia, M. (1996). Fostering reflectivity in the argumentative thinking
of students with different learning histories. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming
Learning Difficulties, 12, 351-384.
Butterfield, E. C., Hacker, D. J., and Plumb, C. (1996). Topic knowledge, linguistic
knowledge, and revision skill as determinants of text revision. In E. C. Butterfield (Ed.),
Advances in cognition and educational practice: Vol. 2. Children‘s writing: Toward a
process theory of the development of skilled writing (pp. 83-141). Greenwich, C. T.: JAI
Press.
Cameron, C. A., and Moshenko, B. (1996). Elicitation of knowledge transformational reports
while children write narratives. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 28, 271-280.
Cameron, C. A., Edmunds, G., Wigmore, B., Hunt, A. K., and Linton, M. J. (1997). Children‘s
revision of textual flaws. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20 (4), 667-
680.
Chanquoy, L. (1997). Thinking skills and composing: Examples of text revision. In J. H. M.
Hamers, and M. Overtoom (Eds.), Inventory of European Programmes for teaching
thinking (pp. 179-185). Utrecht: Sardes.
Couzijn, M. (1999). Learning to write by observation of writing and reading processes: Effects
on learning and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 9, 109–142.
Couzijn, M., and Rijlaarsdam, G. (1996). Learning to write by reader observation and written
feedback. In G. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Bergh, and M. Couzijn (Eds.), Effective teaching
and learning of writing. Current trends in research (pp. 224-252). Amsterdam:
Amsterdam University Press.
Danoff, B., Harris, K. R., and Graham, S. (1993). Incorporating strategy instruction within the
writing process in the regular classroom: Effects on the writing of students with and
without learning disabilities. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 2295-319.
de la Paz, S. (2005). Teaching historical reasoning and argumentative writing in culturally and
academically diverse middle school classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
139-158.
de la Paz, S., and Graham, S. (1997). Effects of dictation and advanced planning instruction on
the composing of students with writing and learning problems. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 89, 203-222.
de la Paz, S., and Graham, S. (2002). Explicitly teaching strategies, skills, and knowledge:
Writing instruction in middle school classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94,
291-304.
Englert, C. S., Raphael, T. E., and Anderson, L. (1992). Socially mediated instruction:
Improving students‘ knowledge and talk about writing. The Elementary School Journal,
92(4), 411-449.
Englert, C. S., Raphael, T. E., Anderson, L. M., Anthony, H. M., Stevens, D. D., and Fear, K.
L. (1991). Making writing strategies and self-talk visible: Cognitive strategy instruction in
152 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
Mason, L. H., Harris, K. R. and Graham, S. (2002). Every child has a story to tell: self-
regulated strategy development for story writing. Education and Treatment of Children,
25 (4), 496-506.
Matsumura, L. C., Patthey-Chavez, G. G., Valdés, R., and Garnier, H. (2002). Teacher
feedback, writing assignment quality and third grade students‘ revision in lower and
higher achieving urban schools. The Elementary School Journal, 103 (1), 3-25.
McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition.
Educational Psychology Review, 8, 299-325.
McCutchen, D. (2006). Cognitive factors in the development of children‘s writing. In C. A.
MacArthur, S. Graham, and J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 115-
130). New York: The Guilford Press.
McCutchen, D., Francis, M., and Kerr, S. (1997). Revising for meaning effects of knowledge
and strategy. Journal of educational psychology, 89 (4), 667-676.
Perez, S. A. (2001). Revising During Writing in a Second Grade Classroom. Educational
Research Quarterly, 25 (1), 27-32.
Piolat, A., Roussey, J. Y., Olive, T., and Amada, M. (2004). Processing time and cognitive
effort in revision: Effects of error type and of working memory capacity. In L. Allal, L.
Chanquoy, and P. Largy (Eds.), Revision cognitive and instructional processes (pp. 21-
38). New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Pritchard, R. J., and Honeycutt, R. L. (2006). The process approach to writing instruction:
examining its effectiveness. In Ch. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, and J. Fitzgerald (Eds.),
Handbook of writing research (pp. 275-290). New York: The Guilford Press.
Rouiller, Y. (2004). Collaborative revision and metacognitive reflection in a situation of
narrative text production. In L. Allal, L. Chanquoy, and P. Largy (Eds.), Revision
cognitive and instructional processes (pp. 171-188). New York: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Sanders, T. J. M., Spooren, W. P. M., and Noordman, L. G. M. (1992). Toward a Taxonomy of
Coherence Relations. Discourse Processes, 15(1), 1-35.
Schraw, G. (2001). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. In H. J. Hartman (Ed.),
Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory, research and practice (pp. 3-16). New
York: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Schunk, D. H., and Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). Social origins of self regulatory competence.
Educational Psychologist, 32, 195-208.
Simmons, D., Kame‘enui, E., Dickson, D., Chard, D., Gunn, B., and Baker, S. (1994).
Integrating narrative reading comprehension and writing instruction for all learners. In C.
Kinzer and D. Leu (Eds.), Multidimensional aspects of literacy research, theory, and
practice. Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.
Spencer, S. L., and Fitzgerald, J. (1993). Validity and Structure, Coherence, and Quality
Measures in Writing. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25(2), 209-231.
Torrance, M., Fidalgo, R., and García, J. N. (2007). The teachability and effectiveness of
strategies for cognitive self-regulation in sixth grade writers. Learning and Instruction, 17,
265-285.
Torrance, M., Thomas, G. V., and Robinson, E. J. (1999). Individual differences in the writing
behaviour of undergraduate students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 189-
199.
154 Raquel Fidalgo, Olga Arias, Jesús Nicasio García et al.
Troia, G., Graham, S., and Harris, K., R. (1999). Teaching students with learning disabilities to
mindfully plan when writing. Exceptional Children, 65, 215-252.
Tynjälä, P., Mason, L., and Lonka, C. (2001). Writing as a learning tool. Dordrecht: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Wallace, D. L., Hayes, J. R., Hatch, J. A., Miller, W., Moser, G., and Silk, C. M. (1996). Better
revision in 8 minutes? Prompting first-year college writers to revise more globally.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 682-688.
Wong, B., Butler, D., Ficzere, S., and Kuperis, S. (1996). Teaching students with learning
disabilities and low achievers to plan, write and revise opinion essays. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 29, 197-212.
Wong, B., Butler, D., Ficzere, S., and Kuperis, S. (1997). Teaching adolescents with learning
disabilities and low achievers to plan, write and revise compare and contrast essays.
Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 12, 2-15.
Wong, B.Y.L., Butler, D.L., Ficzere, S.A., Kuperis, S., and Corden, M. (1994). Teaching
problem learners revision skills and sensitivity to audience through two instructional
modes: Student-teacher versus student-student interactive dialogues. Learning Disabilities
Research and Practice, 92, 78-90.
Yeh, S. (1998). Empowering education: Teaching argumentative writing to cultural minority
middle-school students. Research in the Teaching of English, 33, 49-83.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective. In M.
Boekaerts, P. Pintrich and M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. (pp. 13-39).
San Diego, C.A.: Academic Press.
Zimmerman, B. J., and Kitsantas, A. (2002). Acquiring writing revision and self-regulatory
skill through observation and emulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 660-668.
In: Recent Trends in Education ISBN 978-1-60741-795-8
Editor: Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas © 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Chapter 6
Problem based learning (PBL) has been adopted widely in the education of health
professionals and is used in many disciplines worldwide including medicine, nursing,
midwifery and other medical allied professions. The rationale for a PBL based programme in
midwifery lies in the theoretical claims that it promotes the development of communication
skills, teamwork, sharing information, problem solving and developing independent
responsibility for learning (Wood 2003) and that it may increase retention of knowledge,
transfer of concepts and enhance intrinsic interest (Norman and Schmidt 1992).
PBL was initially developed in the 1980s in medical education in North America as it
was felt that classroom learning did not always transfer to the clinical setting. Early
developers of the approach felt that learning in small groups through the use of clinical
problems would make medical education more interesting and relevant to practice. In the
1990s an EBL based approach was adopted by schools of nursing and midwifery in the UK.
Since the main focus of midwifery practice is on normal, healthy pregnancy and birth, the
term enquiry based learning (EBL) has been used by many midwifery programmes, but PBL
is the most commonly used term in the literature and will be used in this chapter.
From the beginning of the 1990s, midwifery education in the UK moved away from
being based in a hospital setting to being university based and is now largely a degree course
with 50% clinical experience and 50% academic. There are two courses available at present,
an eighteen month programme for those who have already trained as nurses and a three year
degree or diploma programme for those entering midwifery directly with no previous nursing
experience. In the UK, midwifery is an independent profession, and midwives may provide
total care for healthy pregnancy and birth, referring to medical professionals if needed for
medical problems or complications (NMC Midwives Rules 2005). PBL was first introduced
as a total approach to midwifery education in 1998 in the UK and has now been adopted for
156 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
LITERATURE REVIEW
Much of the literature on PBL education in health care has emerged from the field of
medical education, with a smaller number in relation to nursing education. Four reviews of
medical education in the early 1990s (Albanese and Mitchell 1993, Berkson 1993, Vernon
and Blake 1993, Norman and Smidt 1992) found student evaluations and clinical performance
was improved. However, in a few instances students scored lower in basic science
examination and viewed themselves as less well prepared for practice than their
conventionally trained counterparts (Albanese and Mitchell 1993). The reviews by Berkson
(1993), Norman and Smidt (1993) and subsequently Colliver et al (2000) concluded that there
was no clear evidence to demonstrate an important advantage of a PBL curriculum over more
traditional programmes in terms of knowledge acquisition and clinical skills, although
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 157
students may find it more motivating and enjoyable. Further studies in medical education
since the publication of these reviews also indicate that there were no significant differences
in students‘ examination results between PBL and more traditional programmes (Prince et al
2003, Mennin 2003).
Studies in nursing education similarly do not demonstrate clear benefits of a PBL
approach in terms of tested outcomes (Newman 1995, Beers 2005). Lewis and Tamblyn
(1987) using a multiple choice test and assessment by clinical instructors found no significant
differences in measurable theoretical or problem solving outcomes between a group of senior
nursing students who voluntarily participated in a problem based learning programme and
those who voluntarily remained in the traditional format, although some might argue that the
students in this context may have opted for the style that suited their personality best,
therefore achieving similar results.
In evaluating nine cohorts from a problem based learning programme in occupational
therapy Sadlo (1994) found positive evaluations by students, staff and supervisors. Students
received higher marks for their professional skills such as treatment planning and
interpersonal skills than for knowledge, which brings into focus questions about the nature of
professional knowledge.
Limited research literature exists in relation to the effectiveness of a PBL based approach
in midwifery education. McCourt and Thomas (2001) in their analysis of formal assessment
of UK students did not find clear evidence of change to students‘ overall examination
performance, and clinical mentors regarded the group as having similar levels of midwifery
knowledge to previous students, but their perceived strengths were the ability to use research,
independence in learning, critical thinking and questioning. Similarly, the course external
examiner noted few differences in the shift from the traditional to the problem-based
curriculum.
However, grading on assessments may not be a good indicator of the effectiveness of an
educational programme. Research that focuses on formal outcomes may not be the most
appropriate tool for the complexities of education. It may be that PBL educated students
perform better in the long term and retain knowledge by focusing on understanding rather
than rote learning, or engaging in deep rather than surface learning. Randomized controlled
trials may be inappropriate, untenable and unethical in many education settings and even with
randomized controlled trials there is an assumption that both groups are equivalent, but in
educational terms it is impossible to maintain blinding and therefore success or failure cannot
be confidently attributed to the intervention: teachers or students may be more or less
enthusiastic depending on their attitudes to the different educational approaches being
compared and motivation is known to influence educational outcomes.
Research on the students‘ perspectives in medical education has generally found that
students evaluated the PBL approach more positively than traditional curricula (Kaufman and
Mann 1996, Moore et al 1994). Studies of other health care professions such as occupational
therapy have also viewed PBL positively in terms of developing skills of information
management and clinical reasoning (Sadlo 1997). PBL was found to increase motivation
among nursing students (Biley 1999). A previous study of midwifery students found positive
views of their skills in using and conveying information, but anxieties about their factual
learning (McCourt and Thomas 2001).
Some studies demonstrate that students benefit from working in small groups (Albanese
and Mitchell 1993, 2001, Willis et al 2002) enabling students to verify information, develop
158 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
professional behavior, teamwork and communication skills (Biley 1999). Learning in groups
is believed to combine the acquisition of knowledge with other skills and attributes relevant to
practice. However the benefits of working in groups are dependent upon the nature of the
group and the participation and contribution of group members (Steinert 2004, Willis et al
2002) clear goals and the style of the tutors as facilitators (Steinert 2004).
Several studies from differing disciplines identify students‘ initial anxiety or uncertainty
with PBL (Moore et al 1994, Sadlo 1997, McCourt and Thomas 2001), and this appeared to
relate to the apparent lack of structure or guidance. Anxieties may also be around the time
spent searching for relevant resources (Lobb et al 2004) and uncertainty with regard to the
depth and breadth of knowledge required (Solomon and Finch 1998). Despite the perceived
advantages and benefits of PBL, nursing students experienced uncertainty, dissatisfaction,
frustration, stress with regard to finding resources and managing the workload and lacked
confidence in their own abilities (Biley and Smith 1999). Some of this may be related to the
transition from a more didactic approach to learning. McCourt and Thomas (2001) found that
the process was often experienced as challenging and students worried about their reliance on
other group members to cover all the areas of learning. They also expressed considerable
anxiety and uncertainty about their progress and the depth of their knowledge.
It may be that the benefits of a PBL based programme may be seen in the longer term.
There are a limited number of studies which explore the longer term effects of a PBL based
programme on practioners. Studies in medical education found that medical students were
satisfied with their programme in preparing them for practice and tended to favour the PBL
based course (Antepohl 2003, Prince et al 2005, Jones et al 2002). A small study of nursing
graduates found that graduates evaluated the PBL programme well (Biley and Smith 1998). A
small south African study of nursing students and their supervisors found that PBL graduates
tended to fare better in terms of their problem solving ability (Ulys et al 2004). A study of
occupational therapists 8 months after graduation found that PBL encouraged a holistic
approach, teamwork problem solving skills and information retrieval but some students felt
that these skills were intuitive rather than learnt from PBL (Reeves et al 2004).This again was
a small study with a low response rate.
Studies of the teachers‘ perspective found they were uncertain how to intervene and
manage the difficulties which could arise in groups (Kaufman and Holmes 1996, Haith
Cooper 2003). Savin Baden (2003) in her study of PBL amongst various professions found
that the lack of time to explore all areas of knowledge to the same depth was an area of
conflict for many staff as was a discrepancy they felt between a constructivist approach and
the requirements of professional practice. The only study which included the perspective of
midwife teachers was undertaken by McCourt and Thomas (2001). Key processes in the
teachers‘ early experience were around letting go of the previous curriculum, developing
facilitation skills, ‗getting the balance‘ and maintaining personal motivation and subject
interest. The teachers involved were familiar with and favoured participative and discursive
educational approaches. However, they expressed uncertainty about the degree of shift
towards facilitation, the level of students‘ responsibility for their learning and how they
should balance the principles of student-driven approaches with the structures and
requirements of professional education (McCourt 1999).
A number of key themes emerge from the literature. There appears no convincing
evidence that PBL is superior to other programmes in terms of outcomes, although few
studies have involved long terms measures. PBL appears to lead to increased motivation and
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 159
METHODS
A qualitative study using semi-structured interviews and focus groups was conducted to
explore the perceptions of students, graduates and teachers in a university in the South East of
England, based on two separate sites.
The study addressed three key areas related to a PBL based programme in midwifery:
Data were collected on the outcomes of student grades; however there were insufficient
numbers available to draw conclusions.
Current Students
Focus groups were used to elicit students‘ views of the PBL approach. They are
considered to be well suited to obtaining information about people‘s experiences and
perceptions of events and issues and are a way of listening to people and learning from them
(Morgan 1998). They draw on group interaction, combining elements of individuals
interacting with one another and participant observation to produce data and insights that
would be less accessible without the interaction found in such a group (Morgan 1993).
Focus groups were held in the first semester of both the three year and 18 month
midwifery programmes across two sites (n=4 groups) and these were repeated in the final
semester of the students‘ programmes (total=8 groups). The following questions guided the
focus group discussions:
160 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
This included perceptions of the facilitator‘s role, the effectiveness of the approach and
working with one another. The purpose of the focus group was explained verbally and in
writing to the students and participation was voluntary. The numbers of students in each
group are shown in table 1.
Table 1
Site A Site B
3 year; Semester 1 9 9
3 year; Semester 6 18 5
18 month; Semester 1 14 5
18 month; Semester 3 8 15
Two researchers conducted each focus group. These discussions were tape recorded and
transcribed.
Graduates
Given the lack of clear evidence to indicate that PBL is either more or less effective in
terms of formal educational outcomes, it was concluded that it would be useful to look at the
effects of the programme on the graduates in practice. Research on the views and experiences
of PBL graduates may help to unpick the questions that could not be answered by an
examinations-based measurement approach.
A questionnaire was sent to three groups of students, three months following
qualification; of 66 sent out in total 34 questionnaires were returned (19 from those on the 18
month programme and 15 from the three year programme) across the two sites.
To gain an insight into any longer term effects in practice, those involved in an earlier
study (McCourt and Thomas 2001) were interviewed 5-6 years following their graduation.
This involved two cohorts: the last cohort to undertake the traditional midwifery programme
and the first cohort of students to undertake the PBL programme. They had all previously
qualified as nurses and completed an 18-month midwifery programme. Using their last
known contact details, attempts were made to contact all the graduates in both cohorts by
letter and/or telephone. Owing to the long period (5-6 years) since qualifying and a highly
mobile population, this proved challenging. Of the 35 ‗pre PBL‘ group four could be
contacted, all of whom agreed to be interviewed (one by telephone). Of the 37 PBL based
graduates, seven could be contacted of whom two refused, giving being too busy as the
reason. Four were willing to be interviewed face to face and one who had moved out of the
area gave her views in writing giving a total of five responses.
The midwives were asked, using a semi structured topic guide, to discuss their views of
how the course prepared them for practice.
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 161
Teachers
The third arm of the study was an exploration of the perspectives of the midwife teachers
and the ways in which they understood their role and responsibilities. The midwife teachers
were divided into two groups, those who had been facilitators since the implementation of the
PBL based programme and those who had no experience of the previous curriculum. There
were 25 teachers in the department of whom 13 were interviewed following random selection
from each of the two groups.
Data Analysis
The key themes in each focus group were identified. One researcher undertook detailed
analysis of the recordings, coding them to identify themes and categories. A second
researcher, who also took notes, analyzed the data independently and the themes and ideas
generated were compared and then discussed within the research team. Interviews with
teachers and graduates were transcribed in full and coded in detail using open codes generated
from the data to identify key themes from each interview. The key themes from the interviews
with graduates were then compared cross-sectionally and key themes from the interviews as a
whole identified. The themes from the graduates who had undertaken a PBL based
programme and those from a traditional programme were identified separately and
subsequently combined. These were then compared with the themes identified in the focus
groups in the original study undertaken three months following the students‘ graduation
(McCourt and Thomas 2001). To ensure the reliability of the analysis, the researchers
analyzed the data independently and then discussed their preliminary findings together
Ethical permission was granted from the institution‘s ethics committee. Participants were
assured that their views would be treated confidentially and entirely separate from course
management, and all group participants were asked and agreed to respect this confidence.
Findings
Key themes from the three arms of this study are identified and discussed; these were:
The views of the students, graduates and teachers are discussed together under each
theme. We found considerable commonality of themes across the different perspectives, and
where differences of view were found, we highlight this.
162 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
The Process
The PBL process involves students meeting initially to discuss learning needs from the
trigger and what topics they will tackle, then independently researching these and feeding
back to their peers, discussing and then consolidating their knowledge in the last of three
tutorial sessions. Although the intention of the approach was to stimulate group discussion,
from their accounts of the process there appeared to be an emphasis on individual
presentations. Many students reported that feeding back information to their peers in a
presentation style made them anxious, but could also be tedious, particularly if all the
feedback for a trigger was undertaken on the same day. The graduates also felt that because
they were focusing on their individual feedback and were nervous about this they may have
missed what others were saying and this added to their worries about gaps in their learning.
This echoed their concerns expressed in the original study (McCourt and Thomas 2001).
Because of their own anxieties they did not always value or learn from the contributions of
others:
It‘s really nerve racking talking in front of your peers. Now we are getting a bit more
confident, but it‘s still quite scary- you are concentrating so much on getting your information
out that whatever has gone before you is a blur, that‘s what I found anyway. Because I was so
desperate to get mine said and done then after you can listen. S3 (student).
Everyone is nervous about their own one and lots of the time I felt I missed what everyone
else was saying because you were psyched up to present your own. 3 (PBL graduate).
Some students communicated better than others. They recognized that visual aids helped
but that they did not always have the time to develop these. Students tended to believe that
they had to adopt a teaching type role retaining a quite didactic model of education, which
may have fed their anxieties because they believed they were ‗teaching each other‘ rather than
being taught by ‗an expert‘. They tended to focus on individual presentations rather than a
more discursive approach where students talk about what they found, raising questions,
discuss and learn from this. It may be that this is more difficult in the early part of a
programme when knowledge is limited but may also reflect previous models of learning,
anxiety around feeling that they have to come up with a right answer or a notion of learning
as primarily fact-based rather than enquiry-based:
If you haven‘t got the knowledge base it‘s difficult to question because there is nothing to
question and at the same time there is everything to question. S18 (student).
It is therefore important that tutors model a questioning approach to the students in a way
which might help generate debate. In this programme, students were given a series of initial
classroom sessions on finding, understanding and critiquing evidence, but they may have
found the relevance of this difficult to appreciate early in their programme, so this needed re-
enforcement at intervals throughout. In some groups all students read around the topic area of
the trigger as a whole and brought information for discussion to the group. Those who did this
found it more helpful than individual research on an identified aspect.
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 163
All the students found the approach very time consuming and this influenced the quality
of their work. They felt that there was little time to look up the references provided by their
peers
Many of the teachers interviewed found it was challenging to discourage the students
from using rigid ways of presenting their findings and to encourage a more discursive
approach to help draw out the students‘ knowledge. They felt that there was still some way to
go with regard to helping students to question each other as this was often perceived as
threatening. Teachers tried to hold back and tease out the students‘ thinking and reasoning
rather than answering their questions for them. They sometimes needed to challenge the
students to think about what they were saying and how that related to practice:
If I have sat back and nobody in the group has challenged then I would ask the group what did
you make of that. What have you taken from that? Occasionally I put the person on the spot
by asking them to repeat what they said and to explain their understanding of it. 2 (teacher)
Teachers also felt that the students were poorly prepared for feeding back and discussing
their findings. Some of the teachers expressed uncertainty about their role in terms of how
much to intervene, for example in suggesting that a quieter member speaks first she may be
taking over responsibility or about how much to give and what to do if a student was missing
things. For some it was difficult to gauge how much to intervene and how much to stand
back. The literature highlights the fact that too little information may result in hostility
(Alvari 1995 Crookes et al 2001, Pansini Murrell 1996) but added that too much may be
stifling and lead to conflicting objectives (Haith Cooper 2000). Teachers felt that their role
was also dependent on the stage of the programme and the nature of the group. With the
students‘ increasing experience the ability to follow the process becomes a learned behavior,
so that as the group matures, and their level of knowledge and confidence increases, the level
of intervention should be progressively reduced (Pansini-Murrel 1996).
The students saw the teacher as helping to steer them in the right direction and help them
clarify their learning, helping the group reflect on the process and make decisions. Some
wanted more feedback about how they were doing but recognized that they would know from
the teachers if they were off track.
The facilitator feels a bit like big brother it‘s like the unknown curriculum and if you side
track or you want to go elsewhere then a big light goes off. S18 (student)
The students felt that the role of the teacher changed over time but also that teachers
varied in their style and approach. This would sometimes cause resentment between groups.
They suggested that there should be a standard for teachers. It was important to the student
that the teacher was enthusiastic, knowledgeable, in touch with practice and helped them
consolidate their learning.
Although some students in the focus groups found a PBL approach a better way to learn
in that they were more likely to understand and remember information when they had
researched it themselves, many appeared particularly anxious about the level and depth of
164 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
knowledge required. Particular concerns were expressed around ‗factual‘ knowledge such as
anatomy and physiology:
Some things were good in PBL and it did give you the opportunity to go off and get your own
research subject but a lot of the time we were all new students we didn‘t really understand the
subject so you didn‘t really know if you were getting the benefit from other students what they
brought back was it really what we needed to know. 3 (PBL student).
I didn‘t like it at the beginning for the first 6 months I felt like I didn‘t learn anything; the
basic knowledge wasn‘t addressed really. I didn‘t get enough feedback as to whether the
information I was getting was the right one. S 18 (student).
Students throughout the programme requested more input, guidance and feedback from
the teacher. Some would have liked a list of what should be covered in that trigger (even
though the initial tutorials used brainstorming and discussion to identify what should be
learnt) and they valued tutors who gave quizzes and activities to help consolidate learning:
I think it might have been a good idea if we had a guide as to what was expected of each
trigger, we come into the trigger almost blind, we don‘t know what is expected of us and I
think they are giving us the responsibility of facilitating they should give us the means to do
that. W3 (student).
I don‘t think you know how much you know until you need to put it into practice and that‘s
when you realize actually I do know all this. S3 (student).
Although students did receive ‗fixed resource sessions‘ and skills workshops, all the
midwives from the PBL programme felt that they would have benefited from more of a
balance between the PBL approach and lectures in their programme. They felt that a
combination would be better with some input by the teacher first:
I‘d prefer more of a combination really I wouldn‘t like to do a course like that really again
myself, totally PBL because it was too isolated. It was too much wishy washy 1 (PBL
graduate).
I wanted the teacher to open my eyes to the whole range of issues. If you have got to have
people in the ring who all don‘t know what the issues are you are not going to have a really
lively discussion. 1 (PBL graduate).
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 165
It‘s good that I‘ve done it now, but at the time while doing it I needed more, but having done
it now I can see how it works. (PBL graduate).
The literature also highlights the need for time for students to adapt to the approach and
the need for expectations to be made explicit to minimize feelings of insecurity (Taylor and
Burgess 1995, Ryan 1993, Wetzel 1996).
The four graduates who had studied prior to the implementation of PBL stated that they
enjoyed their programme. In contrast to those who had studied on a PBL based programme,
they appeared to have an easier transition from their previous nursing programme and a
clearer sense of direction with less initial anxiety early on in the programme. They appeared
to value the balance between lectures and seminars, which had smaller groups for discussion.
Learning in Groups
Some students felt that the group acted as a stimulus to their learning and that they had an
obligation to the rest of their group to contribute, but not all, and the skills of working
together developed over time. They also recognized that students had differing abilities,
cultural backgrounds and different learning styles.
Many felt that the size of the group was important and that if the number was around ten
everyone had to participate. If the group was too small there would be a reduced number to
find information but too large a group would increase the nervousness and time taken when
giving feedback and result in boredom and a lack of focus. The optimum size of group in the
166 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
literature is given as varying from 5-8 (Westburg and Jason 1996) and in this study, ten-
twelve was felt to be the maximum effective group size.
Students felt that learning depended on the individual student‘s motivation and that this
had an impact on the group:
It‘s an OK way of learning. If you are the person who will go home and study and literally put
your mind to it then you are ok, but if you aren‘t then it never gets followed up. S3 (student).
Resentments could occur in the group if members did not contribute as they had agreed.
Most felt that it was important to set ground rules. However it was sometimes difficult for the
students to question each other because of their lack of knowledge and to give honest
feedback for fear of upsetting others in the group:
You don‘t get anything from it no-one else is supporting you and you are left feeling am I too
hard on my colleague so that at the end of the day you just think oh well let it go. W18
(student).
Although most teachers undertook some form of peer review they recognised that
students were reluctant to evaluate each other honestly. This reluctance to challenge one
another did not appear to change over time and peer review was felt to be ―pointless as it was
not truthful‖. They saw ability to work with others as a central aspect of practice, which
should be supported by this educational approach, but acknowledged that students did not
find this easy in practice.
The questionnaire given to students 3 months following graduation also highlighted that
aspects which they felt should be improved were the poor contribution of some group
members.
The PBL graduates had generally enjoyed working in small groups, although sometimes
members were very disparate both in terms of their attitudes towards one another and the
course. All acknowledged that the quality of the work and consequently their learning was
dependent on the group, their participation and the standard of work produced. In some
groups the standard of work by individuals varied and poor contributions led to frustration:
The way people presented wasn‘t as good as a teacher we had to go away and learn the whole
thing again I had to go and read up. 4 (PBL graduate).
One graduate had felt bored as a student and held back by others in her group who
appeared less interested. Another felt as though she was a self-taught midwife and that no one
knew how much knowledge she had.
The graduates also stated that they felt weak students were not always identified or
followed up by the teacher and that questioning or appearing to challenge their peers was
difficult. One suggested that the student‘s participation in the trigger should be formally
assessed.
You don‘t want to make anyone feel bad so you don‘t say anything so months and weeks can
go by. 3 (PBL graduate).
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 167
Teachers felt that PBL was well suited to motivated articulate students who have a sense
of responsibility, but that it may be less effective for weaker, less motivated students who
found it difficult to access or understand information. The difficulty of facilitating a mixed
ability group was perceived as a challenge by the teachers and that the quality of work varied
hugely between different individuals so that the ―good students‖ were perceived by staff and
students as ―carrying‖ those who were less able or motivated. Some were concerned that the
more able students may be held back.
I have found it extremely difficult in the mixed ability in the group in being able to encourage
and facilitate those who are able and want to and can easily present, balancing that out with
those who for whatever reason, have a constant reason for not doing it. 4 (teacher).
I can see in the group that they have already decided unconsciously I think ‗ok I don‘t need
your information because you cannot be trusted to provide it so I can concentrate and learn
this. 4 (teacher).
I suppose one thing I emphasise right from the beginning is that it‘s team learning and the
group is where the learning takes place, and they are there to help each other, it‘s not a
competition. 12 (teacher).
Sometimes teachers found it was felt necessary to take time out from the trigger material
to discuss what was happening in the group to deal with conflicts and resentments. Hughes
and Lucas (1997) recognised that the tutor needs to feel confident in handling group dynamics
and should focus concentration on perceiving the dynamics of the particular group in
question. This should include an agreed contract about what is acceptable and what is not
acceptable as a reference point.
The importance of the group working together and supporting each other was a key
theme in this study and was also highlighted in the previous study (McCourt and Thomas
2001) and in other literature (Das Carlo et al 2003; Mpofu et al 1998). This view expressed by
students on the programme appears to have changed little following graduation, even after
five years. They tended to see group issues as a challenge, less in terms of benefits such as
teamwork and more with regard to anxieties about acquiring knowledge, and even the longer-
term graduates did not highlight the advantages of group working in the programme for their
subsequent practice experience. Teachers saw this as a key rationale for the PBL approach,
but encountered a number of challenges with this in practice, using a range of skills and
strategies to support effective group working and learning.
168 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
RELATIONSHIP TO PRACTICE
For some students theory and practice related well. This was an important aim of
adopting the PBL approach, following much discussion in the literature about perceived
theory-practice gaps in nursing and midwifery education, even though UK midwifery students
spend 50% of their time in practice placements. Following the theory with a relevant clinical
placement enabled integration of both:
We came back and it just felt that everything we had done had all come together for us.. The
theory side and then when you put it with the practical side when you come back afterwards it
all comes together in one. I quite enjoy it actually S3 (student).
Many recognized that the process would stimulate them to ask questions and be less
accepting of given facts and that it was good preparation for answering women‘s questions in
practice. They also felt they were developing skills of accessing information which would be
enable them to continue learning following qualification.
However this was not a universal view and some students expressed anxiety about their
level of knowledge for practice and felt that they were poorly prepared with regard to clinical
skills. Some students in both three-year and 18 month groups felt that the university relied on
the clinical area to teach and mentors relied on the university. They found that mentors were
often too busy or too tired to teach and they felt that some lacked a knowledge of the
curriculum and expected too much of them. Some of the graduates also felt that the enquiring
approach in theory was not always followed up in practice which was often hierarchical and
based on a medical model. Although the literature suggests that PBL enables students to
relate theory to practice, the practice environment was not always conducive to supporting
students learning in this way and students did not always relate theory and practice well.
However similar comments about a theory practice gap were made by the students on the
traditional programme.
The practical aspects of the programme were clearly identified as vital to the graduates‘
learning and satisfaction with their programme whichever programme they had undertaken
and there was a strong emphasis on the quality of their clinical placement and the importance
of continuous support from the mentors. Pre PBL graduates highlighted the value they placed
on the enthusiasm of the teachers and their ability to link theory to practice with their own
experience.
Graduates from the PBL based programme felt that theory made more sense with
practical experience. One felt that once in practice the theory became clearer and that she was
able to see the woman in a holistic way. She felt that she had developed a questioning
approach:
I had to look at everything as a whole, you have to interlink and look at the woman as a whole
person and the baby. 4 (PBL graduate).
Two students interviewed from the three-year programme who were not intending to
practice following qualification both felt that the programme had not prepared them well and
that the PBL approach had not worked well for them. 62% of recent students surveyed (n=21)
said that the course prepared them for practice either very well or quite well. When asked
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 169
what they found to be the most helpful aspects of the programme they mentioned the PBL
tutorials, discussion, clinical practice, and praxis sessions . Other issues which the students
felt should be changed included providing more practical experience and skills sessions.
The importance of the college tutors‘ presence on the clinical site was stressed by both
students and graduates and sessions conducted by link teachers in practice were valued. It was
felt by most of the PBL graduates that more teaching of skills sessions was required in
college, although the number of skills sessions did not change with the introduction of PBL.
Students in the previous study also identified the gap between theory and practice,
particularly when they were working in a medicalised environment where they did not always
feel confident to question practice (McCourt and Thomas 2001). There was no strong
evidence that this was different for PBL graduates but those interviewed were less likely than
the pre-PBL graduates to mention concerns about a theory-practice gap.
Interviews with the teachers also raised concerns that the PBL approach was not always
followed up in practice and that students did not necessarily apply classroom learning to
practice. This has been addressed in some measure by developing the preparation of mentors:
They are studying very hard in relation to the issues that we present them, yet the transference
from that to other aspects doesn‘t seem to be taking place as yet … it may be that they don‘t
have the time to apply the skills to other things 2 (teacher).
The comments from current students and graduates highlight the limited effects the style
of college-based education may have in practice, and perhaps also highlight the need for PBL
courses to be designed in a manner which is more integrated with the clinical learning
environment. Although it might be hoped that the PBL approach will gel more easily with
practice experience of students, this cannot be assumed, and practice environments may not
always reflect the principles of an enquiry-based approach to learning, by giving space or
priority to questioning, use of evidence and critical appraisal in approaching real-life practice
situations.
CONCLUSION
Although PBL may be practiced in a variety of ways the literature strongly suggests that
there are coherent themes emerging from a range of studies. These shared themes can be
found across a number of professional disciplines, such as medicine, occupational therapy and
nursing. However, we note that there are differences in the social and educational background
of students in different professions, and differences in programme focus and aims, such as the
problem focused approach of medicine, versus the health and wellbeing focus of midwifery,
reflecting childbirth as a healthy life event for most women. This meant that findings from
wider literature could not be applied uncritically to midwifery education, but the presence of
shared themes suggests these have general relevance to understanding the impact of problem-
based learning for professional practice.
Key themes in this study were that students appreciated the aims of the PBL approach,
though they sometimes focused their understanding of this around ‗learning how to get
information‘ rather than on an enquiring and critical-analytic approach to obtaining and using
information. Additionally, they expressed quite high levels of anxiety about their learning,
170 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
which our longer-term interviews suggest does not necessarily decline with time, or with the
reassurance of positive formal outcomes and practice experience, a theme highlighted in
several studies (Moore et al 2004, Sadlo 1997, Solomon and Finch 1998, Biley and Smith
1998, McCourt and Thomas 2001). These anxieties reflect concerns about how one learns,
and about obtaining basic ‗factual‘ knowledge, which perhaps stem from an underlying
tension between content and process in education. While the theoretical roots of PBL, in
constructivist philosophies of learning, suggest that process will profoundly influence the
nature and effectiveness of ‗content‘ of learning, this is not easily appreciated by students
who have previously experienced quite different models of what it is to learn. As health
professional programmes are externally regulated, with specific areas of content to be
covered, to ensure competent and safe practice, this tension is likely to remain present.
Several of the key themes to emerge illuminate further the challenges for students and
teachers, which may contribute to such anxieties. We found that students tended to treat the
tutorial process of individually ‗presenting‘ their learning as ‗performance‘ by the students or
as ‗students teaching each other‘, rather than as feedback to generate discussion, shared
learning and reflection on what they had learnt in their independent research. This may result
from the broad, less problem-based triggers in the midwifery curriculum as compared to those
commonly used in medical education, since a wide range of topics needed to be covered. It
may also reflect a generally lower level of confidence among the students, reflected in the
students‘ reluctance to debate, and their difficulty in seeing the concept of critique as different
from ‗criticism‘. The teachers attempted to encourage a more discursive approach through
modelling a questioning approach and providing development for academic critiquing skills
but did not feel this was always adequate. Ways of distributing learning tasks may need to be
reviewed to encourage further discussion in tutorial groups.
The importance of good group dynamics was identified by students, graduates and
teachers, and the way in which individuals worked together and contributed affected their
learning and is a theme in the literature (Steinert 2004, Willis 2002). Strategies to help
facilitate group working are therefore important. Teachers valued group work highly as an
important, often neglected aspect of professional practice, but this may not have been
conveyed clearly to students, who did not mention the learning about working in groups
which took place as an important skill for practice.
Although theoretical and practical aspects of the programme were related, the students
did not always feel well prepared for practice, a problem also identified by the pre-PBL
students in our earlier implementation study (McCourt and Thomas 2001). However, in our
longer-term follow up interviews it was apparent that students after the introduction of PBL
were more likely to mention being able to make connections with practice, and were also
more likely to show interest and appreciation of continuing education and the relevance of
research to practice.
Positive aspects of the curriculum identified by the students were that the PBL approach
enhanced understanding and memory and skills in information retrieval and critiquing
research. Similar themes were identified by the midwives in practice who had completed their
programme.
Students on the three-year programme generally appeared more positive than those on the
18-month programme. These differences may be accounted for by the fact that direct entry
students had more time to learn about the process or that students on the three year
programme did not have anything to compare their programme with as they had not
Evaluating Problem Based Learning in Midwifery 171
REFERENCES
Albanese, M. Mitchell, S. 1993 Problem-Based Learning: A Review of Literature on its
outcomes and implementation issues Academic Medicine 68 (1): 52-80.
Alavi C 1995 Problem based learning in a health sciences curriculum. Routledge London.
Antepohl, W. Domeij, E. Forsberg, P. Ludvigsson, J. 2003 A follow up of medical graduates
of a problem- based learning curriculum Medical Education 37(2): 155-162.
Beers, G.W. 2005 The effect of teaching method on objective test scores Journal of Nursing
education 44(7): 305-309.
Berkson, L. 1993 Problem Based learning: Have Expectations been met? Academic Medicine
68(10): 579-588.
Biley, F. and Smith, K. 1998 ―The Buck Stops here‖: accepting responsibility for learning
and actions after graduation from a problem based learning education curriculum Journal
of Advanced Nursing 27: 1021-1029.
172 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
Morgan D 1993 Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art. Newbury Park,
California: Sage Publications.
Mpofu, D. Das, M. Stewart, T. Dunn, E. Schmidt, H. 1998 Perception of group dynamics in
problem based learning sessions: a time to reflect on group issues. Medical Teacher
20(5): 421-426.
Newman, M.G. 1995 A comparison of nursing students in problem based and the lecture
method. Unpublished master‘s thesis. University of Alberta Edmonton AB.
Newman, M.G 2003 A pilot systematic review and meta-analysis on the effectiveness of
problem based learning. On behalf of the Campbell collaboration, Systematic Review
Group on the effectiveness of problem based learning. Newcastle UK: University of
Newcastle, Learning and Teaching support network, 2003.
Norman, G. and Schmidt, H. 1992 The psychological basis for problem based learning
Academic Medicine 67(9) : 557-565.
NMC 2005 (Nursing and Midwifery Council). Rules and Standards.
Peters, A. Greenburger-Rosovsky, R. Crowder, C. Block, S. Moore, G. 2000 Long term
outcomes of a New Pathway Program at Harvard Medical School Academic Medicine 75:
470-479.
Prince, K. van Eils, P Boshuizen, H. et al. 2005 General competencies of problem based
learning (PBL) and non- PBL graduates Medical Education 39(4): 394-401.
Prince, K. van Mameren, H. Hylkerma, N.et al. 2003 Does Problem Based Learning lead to
deficiencies in basic science knowledge? An empirical case on anatomy. Medical
Education 37(1): 15-21.
Reeves, S. Summerfield Mann, L. Caunce, M. Beecraft, S. 2004 Understanding the effects of
Problem based learning on practice: Findings from a survey of newly qualified
occupational therapists British Journal of Occupational therapy 67: 7.
Rowan, C. McCourt, C. Midwifery students‘ perspectives of a PBL based curriculum.
Conference presented May 5th 2006. Strength in Partnership: Education for contemporary
healthcare. Aviemore.
Ryan G 1993 Student perceptions about self directed learning in a professional course
implementing PBL. Studies in Higher Education 18(1): 53-63.
Sadlo, G. 1994 Problem based learning in the development of an occupational therapy
curriculum- part 2. The BSc at the London school of occupational therapy. British
Journal of Occupational therapy 57(3): 79-84.
Sadlo, G. 1997 Problem Based Learning enhances the educational experience of occupational
therapy students. Education for Health 10(1): 101-114.
Savin-Baden M 2003 Facililitating problem based learning SRHE Open University press.
Solomon, P. and Finch, E. 1998 A qualitative study identifying stressors associated with
adapting to problem based learning. Teaching and Learning in Medicine 10(2): 58-64.
Steinert, Y. 2004 Student perceptions of effective small group teaching Medical Education
38(3): 286-293.
Taylor I and Burgess H 1995 Orientation to self directed learning: paradox or paradigm.
Studies in Higher Education 20(1): 87-97.
Thomas, B. Quant, V. and Cooke, P. 1998 The development of a problem-based curriculum
in midwifery Midwifery 14: 261-265.
Ulys, L. Van Ryyn, L. Gwele, N. McInerney, P. Tanga, T. 2004 Problem solving competency
of Nursing Graduates Journal of Advanced Nursing 48(5): 500-509.
174 C. Rowan, S. Beake and C. McCourt
Chapter 7
ABSTRACT
The information age has permeated all aspects of our everyday lives, including
health care. Patients and the general public are now better educated and have access to
vast amounts of medical knowledge, previously only available to health care
professionals. Concurrently, many patients want to play a larger role in their personal
health care decisions. Shared decision making (SDM) is a term used to describe the
collaborative process by which patients and their health care providers make medical
decisions, and it is generally considered to be the most preferred approach. In this
chapter, we will review the current status of medical decision making, highlight the
challenges in trying to ensure that patients are empowered to participant in their
decisions, and discuss issues related to training health care providers to assist and engage
their patients in decision making. We will also explore opportunities that SDM provides
for improving health care in the future through better education, communication, and
exchange.
*
Corresponding Author: Tara Tucker, Division of Palliative Care, Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, 43
Bruyere St., Ottawa, ON, Canada, K1N 5C8, Phone: 613-562-6262 ext. 1067, Fax: 613-562-6371, Email
[email protected]
176 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
INTRODUCTION
The process of decision making in the clinical medical context has evolved over the years
from the paternalistic stance, in which the physician made medical decisions independently,
and carried them out with varying degrees of informing the patient [1], to the current belief
that patients should have the choice to take a more active role in decision making, if they
desire [2]. Recent studies have shown that the majority of patients prefer to choose how they
will engage in the decision making process relevant to their health [3,4,5,6]. Those decision
making options that a patient may choose include leaving all decisions to the medical team;
having the medical team make the final decision while respecting the patient‘s opinion;
sharing the responsibility of the decision making; making the decision independently while
considering the opinion of the team; and making decisions alone without the input of the team
[7).
Patients‘ preferences about how much they wish to be involved in decision making may
vary depending on the clinical situation, health status, and age [8,9,10]. Furthermore, these
preferences are likely to change over time if patients are exposed to tools, such as decision
aids, that support them in the process of decision making.. When given the opportunity to
choose, some patients may still routinely prefer to have the physician make treatment related
and other medical decisions [10]. However, studies suggest that the majority prefers a more
active or shared role [3,11]. In fact, there is early data to support that active involvement in
treatment decisions is associated with better quality of life [9]. Active involvement of patients
in decision making has also been shown to improve patient satisfaction and decrease
decisional conflict between patients and clinicians [12,13,14,15].
Decision making in the context of a child with life-limiting illness has added complexity
for a number of reasons: a) parents act as surrogate decision makers; b) children have varying
ability to share in decision making that is not merely based on age but on maturity,
developmental level, and competence; and c) there can be conflict between clinicians and
parents as to the rights of the child [16,17,18]. Parents of ill children have varying ability and
desire to be involved in decision making, and their desire for involvement depends on the
clinical situation, their decision making style, and their own knowledge of alternatives
[19,20,21]. With increasing awareness of the benefits of a more participatory decision making
process, and increased expectations of patients‘ control over their child‘s medical care,
professional associations are endorsing the use of participatory decision making processes
that involve clinicians, patients, children, and parents [22,23,16].
Although engaging patients in a more participatory form of decision making is not an
intuitive process, studies have shown that it is a skill that physicians can learn with structured
educational sessions [12,24,25,26]. In particular, one type of participatory decision making,
coined shared decision making (SDM), has been extensively studied as a method in which the
clinician and patient develops a partnership and both participate in the decision making
process [27-29]. This chapter will review the current status of healthcare decision making,
highlight the challenges of educating health care providers in how to engage their patients in a
shared decision making process, and explore opportunities to improve health care delivery
through better communication and discussion between health care providers and their
patients/clients.
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 177
DECISIONAL CONFLICT
Given the variety of different medical options available, whether for making a diagnosis
or for treatment, there are many opportunities in which patients may experience decisional
conflict. Decisional conflict occurs when an individual is confronted with a decision for
which there is ―uncertainty about which course of action to take when choice among
competing options involves risk, loss, regret or challenged personal life values‖[30]. As
shown in Table 1, individuals experiencing decisional conflict are more likely to change their
mind, delay making a decision, regret the decision they made, fail a knowledge test and blame
their doctor for bad outcomes [31-33]. Although there may be obvious indicators for
decisional conflict such as verbalized uncertainty, there are also subtle signs that can include
perseverance over the decision, concern about possible side effects or bad outcomes, and
feelings of distress or tension [34].
Many individuals making preference-sensitive medical decisions experience decisional
conflict; which has been clearly documented in post-menopausal women considering
hormone replacement therapy and patients with atrial fibrillation considering anticoagulation,
screening for prostate, breast or colon cancer, considering infant vaccination, and deciding on
cancer surgery [30]. The modifiable factors which appear to contribute most to patients‘
decisional conflict are consistent across studies and include feeling uninformed, being unclear
about their values, and being unsupported in the decision making process [33,34].
The concepts of shared decision making (SDM), informed decision making (IDM), and
informed consent are related in their goal to increase patients understanding and control in
their medical care; however, it is important to understand how each is different and what sets
SDM apart in order to critically evaluate the literature and effectively practice SDM.
Informed consent arises from a legal requirement in which a discussion must occur between a
patient and a clinician prior to a medical intervention. The clinician imparts knowledge of
purpose, risks, benefits, and alternatives relevant to the decision, and the patient agrees or
refuses to proceed. In some clinical scenarios (i.e. laparotomy for a gunshot wound), this may
be the most appropriate method of making a decision [35]. The result of this process is the
signing of a document that assumes that appropriate discussions occurred, and subsequently
reducing physician liability. Discussing how patients wish to receive information, or what
role they wish to take in the decision making is not required in this process [36]
178 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
With informed decision making (IDM), a patient is able to make a decision based on
having a good understanding of the nature of a disease, treatments, risks, benefits, and
uncertainties involves similar aspects. Informed decision making interventions do not
necessarily require a one-on-one discussion with a clinician, provided that enough
information is given to a patient such that they can make an informed decision, for example
through mass media, or computer based teaching tools [37]. As in informed consent, there is
no requirement for the clinician to discover the patient‘s preference for role in the process,
and there may be minimal interaction between patient and clinician.
Shared decision making includes many of the same elements of IDM, in fact, it can be
defined as a type of IDM [37]. What distinguishes SDM from other types is the concept of
partnership and sharing of decision making between the patient and the clinician [29,37] and
this is highlighted in Table 2. It also involves the components of defining the clinical
problem, presenting options (including doing nothing further) identified by both patient and
clinician discussing pros and cons of each option, identifying patients values and preferences,
discussing patients self-efficacy in following a plan, checking and clarifying understanding
throughout the process, and arranging follow up when the decision is made [38]. Shared
decision making is further defined as a process whereby patients together with their clinicians
discuss current evidence on options and arrive at a mutually agreed choice [27,29,39]. This
definition has been expanded by Elwyn and colleagues to include the explicit identifying by
the practitioner of the uncertainties or equipoise surrounding the decisions [39]. Therefore,
SDM facilitates an individualized patient-centered approach with patients actively
participating in achieving high quality decisions that are based on their own informed values
[40-42]. Through this type of process the patients‘ views are clearly acknowledged and
valued by the health care team members and incorporated into the decision making system.
Table 3 summarizes the main reasons for its importance.
Figure 1. International survey of the general public‘s views of how medical decisions should be made
(Magee, 2003).
180 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
Published studies suggest that patients do not want a paternalistic style of decision
making, nor are they comfortable with having the sole responsibility for decision making
[1,43,44,50]. There is variability in patient‘s preferred role in decision making depending on
gender, age, perception of illness, and sociocultural background, amongst others [7,8,10,51].
A national, population based survey of 2,750 adults in the US indicated that half of them
preferred that physicians make final decisions about health matters. The majority, however,
wished to discuss options and be able to give their opinions [10], suggesting a need for
continued bi-directional information exchange. While SDM allows patients and their families
to take more control over health-related decisions, this consumer centered approach has
limitations [27,52]. The burden of decision making can lead to significant patient uncertainty
and anxiety, and clinicians may not be comfortable with carrying though with patients‘
chosen options. In fact, patient preferences can change over time [9] so it is imperative for
physicians and other health professionals to be skilled in multiple techniques of decision
making, and skilled in ascertaining the individual patient‘s preference.
The variability of patient‘s participation as identified in research studies may be due to
limitations in the design of quantitative research tools. Although validated tools do exist [9,53
- 55], these tools may inadvertently exclude important concepts relevant to decision making.
In particular, the fact that decision making is an ongoing process, not a static one, and the fact
that patients may not have a full appreciation of the importance of their input may impact on
the results of quantitative, one time assessments [8]. Also, patients often make decisions in
the context of a larger social unit, the impact of which is not captured in studies that focus on
the individual [8]. Whilst it is clear that there is a variation in patient preferences, there is
growing evidence that decisions made using a SDM approach have beneficial outcomes for
patients in terms of satisfaction, increased confidence in decisions, lowered decisional
conflict, and improved compliance with treatment [9,13,14,15,56,57]. These patients also
identify higher overall quality of life, and higher physical and social functioning [9,58]. No
studies have indicated a negative impact of SDM.
Given the fact that SDM first involves eliciting the patient‘s needs and preference for role
in decision making, its initial steps will actually guide the clinician to engage the patient in
the decision making style appropriate for that patient. In the pediatric context, there is an
added complexity that parents often act as surrogate decision makers for their children.
Including a child with life-limiting illness in decision making may involve discussions of the
possible death of that child which requires openness and honesty to create an atmosphere of
trust and to give the child maximum opportunity to participate [59]. Lack of openness and
honesty are often leads to regret on the part of the parent if the child dies [60], which may
complicate subsequent bereavement. There is a degree of decisional uncertainty associated
with surrogate decision making when a parent is attempting to make decisions for their child,
especially when the decision making process does not include a needs assessment of the
parents own needs, and adequate information sharing [61]. Greater involvement of physicians
and sharing in decision making situations of high uncertainty for parents is necessary so that
parents do not feel abandoned [62].
The literature that supports SDM in pediatrics is in its relative infancy compared to that
in the adult population. However, it is becoming increasingly evident that parents do wish to
have a choice and that there is acceptance and increased satisfaction with a SDM model [63-
65]. Although it is important to involve the children in decision making if possible [63], the
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 181
barriers to engaging them include age, cognitive level, maturity, parental restrictions, amongst
others [66-68].
Table 4. Common barriers and facilitators of shared decision making (Gravel et al.,
2006- AN UPDATE REVIEW OF THIS IS NOW IN PRESS – Legare et al., in press
with Patient Education and Counselling special issue from Freiberg conference)
Barriers Facilitators
#1 Time constraints #1 Motivation of health professional
#2 Lack of applicability due to pt #2 Perception that SDM will lead to
characteristics positive impact on clinical process
#3 Lack of applicability due to clinical #3 Perception that SDM will lead to
scenario positive impact on clinical outcome
#4 Presumed patient preferences not in #4 Perception that SDM is useful/practical
keeping with SDM
#5 Not agreeing to ask patients their #5 Patient preference for SDM
preferred role for decision-making
Others include lack of self-efficacy and
familiarity with SDM
182 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
Facilitators of SDM have also been identified (Table 4), and strategies to increase SDM
knowledge and skills amongst physicians have been implemented and evaluated.
Communication problems of physicians are not solved merely with time and clinical
experience [79]. Educational interventions have been shown to increase communication skills
[25], even when delivered as short, intensive courses [24]. There is an increasing evidence
base that interventions such as educational training workshops, and using tools to screen for
decisional conflict in routine clinical practice may be effective in overcoming some of the
identified barriers [12,80,81,29,39,72].
A number of techniques to measure the clinician‘s ability to engage patients in the SDM
process have been validated. The OPTION scale, or ―observing patient involvement‖ scale, is
completed by raters who assess a consultation between a patient and clinician and evaluate
the degree of shared decision making that occurs in the interaction. It examines how well
problems are defined, role preference is evaluated, options are discussed, information is
provided, and decisions are made [82]. The Decision Support Analysis Tool (DSAT) consists
of six categories of decision support skills, and four categories of communication skills.
Similarly to the OPTION scale, raters observe an interaction between patients and physicians
and rate the items of the scale to provide an overall score [83]. Both scales can be used to
evaluate video or audio recordings of decision making encounters. They are strictly
performance based tools, and so they cannot be used to infer cognitive processes and
products. There are also many other tools available to assess decision making concepts [53-
55] but they all have their limitations and as yet no single instrument is able to identify and
measure all aspects related to the process of SDM.
DECISION SUPPORT
Decision support has emerged has a major area of research with a host of clinical
applications, so various decision support frameworks have been proposed [40-42]. The goals
of decision making support have been summarized by O‘Connor et al. [1] in the Ottawa
Decision Support Framework (Figure 2) as including the following: determining if a decision
needs to be made; providing counseling, decision tools and coaching; and reaching a high
quality decision. Decision support specifically requires the following elements to be achieved:
clarifying the decision and the patients‘ needs with regards to the decision making process;
providing facts and probabilities; clarifying the patient‘s values; providing support, guidance
and coaching; and moderating and facilitating progress. Decision quality in this context is
defined as being evidence-based, and supported by the best and most recent information
available, and entirely consistent with the values of the patient [40].
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 183
Typically the individuals trained to help patients prepare for their medical consultations
are nurses, patient navigators or cancer center resource centre staff. Breast cancer patients
who participated in consultation planning were more satisfied and reported fewer barriers to
communicating with their oncologist [49,87]. However, further research is required to address
what effects these interventions have on improving discussion of patients‘ informed values
associated with their options, which is an important element of decision quality.
Patient decision aids are tools that are designed to translate evidence into a patient
friendly format by providing information about the options, benefits, and risks along with
implicit methods of clarifying personal values [90]. Many decision aids also include
information on the condition, probabilities of outcomes related to the various options
(including the associated benefits and harms), exercises to help patients explicitly clarify their
values and guidance in the steps involved in decision making. The essential elements of
patient decision aids are illustrated in Table 5. Most patient decision aids are self-
administered and are available in a variety of formats including paper-based resources, videos
and DVDs, and computer software. However, there are also practitioner-administered
decision aids which can involve more complex approaches [91-93]. The internet has become
the most widely available resource for disseminating patient decision aids given the ease of
updating the tools as new evidence emerges and the minimal costs associated with
dissemination.
A recent update of the Cochrane review of patient decision aids identified 55 randomized
controlled trials to assess their efficacy [50] and Table 6 summarizes the main benefits of
SDM that have been identified in these studies. These decision aids covered a variety of areas
including medical treatments, surgical options, screening and vaccination. In general, patient
decision aids were found to consistently improve patient knowledge and accurate risk
perceptions, reduce decisional conflict and result in choices that were concurrent with
patients‘ values. When specific decision aids were compared to usual care, those who
received patient decision aids had higher average knowledge scores. Also, studies comparing
detailed patient decision aids with simpler ones showed a similar but smaller improvement.
Decision aids with descriptions about outcomes and probabilities were more likely to provide
accurate risk perceptions than those that did not. Furthermore, there was an approximate 50%
reduction in the proportion of patients who assumed a passive role in decision making when
decision aids were incorporated. Decision aids did not appear to have any adverse effects on
anxiety, health status, or patient satisfaction based on the studies that have been published.
Patient decision aids can potentially reduce over-use of some interventions, such as
aggressive surgery for breast cancer and hormonal replacement therapy with estrogen for
post-menopausal women, and also reduce the under-use of other interventions, such as colon
cancer screening [50]. Patient decision aids have also been shown to reduce the use of
aggressive interventions when the base rates are already low. For example, women with early
stage breast cancer who utilize the decision aids are less likely to choose a mastectomy
compared to lumpectomy plus radiation even when the baseline rates of mastectomy are quite
low [93,94].
Decision coaching is another process facilitating patient engagement in SDM [95].
Decision coaches are health care professionals who are trained to do the following: (a) assess
patients with decisional conflict and related needs; (b) tailor decision support to address the
patients‘ specific needs by offering patient decision aids and/or providing evidence- based
information, verifying understanding, clarifying values, and building skills and assessing
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 185
support; (c) guide patients through the decision making process; and (d) monitor for the
factors that can influence implementing the decision (such as motivation, self efficacy and
barriers).
Inform
• Provide facts
• Condition, options, benefits, harms
• Communicate probabilities
Clarify values
Support
Table 6. Effect of patient decision aids. (O’Connor et al., Cochrane Library, 2003)
Studies of decision coaching indicate that, when combined with patient decision aids, it
improves decision quality, increases patient satisfaction and is cost-effective [96,97]. For
example, in a study by Wirrman et al. [97] on the effect of decision aids plus nurse coaching
for men considering prostate cancer treatment, they found that men in the coaching
intervention group had higher knowledge scores and were more likely to receive treatment
consistent with their values.
186 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
Health-related call centers have been established in the United States and elsewhere in
the world to integrate decision aids, decision coaching and patient participation in relation to
medical decision making [98]. Often decision support will require a combination of
approaches including clinical consultation with the physician and the health care team as well
as patient decision aids and coaching. These approaches are now being integrated into clinical
care pathways in various health care systems in order to facilitate their acceptance and uptake
[98,99].
SUMMARY
Patients want to be involved in their health care decisions and have a right to do so.
Given the preference-sensitive nature of many medical decisions, it is essential to use
decision making approaches that acknowledge the patients‘ informed values. To support their
involvement in decision making, patients need access to clinicians skilled in decision making
and effective interventions such as question prompt sheets, consultation planning, patient
decision aids and decision coaching. Considerable effort is required to ensure that shared
decision making and effective interventions to facilitate this process are incorporated into
188 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
medical care pathways and the informed consent processes. The result will be to achieve high
quality decisions by balancing patient autonomy with clinician expertise, encouraging open
dialogue between patients and their health care team, and sharing in the responsibility for
these decisions.
REFERENCES
[1] O'Connor AM, Drake ER, Wells GA, Tugwell P, Laupacis A, Elmslie T. A survey of
the decision-making needs of Canadians faced with complex health decisions. Health
Expect 2003; 6(2):97-109.
[2] Shotton, L. Health Care Law and Ethics. Katoomba, NSW, Australia: Social Science
Press; 1997.
[3] Bruera E, Willey JS, Palmer JL, Rosales M. Treatment decisions for breast carcinoma:
Patient preferences and physician perceptions. Cancer 2002; 94(7): 2076-2080.
[4] Ford S., Schofield T, Hope T. What are the ingredients for a successful evidence-based
patient choice consultation?: A qualitative study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2003; 56:589-602.
[5] Martin S. ―Shared responsibility‖ becoming the new medical buzz phrase. CMAJ 2002;
167(3):295.
[6] Mazur DJ, Hickam DH, Mazur MD, Mazur MD. The role of the doctor‘s opinion in
shared decision making: What does shared decision making really mean when
considering invasive medical procedures? Health Expect. 2005; 8:97-102.
[7] Heyland DK, Tranmer J, O‘Callaghan CJ, Gafni A. The seriously ill hospitalized
patient: Preferred role in end-of-life decision making? J. Crit. Care 2003; 18(1):3-10.
[8] Fraenkel L, McGraw S. Participation in medical decision making: The patients‘
perspective. Med. Decis. Making 2007; 27(5):533-538.
[9] Hack TF, Degner LF, Watson P, Sinha L. Do patients benefit from participating in
medical decision making? Longitudinal follow-up of women with breast cancer.
Psychooncology 2006; 15:9-19.
[10] Levinson W, Kao A, Kuby A, Thisted RA. Not all patients want to participate in
decision making: A national study of public preferences. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2004;
20:531-535.
[11] Bruera E, Sweeney C, Calder K, Palmer L, Benisch-Tolley S. Patient preferences
versus physicians‘ perceptions of treatment decisions in cancer care. J. Clin. Oncol.
2001; 19(11):2883-2885.
[12] Elwyn G, Edwards A, Robling M, Atwell C, Russell I, Wensing M, et al. Achieving
involvement: Process outcomes from a cluster randomized trial of shared decision
making skill development and use of risk communication aids in general practice. Fam.
Pract. 2004; 21(4):337-346.
[13] Gattellari M, Butow PN, Tattersall MHN. Sharing decisions in cancer care. Soc. Sci.
Med. 2001; 52:1865-1878.
[14] Légaré F, O‘Connor AM, Graham ID, Wells GA, Tremblay S. Impact of the Ottawa
decision support framework on the agreement and the difference between patients‘ and
physicians‘ decisional conflict. Med. Decis. Making 2006; 26:373-390.
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 189
[31] Sun Q. Predicting downstream effects of high decisional conflict: Meta-analysis of the
Decisional Conflict Scale. University of Ottawa, Master of Science in Systems Science,
School of Management, 2004.
[32] Gattellari M, Ward JE. Men's reactions to disclosed and undisclosed opportunistic PSA
screening for prostate cancer. Med. J. Aust 2005; 182(8):386-389.
[33] O‘Connor AM. Predicting downstream effects of high decisional conflict.3rd
International Shared Decision Making Conference June 2005; Ottawa, Canada.
[34] O‘Connor, A. ―Decisional Conflict‖. In G.K. McFarland and E.A. McFarlane (eds),
Nursing Diagnosis and Intervention (3rd ed). Toronto: The C.V. Mosby Company, pp.
486-496, 1997.
[35] Whitney SN, McGuire AL, McCullough LB. A typology of shared decision making,
informed consent, and simple consent. Ann. Intern. Med. 2004; 140:54-59.
[36] Spinette JJ, Masera G, Jankovic M, Oppenheim D, Martins AG, Ben Arush MW, et al.
Valid informed consent and participative decision-making in children with cancer and
their parents: A report of the SIOP working committee on psychosocial issues in
pediatric oncology. Med. Pediatr. Oncol. 2003; 40:244-246.
[37] Briss P, Rimer B, Reilly B, Coates RC, Lee NC, Mullen P, et al. Promoting informed
decisions about cancer screening in communities and healthcare systems. Am. J. Prev.
Med. 2004; 26(1):67-80.
[38] Makoul G, Clayman ML. An integrative model of shared decision making in medical
encounters. Patient Educ. Couns. 2006; 60:301-312.
[39] Elwyn G, Charles C. Shared decision making: The principles and the competences. In:
Edwards A, Elwyn G, editors. Evidence-based patient choice. Inevitable or impossible?
Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2001: 118-143.
[40] Elwyn G, O'Connor A, Stacey D, Volk R, Edwards A, Coulter A et al. Developing a
quality criteria framework for patient decision aids: online international Delphi
consensus process. Br. Med. J. 2006; 333(7565):417-422.
[41] Sepucha KR, Fowler FJ, Mulley AG. Policy support for patient-centered care: The need
for measurable improvements in decision quality. Health Aff 2004;Suppl. Web.
Exclusives:VAR54-62.
[42] Ratliff A, Angell M, Dow R, Kupperman M, Nease R, Fisher R et al. What is a good
decision? Eff. Clin. Pract. 1999; 2:185-197.
[43] Coulter A, Jenkinson C. European patients' views on the responsiveness of health
systems and healthcare providers. Eur. J. Public Health 2005; 15(4):355-360.
[44] Magee M. Relationship-based health care in the United States, United Kingdom,
Canada, Germany, South Africa, and Japan. A comparative study of patient and
physician perceptions worldwide. In Ferney-Voltaire, France: World Medical
Association Patient Safety in Care and Research. 2003.
[45] Davison BJ, Gleave ME, Goldenberg SL, Degner LF, Hoffart D, Berkowitz J.
Assessing information and decision preferences of men with prostate cancer and their
partners. Cancer Nurs 2002; 25(1):42-49.
[46] Degner LF, Kristjanson LJ, Bowman D, Sloan JA, Carriere KC, O'Neil J et al.
Information needs and decisional preferences in women with breast cancer. JAMA
1997; 277(18):1485-1492.
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 191
[47] State of Washington. Providing high quality, affordable health care to Washingtonians
based on the recommendations of the blue ribbon commission on health care costs and
access (Bill 5930). 2007.
[48] Mazur DJ, Hickam DH. Patients' preferences for risk disclosure and role in decision
making for invasive medical procedures. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 1997; 12(2):114-117.
[49] Brown RF, Butow PN, Henman M, Dunn SM, Boyle F, Tattersall MH. Responding to
the active and passive patient: flexibility is the key. Health Expect. 2002; 5(3):236-245.
[50] O'Connor AM, Bennett C, Stacey D, Barry MJ, Col NF, Eden KB et al. Do patient
decision aids meet effectiveness criteria of the International Patient Decision Aid
Standards Collaboration? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Med. Decis. Making
2007; 27(5):554-574.
[51] Kremer H, Ironson G, Schneiderman N, Hautzinger M. ―It‘s my body‖: Does patient
involvement in decision making reduce decisional conflict? Med. Decis. Making 2007;
27(5):522-532.
[52] Wennberg JE. Unwarranted variations in healthcare delivery: implications for academic
medical centres. Br. Med. J. 2002; 325(7370):961-964.
[53] Dy, S.M. Instruments for evaluating shared decision making: A structured literature
review. Med. Care Res. Rev. 2007; doi:10.1177/1077558707305941 –
[54] Elwyn G, Edwards A, Mowle S, Wensing M, Wilkinson C, Kinnersley P, et al.
Measuring the involvement of patiemts in shared decision making: A systematic review
of instruments. Patient Educ. Couns. 2001; 43:5-22.
[55] Simon D, Loh A, Härter M. Measuring (shared) decision-making – a review of
psychometric instruments. Z.ärztl. Fortbild. Qual.Gesundh.wes 2007; 101: 259-267.
[56] Edwards A, Elwyn G, Hood K, Atwell C, Robling M, Houston H, et al. Patient-based
outcomes results from cluster randomized trial of shared decision making skill
development and use of risk communication aids in a general practice. Fam. Pract
2004; 21(4):347-354.
[57] Neuman HB, Charlson ME, Temple LK. Is there a role for decision aids in cancer-
related decisions? Crit. Rev. Oncol. Hematol. 2007; 62:240-250.
[58] Street RL, Jr., Voigt B. Patient participation in deciding breast cancer treatment and
subsequent quality of life. Med. Decis. Making 1997; 17(3):298-306.
[59] Beale EA, Baile WF, Aaron J. Silence is not golden: Communicating with children
dying from cancer. J. Clin. Oncol. 2006; 23(15):3629-3631.
[60] Kreicbergs U, Valdimarsdóttir U, Onelöv E, Henter J, Steineck G. Talking about death
with children who have severe malignant disease. NEJM 2004; 351(12):1175-1186.
[61] Guerriere DN, McKeever P, Llewellyn-Thomas H, Berall G. Mothers‘ decisions about
gastrostomy tube insertion in children: Factors contributing to uncertainty. Dev. Med.
Child Neurol. 2003; 45:470-476.
[62] Payot A, Gendron S, Lefebvre F, Doucet H. Deciding to resuscitate extremely
premature babies : How do parents and neonatologists engage in the decision? Soc. Sci.
Med. 2007; 64: 1487-1500.
[63] Hallström I, Elander G. Decision-making during hospitalization: Parents‘ and childrens‘
involvement. J. Clin. Nurs. 2004; 13:367-375.
[64] Merenstein D, Diener-West M, Krist A, Pinneger M, Cooper LA. An assessment of the
shared-decision making model in parents of children with acute otitis media. Pediatrics
2005; 116(6):1267-1275.
192 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
[65] Skinner EA, Diette GB, Algatt-Bergstrom PJ, Nguyen TTH, Clark RD, Markson LE et
al. Dis. Manag. 2004; 7(4):305-313.
[66] Butz AM, Pulsifer M, Walker JM, Winkelstein M. Shared decision making in school
age children with asthma. Ped. Nurs. 2007; 33(2):111-116.
[67] Hallström I, Elander G. Decision making in pediatric care: An overview with reference
to nursing care. Nurs Ethics 2005; 12(3):223-238.
[68] Kuther TL. Medical decision-making and minors: Issues of consent and assent.
Adolescence 2003; 38(150):343-358.
[69] Elwyn G, Hutchings H, Edwards A, Rapport F, Wensing M, Cheung WY et al. The
OPTION scale: measuring the extent that clinicians involve patients in decision-making
tasks. Health Expect 2005; 8(1):34-42.
[70] Loh A, Simon D, Hennig K, Hennig B, Harter M, Elwyn G. The assessment of
depressive patients' involvement in decision making in audio-taped primary care
consultations. Patient Educ. Couns. 2006; 63(3):314-318.
[71] Braddock CH, Edwards KA, Hasenberg NM, Laidley TL, Levinson W. Informed
decision making in outpatient practice: Time to get back to basics. JAMA 1999;
282(24):2313-2320.
[72] Gravel K, Legare F, Graham ID. Barriers and facilitators to implementing shared
decision-making in clinical practice: a systematic review of health professionals'
perceptions. Implement. Sci. 2006; 1:16.
[73] Butow P, Harrison JD, Choy ET, Young JM, Spillane A, Evans A. Health professional
and consumer views on involving breast cancer patients in the multidisciplinary
discussion of their disease and treatment plan. Cancer 2007; 110(9):1937-1944.
[74] Elkin EB, Kim SH, Casper ES, Kissane DW, Schrag D. Desire for information and
involvement in treatment decisions: elderly cancer patients' preferences and their
physicians' perceptions. J. Clin. Oncol. 2007; 25(33):5275-5280.
[75] Kleeberg UR, Feyer P, Günther W, Behrens M. Patient satisfaction in outpatient cancer
care: a prospective survey using the PASQOC(R) questionnaire. Support Care Cancer.
2008;16(6):567-75.
[76] Gravel K, Légaré F, Graham ID. Barriers and facilitators to implementing shared
decision-making in clinical practice: A systematic review of health professionals‘
perceptions. Implement Sci. 2006; 1(16). doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-1-16.
[77] Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Gandek B, Rogers WH, Ware, Jr. J.E. Characteristics of
physicians with participatory decision-making styles. Ann. Intern. Med. 1996;
124(5):497-504.
[78] Shepherd HL, Tattersall MHN, Butow PN. Physician-identified factors affecting patient
participation in reaching treatment decisions. J .Clin. Oncol. 2008; 26(10):1724-1731.
[79] Fallowfield L, Jenkins V, Farewell V, Saul J, Duffy A, Eves R. Efficacy of a cancer
research UK communication skills training model for oncologists: A randomised
controlled trial. Lancet 2002; 359:650-656.
[80] Sullivan MD, Leigh J, Gaster B. Brief report: Training internists in shared decision
making about chronic opioid treatment for noncancer pain. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2006;
21:360-362.
[81] Weiner JS, Cole SA. ACare : A communication training program for shared decision
making along a life-limiting illness. Palliat. Support. Care 2004; 2: 231-241.
Shared Decision Making in Medicine: Challenges and Opportunities 193
[82] Elwyn G, Edwards A, Wensing M, Hood K, Atwell C, Grol R. Shared decision making:
developing the OPTION scale for measuring patient involvement. Qual. Saf. Health
Care 2003; 12:93-99.
[83] Guimond P, Bunn H, O'Connor AM, Jacobsen MJ, Tait VK, Drake ER et al. Validation
of a tool to assess health practitioners' decision support and communication skills.
Patient Educ. Couns. 2003; 50:235-245.
[84] Gaston CM, Mitchell G. Information giving and decision-making in patients with
advanced cancer: a systematic review. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005; 61(10):2252-2264.
[85] Kinnersley P, Edwards A, Hood K, Cadbury N, Ryan R, Prout H et al. Interventions
before consultations for helping patients address their information needs. Cochrane
Database of Systematic Reviews 2007; (3):CD004565.
[86] Clayton JM, Butow PN, Tattersall MH, Devine RJ, Simpson JM, Aggarwal G et al.
Randomized controlled trial of a prompt list to help advanced cancer patients and their
caregivers to ask questions about prognosis and end-of-life care. J. Clin. Oncol. 2007;
25(6):715-723.
[87] Sepucha KR, Belkora JK, Aviv C, Mutchnik S, Esserman LJ. Improving the quality of
decision making in breast cancer: consultation planning template and consultation
recording template. Oncol. Nurs Forum 2003; Online. 30(1):99-106.
[88] Belkora J, Katapodi M, Moore D, Franklin L, Hopper K, Esserman L et al. Evaluation
of a visit preparation intervention implemented in two rural, underserved counties of
Northern California. Patient Educ. Couns. 2006; 64(1-3):350-359.
[89] Sepucha KR, Belkora JK, Mutchnick S, Esserman LJ, et al. Consultation planning to
help breast cancer patients prepare for medical consultations: effect on communication
and satisfaction for patients and physicians. J. Clin. Oncol. 2002; 20(11):2695-2700.
[90] O'Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, Tetroe J, Entwistle V, Llewellyn-Thomas H.
Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: A Cochrane
systematic review. Br. Med. J. 1999; 319:731-734.
[91] Dolan JG, Frisina S. Randomized controlled trial of a patient decision aid for colorectal
cancer screening. Med. Decis. Making 2002; 22(2):125-139.
[92] Whelan T, Sawka C, Levine M, Gafni A, Reyno L, Willan A et al. Helping patients
make informed choices: a randomized trial of a decision aid for adjuvant chemotherapy
in lymph node-negative breast cancer. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 2003; 95(8):581-587.
[93] Whelan T, Levine M, Willan A, Gafni A, Sanders K, Mirsky D et al. Effect of a
decision aid on knowledge and treatment decision making for breast cancer surgery: a
randomized trial. JAMA 2004; 292(4):435-441.
[94] Street RL Jr, Voigt B, Geyer C Jr, et al. Increasing patient involvement in choosing
treatment for early breast cancer. Cancer 1995; 76:2275-2285.
[95] Stacey D, Murray MA, Legare F, Dunn S, Menard P, O'Connor A. Decision coaching
to support shared decision making: A framework, evidence, and implications for
nursing practice, education, and policy. Worldviews Evid Based Nursing 2007.
[96] Kennedy A, Sculpher MJ, Coulter A, Dwyer N, Rees M, Abrams KR et al. Effects of
decision aids for menorrhagia on treatment choices, health outcomes, and costs. A
randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2002; 288(21):2701-2708.
[97] Wirrmann E, Askham J. Implementing patient decision aids in urology. ISBN 1 905945
02 7. 2006. Oxford, Picker Institute Europe.
194 Tara Tucker, Rajiv Samant and Dawn Stacey
Chapter 8
ABSTRACT
The most common Problem Based Learning (PBL) model is the one in which
problems appear at the beginning of the learning sequence, being introduced by the
teacher and solved by the students. This paper acknowledges a conception of PBL
organized around sets of problems formulated by the students from scenarios that may
focus on a broad theme.
In such a PBL environment the teachers‘ key role is to select or develop scenarios
that can originate relevant problems from an educational point of view. Scenarios can be
of different types, ranging from the verbal to the image-based ones, and may induce
different problems, depending on the information they offer and the intriguing power
they convey.
Bearing in mind the role of problems in a PBL sequence, the relationship between
problems and scenarios, and the fact that in traditional school settings students are hardly
given the opportunity to ask questions, two issues can be raised: are students able to
formulate relevant questions to be used for PBL purposes? How do different types of
scenarios (texts, comics and images) compare in terms of their potential to originate such
questions? Are teachers able to anticipate students‘ questions? What are the
characteristics of the social environment that better foster the formulation of high-level
questions?
Results from research carried out with teachers and lower and upper secondary
school students suggest that students can formulate high-level questions from diverse
types of scenarios and that teachers can anticipate them. As far as the social environment
is concerned, results indicate that the older students are, the less valuable group work is
in terms of high level questions induction, whatever the type of scenario. However,
results are not conclusive with regard to the comparative effectiveness of diverse types of
scenarios in what concerns their power to induce relevant questions for PBL purposes.
196 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
INTRODUCTION
Problem Based Learning (PBL) is a teaching and learning methodology first developed
during the late sixties, at MacMaster University, School of Medicine, to promote effective
education of prospective medical doctors (Boud and Feletti, 1997; Duch, 1996). It leads to the
mastery of new conceptual knowledge through the resolution of problems (Boud and Feletti,
1997; Duch, 1996; Lambros, 2002). In addition, it promotes the development of problem
solving competencies as well as social and communicational skills (Lambros, 2004).
However, opposite to what could be expected, the idea of using problems as starting points
for learning has originated different conceptions of PBL, and a wide variety of PBL models,
ranging from teacher to student centered models (Savin-Baden and Major, 2004). The latter
case includes the most typical PBL model that is the one in which problems appear at the
beginning of the learning sequence, and are solved by the students (Lambros, 2002; Savin-
Baden and Major, 2004). It should be emphasized that, usually, each PBL sequence includes a
single or very few problems embedded in problem contexts brought in by the teacher. This
paper acknowledges a rather different conception of PBL, described in Leite and Afonso
(2001) and put into practice by Gandra (2001). It is organized around sets of interrelated
problems that are formulated by the students from scenarios. The scenarios may be brought in
by the teachers and must focus on a quite broad theme. Thus, problems formulated from a
scenario may require students to work out a set of concepts that constitute the core of an
everyday or a curriculum relevant theme. The added value of working on sets of interrelated
problems rather than on individual problems lies in the fact that the former promotes
conceptual integration and facilitates the development of overarching ideas. In addition,
problem formulation by the students themselves increases their motivation to engage in an
inquiry process aiming at solving the problems. If students feel the problems to be solved as
their own problems, then the problem solving process will be less painful for them (Watts,
1991) and learning will be more rewarding (Lambros, 2004) and everlasting.
Introducing PBL in school curriculum may be an easy or a complex task depending on
the sort of curriculum that is acknowledged. Problem-based curricula promote the integrated
development of conceptual and procedural competencies (Margetson, 1997) as students learn
from their attempts to find out solutions for the curriculum prescribed problems. This type of
curriculum differs from problem-oriented curriculum as well as from problem solving
curriculum (Ross, 1997). In the former type of curriculum, relevant problems are used as
criteria for selecting content to be included in the curriculum but students may learn it
through a variety of educational strategies. In the latter case, the curriculum main goal is to
foster the learning of problem solving strategies and therefore the focus is on the problem
solving process being conceptual leaning released to a second plan. In addition, to these types
of curriculum one can think about concept-based curricula, that prescribe the concepts and
principles that students are required to learn, regardless of their relationship to problems. If
this type of curriculum is to be approached from a problem-based perspective, problems to be
selected must require the learning of the prescribed concepts.
The conception of PBL acknowledged above has been put into practice in secondary
school science teaching within concept-based (Gandra, 2001) and competencies-based
(Esteves, Coimbra and Martins, 2006) curricula frames. Although both curricula emphasize
the concepts to be learned, the competencies-based curriculum is more flexible and it
Putting PBL Into Practice: 197
facilitates alternative planning both intra and inter school subjects. The same PBL approach
has also been implemented in a science teacher education context (Leite and Esteves, 2005;
Esteves and Leite, 2005).
It is worth noticing that, in such a PBL environment the teachers‘ key role is to select or
develop scenarios that can originate relevant problems from an educational point of view
(Lambros, 2002). Scenarios can be of different types, ranging from the verbal to the image-
based ones (Lambros, 2002; Dahlgren and Öberg, 2001). Dalghren and Öberg (2001) got
some empirical evidence that different types of scenarios may induce different problems,
depending on the information they offer and the intriguing power that underlies them. Thus,
bearing in mind the role of problems in a PBL sequence, the relationship between problems
and scenarios, and the fact that in traditional school settings students are hardly given the
opportunity to ask questions, four main research issues can be raised: are students able to
formulate relevant questions to be used for PBL purposes? Are teachers able to anticipate the
questions that students raise from a scenario? How do different types of contexts (texts,
comics and images) compare in terms of their potential to originate such questions? What are
the characteristics of the social environment that foster the formulation of high-level
questions?
This paper aims at shading some light on these issues by providing empirical information
on the characteristics of good scenarios as well as on the best social conditions of using them
for students‘ question formulation purposes.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
Problems are the starting points for learning in a PBL context. The motivational power of
problems is therefore a key factor for PBL success. The more real problems are or look like,
the more students will feel learning meaningful and appealing. This is important because
motivation positively influences learning (Wellington, 2000) and contextualization promotes
retention (Boud and Felettti, 1997; Albanese and Mitchell, 1993; Hennessy, 1993; Duch,
1996). These are the main reasons why several authors emphasize de importance of scenarios
to contextualize science education problems (Boud and Felettti, 1997; Albanese and Mitchell,
1993; Hennessy, 1993; Duch, 1996; Rennie and Parker, 1996; Lambros, 2004). The
motivational power of the problems is dependent on the scenario within which problems
emerge. Hence, scenarios should engage, puzzle, challenge, motivate students and lead them
to raise high-level questions that require an inquiry approach to be solved (Lambros, 2004;
Mauffette, Kandibinder and Soucisse, 2004). This means that scenarios are not good or bad in
any absolute sense. The usefulness of scenarios depends on their power to activate students‘
minds. Consequently, it means that criteria for scenario selection or construction should be
students‘ interests dependent (Lambros, 2002, 2004). It can be expected that the older the
students are, the easier it is to find appropriate scenarios (Lambros, 2004) from a motivational
point of view. In fact, high secondary school students can more easily imagine themselves in
the role of a professional (e.g. policeman, a nurse, an architect, or a teacher) than junior high
school students do (Lambros, 2004). This may lead them to better feel the relevance of a
context and the meaning of a given piece of knowledge (Lambros, 2004).
According to some authors (Lambros, 2004; Savin-Baden and Major, 2004), scenarios
may have different formats, ranging from text-based (e.g. newspaper or journal article,
poems) to image-based (e.g. picture, drawing), and they may also combine different
Putting PBL Into Practice: 199
languages (e.g. comics, song, video). While text based scenarios can offer more details to the
reader, they may also be less motivating and become boring for those who do not enjoy
reading too much. Image-based scenarios are less informative and leave more room to
imagination and creativity. Scenarios that combine two or more languages may overcome the
disadvantages of one with the strengths of the other. It should also be emphasized that
scenarios should be related to students‘ world and interests (Alcázar, 2006; Boud and Feletti,
1997; Lambros, 2004; Savin-Baden and Major, 2004; Mauffette et al, 2004; Barrel, 2007) as
there is some evidence that students understand problems better when they can relate them to
their everyday lives, so that they look like real (Gandra, 2001).
Besides, scenarios should not induce answers (Dahlgren and Öberg, 2001; Hmelo-Silver,
2004; Alcázar, 2006) and should be as short as possible to not bore students (Dahlgren and
Öberg, 2001). These are the main reasons why it may be hard to find out a scenario ready to
be used as such. The most likely is that real scenarios need to be adapted to cut off irrelevant
information and/or to eliminate information that might lead to anticipate answers.
Besides, despite the fact that to build a good and motivating scenario is never an easy
task (Mauffette, Kandibinder and Soucise, 2004), it is even harder to do it when PBL is to be
integrated in a concept based curriculum. When this type of curricula is at stake, scenarios
have an additional requirement: they need to raise problems that require a certain amount of
concepts to be learnt. Therefore, the first task to be carried out is the identification of the
concepts and the competencies that students are expected to develop (Esteves and Leite,
2005). Afterwards a matching scenario is to be prepared or selected. Of course, the scenario
has to be appropriate to students‘ age (Lambros, 2002; 2004) in order to offer challenges that
students are able to overcome.
The majority of the studies aiming at analyzing students‘ questioning from scenarios
dealt with only one type of scenario. In most of these studies the format of the scenario was a
text-based one and therefore no comparison between types of scenarios could de done.
When students are explicitly asked to formulate questions from scenarios, they formulate
more questions than they usually do in the classrooms (Costa et al., 2000; Leite and Palma,
2006) although they may not be able to formulate as much questions as they are asked to
(Leite and Palma, 2006; Leite, Palma and Leme, 2007). In addition, whatever the format of
the scenario, students formulate questions of diverse levels of complexity, even though the
low level questions prevail over the other (Chin, 2001; Chin and Chia, 2004; Sanjosé et al.,
2006). Opposite to what could be expected, younger students seem to ask more questions than
older students do (Costa et al., 2000). This result may be due to the fact that older students
have higher levels of expertise on the issues dealt with in the scenario and therefore they do
not find it necessary to ask too many questions. On the other hand, students seem to be more
interested in explanations of phenomena than on their implications, as they ask more
questions focusing on causes than on consequences (Gomes, 1999). In addition, the level of
students‘ questions does not seem to correlate with students‘ conceptual performance (Harper,
Etkina and Lin, 2003). This means that low achievers can ask better questions than high
performers probably because the former feel that they have a lot to learn. However, older
students seem to ask more complex questions than younger students (Sanjosé et al., 2006).
200 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
This may mean that the level of complexity of the questions is dependent on students‘
cognitive abilities that develop with age.
Dahlgren and Öberg (2001) compared students‘ questions asked from five different types
of scenarios: comics, picture, drawing, saying, and text from journal. All the scenarios raised
the five types of questions used by authors for data analysis purposes: encyclopaedic,
meaning-oriented, relational, value-oriented and solution-oriented. However, the most
frequent types of questions were found to depend on the types of scenario. In fact, comics
raised more meaning-oriented questions while the picture raised more encyclopaedic
questions and the drawing more solution-oriented questions. As far as the text and the old
saying are concerned, no type of questions was found to prevail. According to the authors, the
motivating power of the students as well as the level of information they convey may explain
the effect of the type of context on students‘ questions.
It should also be noticed that when the formulation of questions takes place in small
groups, the level of the questions tends to be higher than when questioning takes place in a
students‘ individual basis (Chin and Kayalvizhi, 2002; Dahlgren and Öberg, 2001; Leite and
Palma, 2006). This result suggests that if questioning from scenarios is to be carried out for
PBL purposes, then it might be worthwhile to have students working in small groups in order
to increase the number of high level questions.
METHODOLOGY
Synopsis of the Study
Two research studies were conducted in order to answer the research questions presented
above. Study one (drawing heavily on Loureiro, 2008) focused on the relationship between
the questions formulated by secondary school students and those anticipated by physical
sciences teachers from three different types of scenarios. Study two (based on Oliveira, 2008)
concentrated on the relationship between different social conditions of the learning
environment used for secondary school students‘ questions formulation from the same three
types of scenarios. The conditions taken into account were individual question formulation,
group question formulation and group formulation following individual formulation.
Sample
Study one included a sample made of 30 physical sciences teachers and 176 7th, 9th, and
11th graders. Teachers were spread over of the northern part of Portugal. Students were
selected on a class basis, from three secondary schools located in the north of the country as
well.
Study two involved 175 9th and 11th grade students, belonging to six classes from three
secondary schools in the northern part of Portugal. To attain the objectives of the study two
groups of students were required: one that should formulate questions only in group settings
and another one that should start by formulating questions in an individual basis. Therefore,
Putting PBL Into Practice: 201
the six classes were arranged in such a way as to originate two groups of students attending
the two grade levels.
Data Analysis
RESULTS
Students’ Produced Questions Versus Teachers’ Anticipated Questions
As it is shown by graph 1, diverse types of scenarios and science topics led to different
mean numbers of questions formulated per student. These mean numbers range from 1.9 (9th
grade, news-type climate change scenario) to 4.7 (7th grade, news-type seasons scenario).
However, when comparing the performance of the students belonging to each one of the three
grade levels in the formulation of questions from the three types of scenarios, two types of
patterns were found, one for each science topic.
As far as the climate change scenarios are concerned, there is a well-defined type of
pattern with a maximum for the Comics scenarios and a minimum for the news scenario. This
means that the three types of scenarios had similar effects in terms of raising questions on the
three grade levels. It also means that the most provocative scenario was the comics type
scenario, whatever the grade level. In addition, a comparison of the relative position of the
specific patterns obtained for the three grade levels indicates that seventh graders were the
students that formulated higher mean numbers of questions, irrespective of the type of
scenario. This result may be due to the bigger sense of curiosity of the youngest students
(Chin and Chia, 2004; Sanjosé et al., 2006) that may have lead them to ask more questions
than their counterparts did. In addition, the increase in the mean number of questions from 9th
to 11th grade may reveal that older students are either more able to ask questions (Costa et al.,
2000; Sanjosé et al., 2006) or more worried about the issue of climate change than their ninth
grade counterparts are, despite having studied the issue deeper at school. Hence, subjects‘
sense of curiosity, cognitive development and familiarity with the issue dealt with in the
Putting PBL Into Practice: 203
scenario may exert influence on the mean number of questions formulated by the students
although this influence does not interfere with the power of the diverse types of scenarios to
induce questions.
When the Seasons topic is at stake, the patterns obtained for the diverse grade levels were
less clear than in the climate change case although they all show a maximum for the news-
type scenario. Besides, their relative position compares to the one obtained for the climate
change scenarios. This result reinforces the idea of curiosity leading younger students to
produce more questions and the positive influence of cognitive development and/or schooling
on the number of questions produced by the students.
Comparing the three types of scenarios in terms of power to produce questions about
seasons and climate change, it seems that the type of scenario that includes more details in
terms of information (news) was the one that induced more questions in the cased of the
seasons but the scenario that combines text and image (comics) was the one that originated
more questions in the climate change case. Therefore, no straightforward relationship was
found among the three types of scenarios with regard to the number of questions they induce
and the provocative feeling they originate in students.
Graph 2 shows the mean number of questions that teachers anticipated that students
belonging to the same three grade levels would produce from the same of scenarios.
7,0
6,0
5,0
students' mean number of questions
7th grade S
4,0 7th grade CC
9th grade S
9th grade CC
3,0 11th grade S
11th grade CC
2,0
1,0
0,0
News Comics Image
T ypes of scenarios
Graph 1. Students‘ mean number of questions formulated from the three types of scenarios, per grade
level and topic (S=Seasons; CC= Climate Change).
204 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
7,0
6,0
Teachers' mean number of questions
5,0
7th grade S
4,0 7th grade CC
9th grade S
9th grade CC
3,0
11th grade S
11th grade CC
2,0
1,0
0,0
News Comics Image
T ypes of scenarios
Graph 2. Teachers‘ mean number of questions anticipated from the three types of scenarios, per grade
level and topic (S=Seasons; CC= Climate Change).
A comparison graph 1 and graph 2 suggests that teachers‘ patterns are more ill-defined
than were those obtained in the students‘ case. However, a closer look at graph 2 indicates
that patterns may depend at least in part on the school level in which teachers were focusing
on. In fact, well-defined patterns were got for both topics at the low secondary school levels
but these patterns are a bit different from those obtained with for the upper secondary school
level. Nevertheless, specific teachers‘ patterns are roughly similar for the climate change
topic and quite different for the seasons ones. In addition, teachers‘ patterns for the climate
change topic differ from the students‘ ones. Hence, as far as the type of scenario that
originates the highest numbers of questions for the climate change topic is concerned, it
seems that there is no opposition between students and teachers. However, the same does not
apply to the type of scenario that originates the lowest number of questions, as it is the news
one for the students and the image one for the teachers. On the other hand, the patterns got for
the seasons scenarios have a maximum at the news (as in the students case) one and a
minimum at the image one (which differs from the students case). Thus, it seems that
teachers‘ anticipation of students‘ questions from the three types of scenarios was more
accurate for the low secondary school grade levels than it was for the 11th grade.
A comparison of the two graphs referred to above indicates that the mean numbers of
questions anticipated by the teachers tends to be larger than the mean numbers of questions
formulated by the students from each scenario. Also, with the exception of 11th grade climate
change comics scenario, opposite to what was obtained with the students (graph 1), teachers
(graph 2) seem to believe that the younger students are, the more questions they would
Putting PBL Into Practice: 205
produce from the three scenarios and science topics. Possible explanations for this
relationship may be related to teachers‘ belief in that the number of questions: depends on
students curiosity in such a way as the younger students are, the more curious they are, and,
therefore, the more questions they produce (Bennett, 2003; Chin and Chia, 2004); decreases
as students mastery level of the issues increases (Bennett, 2003; Costa et al., 2000).
Graph 3 shows the types of questions formulated by students belonging to the three grade
levels from the three types of scenarios dealing with the two science topics. The two most
frequent types of questions formulated by the students, whatever the type of scenario, the
grade level and the topic are the encyclopaedic and the meaning-oriented questions. However,
there is a tendency for the meaning-oriented questions to prevail over the other types of
questions, except for the 7th grade when students concentrated on the climate change news
and comic scenarios. Also, news and comics scenarios compare in terms of percentage of the
two most frequent categories of questions and they differ from the image one. In fact, there is
a big distance between the percentages of meaning-oriented and encyclopaedic questions,
especially in the two lower grades. It should be stressed that although students produced large
amounts of encyclopaedic questions, this type of questions has a low cognitive demand (Chin
and Brown, 2000; Costa et al., 2000; Dalghren and Öberg, 2001; Leite and Palma, 2006) and
therefore includes non-challenging and inappropriate questions for the implementation PBL
approach. However, it should be noticed that the meaning-oriented questions might be used
for PBL purposes (Chin and Kayalvizhi, 2002; Leite and Palma, 2006), as providing an
answer to them requires an understanding of the issue that is at stake (Chin and Kayalvizhi,
2002; Dalghren and Öberg, 2001; Leite and Palma, 2006).
Relational, value-oriented and solution-oriented questions were quite rare although the
third type of questions seems to be a bit more frequent when climate change based scenarios
are at stake. The results suggests that despite the fact that climate change is a current topic
often discussed in mass-media and dealt with in the school as the consequences of the climate
change are a threat to life on earth, students did not produce too many questions providing
evidence that they feel curious and worried about it. As far as the seasons topic is concerned,
the explanation for students having produced few high level questions may lay in the fact that
they take seasons for granted and therefore think of them as something that they cannot
interfere with.
The percentages of the different types of questions that teachers anticipated that students
of the three grade levels would formulate from the three types of scenarios focusing on the
two science topics (seasons and climate change) are shown in Graph 4.
A comparison of graphs 3 and 4 reveals that teachers‘ anticipation of students‘ questions
is quite consistent with students‘ formulation of questions. In fact, the majority of the
questions that teachers anticipated that students would formulate from the diverse scenarios
are meaning-oriented and encyclopaedic questions (graph 4), These are also the types of
questions that prevail among students‘ formulated questions (graph 3). Likewise in students‘
questions (graph 3), meaning-oriented questions prevail over the teacher anticipated
encyclopaedic questions in all but two cases: 7th grade seasons and climate change. In
addition, it should be noticed that although teachers anticipated that students would
formulated a low percentage of the three higher level questions (as students did), the
percentages of questions that they anticipated are higher than those obtained with the
students.
206 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
100,0
90,0
80,0
70,0
60,0 Encyclopaedic
Percentage
Meaning orient ed
50,0 Relational
Value orient ed
40,0 Solution oriented
30,0
20,0
10,0
0,0
11th
11th
11th
11th
11th
11th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
S CC S CC S CC
News Comics Image
Graph 3. Types of questions formulated by the students from the different types of scenarios focusing
on the two topics (S=Seasons; CC= Climate Change).
100,0
90,0
80,0
70,0
60,0 Encyclopaedic
Percentage
Meaning orient ed
50,0 Relational
Value orient ed
40,0 Solution orient ed
30,0
20,0
10,0
0,0
11th
11th
11th
11th
11th
11th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
7th
9th
S CC S CC S CC
News Comics Image
Graph 4. Types of questions anticipated by the teachers from the different types of scenarios focusing
on the two topics (S=Seasons; CC= Climate Change).
Putting PBL Into Practice: 207
Ninth and 11th graders were asked to formulate questions from three types of scenarios
(news, comics and image), focusing on the energy crisis issue, in three different social
conditions: individual formulation (I), formulation in small groups (G), formulation in small
groups after formulation in an individual basis (IG). According to data given in graph 5, the
social conditions under which the questions were produced from the three types of scenarios
led to similar patterns of the individual (I) and group (IG and G) mean numbers of questions
in the 9th grade. In fact, in the 9th grade, maximums were got for the news scenario and
minimums for the comics whatever the social conditions under which the questions were
produced.
12,0
10,0
Students' mean number of questions
8,0
I
6,0 IG
G
4,0
2,0
0,0
News Comics Image News Comics Image
9th grade 11th grade
Graph 5. Students‘ mean number of questions formulated from the three types of scenarios focusing on
the topic energy, in diverse social conditions (I=Individually; G=small Group).
208 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
As far as the 11th grade is concerned, two social conditions (G and IG) yield similar
specific patterns that differ from the pattern of questions formulated by students in an
individual (I) basis. This difference in patterns is due to the fact that the news scenario
originated a mean number of questions per student when they formulated them in an
individual basis lower than the mean number of questions per group when they did it in the
other two social conditions. It is worth noticing that the formulation of questions (only) in
small groups led to larger means of questions per group than their formulation by groups of
students that had produced questions individually before trying to produce them in small
groups. This result is opposite to the expectation based on social constructivism (Vygotsky,
1986) that group work would yield a larger variety of questions than individual work would
do (Chin and Kayalvizhi, 2002; Palma and Leite, 2002). A possible explanation for this
unexpected result may be due to the fact that the previous production of questions in an
individual basis may reduce students‘ curiosity and willingness to repeat the task although in
a different social condition, that is in a small group basis. The different position of the
individual pattern in 9th and 11th grade with regards to the other two social conditions patterns
is in agreement with the result described in the previous section according which 11th graders
produced higher mean numbers of questions than 9th graders did. However, while group work
seems to have had no meaningful reducing effect in the 9th grade, it had a reducing effect in
the 11th grade in all but the news type scenario. This may mean that the role of group work
decreases as the school grade increases probably due to the increase of students‘ cognitive
development and critical thinking (Costa et al., 2000; Jesus, 2001).
Graph 6 shows that encyclopaedic and meaning oriented questions were the types of
questions that prevailed among the questions formulated by the students from the three
scenarios based on the energy crisis issue.
It also shows that with the exception of the case of 11th graders formulating questions in
small groups, the most frequent questions were the encyclopaedic-like type of questions. This
result differs from the seasons and the climate change scenarios (previous section), where the
meaning-oriented questions usually prevailed over the encyclopaedic ones for both students‘
formulated questions and teachers‘ anticipated ones.
As far as the three higher-level types of questions are concerned, students produced small
amounts of these types of questions. This result is consistent with the one described in the
previous section in relation to the seasons and the climate change topics. A comparison of the
percentages of questions formulated by students of both grade levels in the diverse social
conditions reveals that, with the exception of 11th grade IG, whatever the grade level and the
social condition, similar patterns were obtained for the solution-oriented questions. These
patterns have a maximum for the comics-type scenario. It seems hard to find an explanation
for this but it may happen that this specific scenario contains some piece of information that
led students to feel the need for measures to solve the energy crises to be taken.
In addition, it seems that 11th grade produced larger percentages of relational questions
than their 9th grade counterparts. This may indicate that these students felt a deeper concern
about the energy crises and a willingness to better understand the energy issue. Despite the
fact that the percentages are very small, whatever the grade level and the social conditions,
the image seems to be the type of scenario that originated lowest percentages of value-
oriented questions. This may mean that students are not used to interpret images and/or that
they need a considerable basis of information about a given issue in order to feel compelled to
try to better understand it (Dahlgren and Öberg, 2001).
Putting PBL Into Practice: 209
70,0
60,0
50,0
Encyclopaedic
Percentage
20,0
10,0
0,0
News
News
News
News
News
News
Image
Image
Image
Image
Image
Image
Comics
Comics
Comics
Comics
Comics
Comics
9th 11th 9th 11th 9th 11th
I IG G
Graph 6. Percentage of the different types of questions formulated by the students from the three types
of scenarios focusing on the topic energy, in diverse social conditions (I=Individually; G=small Group).
Thus, an overall analysis of the results indicates that students formulated larger mean
numbers of questions from scenarios focusing on the energy issue than from those scenarios
dealing with the seasons and the climate change scenarios. Also, group work seems to be
more productive if used as the only environment in which questions are produced than if it is
used together with individual work.
REFERENCES
Albanese, M. and Mitchell, S. (1993). Problem based learning: a review of literature on its
outcomes and implementation issues. Academic Medicine, 68, 52-81.
Alcázar, M. (2006). Problems of teaching in problem based learning. In Actas do Congresso
Internacional PBL2006ABRP (Cd-Rom). Lima (Peru): Pontifícia Universidad Católica
del Perú.
Putting PBL Into Practice: 211
Atwood, A. and Wilen, W. (1991). Wait time and effective social studies instruction: what
can research in science education tell us? Social Education, 55(3), 179-181.
Baram-Tsabari, A. et al. (2006). Using questions sent to an ask-a-scientist site to identify
children‘s interests in science. Science Education, 90, 1050–1072.
Barell, J. (2007). Problem-based learning: an inquiry approach. Thousand Oaks: Corwin
Press.
Bennett, J. (2003). Teaching and learning science: a guide to recent research and its
applications. London: Continuum Books.
Boud, D. and Feletti, G. (1997). Changing problem-based learning. In Boud, D. and Feletti,
G. (Eds.). The challenge of problem based learning. London: Kogan Page, 1-14.
Chin, C. and Brown, D. (2000). Learning in science: A comparison of deep and surface
approaches. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37 (2), 109-138.
Chin, C. and Chia, L (2004). Problem-based learning: Using students‘ questions to drive
knowledge construction. Science Education, 88, 707-727.
Chin, C. and Kayalvizhi, G. (2002). Posing problems for open investigations: what questions
do pupils ask? Research in Science and Technological Education, 20(2), 269-287.
Chin, C. (2001). Learning in science: What do students‘ questions tell us about their thinking?
Education Journal, 29(2), 85-103.
Chin, C. (2006). Classroom interaction in Science: teacher questioning and feedback to
students´ responses. International Journal of Science Education, 28(11), 1315-1346.
Costa, J. et al. (2000). An analysis of question asking on scientific texts explaining natural
phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(6), 602-614.
Dahlgren, M. and Öberg, G. (2001). Questioning to learn and learning to question: Structure
and function of problem based learning scenarios in environmental science education.
Higher Education, 41, 263-282.
Dori, Y. and Herscovitz, O. (1999). Question-posing as an alternative evaluation method:
Analysis of an environmental case study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
36(4), 411-430.
Duch, B. (1996). Problem-based learning in physics: The power of students teaching students.
Journal of College Science Teaching, March/April, 326-329.
Esteves, E. and Leite, L. (2005). Learning how to use the laboratory through problem based
learning: A pilot study in an undergraduate physical sciences teacher education
programme. In Proceedings of the ATEE Conference. Amesterdam: Amsterdam Free
University. http: www.atee2005.nl/search/paperworks.php?contrid=121.
Esteves, E., Coimbra, M. and Martins, P. (2006). A aprendizagem da Física e Química
baseada na resolução de problemas: um estudo centrado na sub-unidade temática ―Ozono
na estratosfera, 10º ano. In Costa, L. et al. (Coords.). Actas do XIX Congresso Enciga
(CD-Rom). Póvoa de Varzim: Escola Secundária Eça de Queirós.
Gandra, P. (2001). A Aprendizagem da Física Baseada na Resolução de problemas. Um
estudo com alunos do 9º ano de escolaridade na área temática ―Transportes e
Segurança‖. Unpublished master dissertation, University of Minho.
Gomes, C. (1999). Desenvolvimento de destrezas metacognitivas em aulas de Física: relação
entre a profundidade das explicações em textos científicos e a formulação de perguntas
pelos alunos. Unpublished master dissertação, University of Minho.
212 Laurinda Leite, Isménia Loureiro and Paula Oliveira
Harper, K., Etkina, E. and Lin, Y. (2003). Encouraging and analyzing student questions in a
large physics course: Meaningful patterns for instructors. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 40(8), 776-791.
Hmelo-Silver, C. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how do students learn?
Educational Psychology Review, 16 (3), 235-266.
Hennessy, S. (1993). Situated cognition and cognitive apprenticeship: Implications for
classroom learning. Studies in Science Education, 22, 1-41.
Hofstein, A. et al. (2004). Developing students‘ ability to ask more and better questions
resulting from inquiry-type chemistry laboratories. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 42 (7), 791-806.
Jenkins, E. (2006). The student voice and school science education. Studies in Science
Education, 42, 49-88.
Jesus, M. (1997). Perguntas e ideias de alunos do 9º ano sobre a estrutura atómica da matéria.
In Leite, L. et al. (Eds.). Didácticas/Metodologias da Educação. Braga: University of
Minho, 373-387.
Jesus, M. et al. (2001). Questioning in Chemistry at the University. In Proceedingas of the 6th
European Conference on Research in Chemical Education. University of Aveiro,
http://sweet.ua.pt/~a22603/92001_ECRICE_Questioning_Chemistry_University-.pdf
(accessed: 20/12/2006).
Lambros, A. (2002). Problem-Based Learning in K-8 classrooms. Thousand Oaks: Corwin
Press.
Lambros, A. (2004). Problem-Based Learning in middle and high school classrooms.
Thousand Oaks: Corwin Pres.
Leite, L. and Afonso, A. (2001). Aprendizagem baseada na resolução de problemas:
Características, organização e supervisão. Boletín das Ciencias, 48, 253-260.
Leite, L. and Esteves, E. (2005). Ensino orientado para a Aprendizagem baseada na resolução
de problemas na Licenciatura em ensino de Física e Química. In Silva, B. and Almeida,
L. (Eds.). Actas do Congresso Galaico-Português de Psicopedagogia (Cd-Rom). Braga:
University of Minho, 1-17.
Leite, L. and Palma, C. (2006). Formulação de questões, educação em ciências e
aprendizagem baseada na resolução de problemas: Um estudo com alunos portugueses do
8º ano de escolaridade. In Actas do Congresso PBL 2006 ABP (CD-Rom). Lima (Peru):
Universidade Pontifícia Católica del Peru (8 pág).
Leite, L., Costa, C. and Leme, J. (2007). Energia e Educação em Ciências para a Cidadania:
dos interesses dos alunos às temáticas abordadas por manuais escolares. In Barca, A. et
al. (Eds.). Actas do IX Congresso Internacional Galego-Portugués de Psicopedagoxiía
(Cd-Rom). Coruña: Universidade de A Coruña, 2597-2609.
Loureiro, I. (2008). A aprendizagem baseada na resolução de problemas e a Formulação de
questões a partir de contextos problemáticos: Um estudo com professores e alunos de
Física e Química. Unpublished master dissertation, University of Minho.
Marbach-Ad, G. and Sokolove, P. (2000). Can undergraduate biology students learn to ask
higher level questions? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(8), 854-870.
Margetson, D. (1997). Why problem-based learning is a challenge?. In Boud, D. and Feletti,
G. (Eds). The challenge of problem-based-learning, London: Kogan Page, 36-44.
Putting PBL Into Practice: 213
Chapter 9
ABSTRACT
This study assessed the effect of advance organizers in music instruction. Four intact
beginning instrumental grade nine classes participated in the study. Two of the classes
were randomly assigned to receive the advance organizer treatment, whereas the other
two classes served as controls. Nine musical concepts were used as dependent variables:
instrumentation, counting, note values, slurring, key signature, meter, dynamics,
harmony, and dotted note. The treatment group and the control group were pre-tested and
post-tested on their knowledge of the musical concepts before and after the advance
organizer instruction was implemented. They were also tested on their performance and
listening skills based on the musical concepts following instruction. Findings indicate that
knowledge of three concepts - instrumentation, dynamics and harmony – was
significantly affected by advance organizer instruction. None of the performance or
listening skills based on the nine concepts were significantly affected by the advance
organizer instruction.
INTRODUCTION
1
An advance organizer is an instructional device designed to facilitate the learning and
retention of meaningful verbal information. Advance organizers are introduced prior to new
learning material and are presented at a higher level of abstraction, generality and
inclusiveness. They are usually based on the main concepts, principles or generalizations of a
1
The term ―facilitate‖ is used in this paper to refer to a deliberate manipulation designed to enhance learning
beyond what might otherwise occur if this technique were not used.
216 Bernard W. Andrews
subject or discipline. David Ausubel (1960, 1963) described two types of organizers: an
2
expository or abstract organizer and a comparative or concrete organizer. Ausubel and his
associates demonstrated that both types of organizers facilitate learning (Ausubel, 1960;
Ausubel & Fitzgerald, 1961, 1962; Ausubel & Youssef, 1963).
Abstract organizers describe a concept, principle or rule at a higher level of conceptual
organization (i.e., superordinate level). For example, beginning instrumental music students
often learn to play an instrument (subordinate level) without fully understanding its place
within the concert band structure. An abstract organizer on instrumentation would introduce
the concept of concert band as a performing ensemble comprised of groups of instruments,
called families, each with distinctive characteristics. Such an organizer would be presented to
the class verbally and/or through visual aids, video, internet or the demonstration of
instruments prior to students learning about their individual instruments. For example:
Many popular types of bands are comprised of groups of instruments. For example:
2
Initially, the terms expository and comparative were introduced which were gradually replaced by abstract and
concrete, respectively.
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 217
involves both knowledge of the discipline, and performing and listening skills. It can be a
rule, principle or generalization presented as a statement, didactic description, question,
demonstration, or even a musical recording. The form, then, is less important than that it be
clearly understood and be related to the material it is organizing.
The substantive element of meaningful learning allows for greater storage as the learner
retains only the substance of the ideas rather than the exact words in expressing them. Hence,
the concept of melody is easier to retain than its description – ‗a melody is a series of notes.‘
By analogy and transfer to aural cognition, it was speculated that the tonal memory of a
melody (i.e., its shape) would be easier to retain than each individual note when preceded by
an advance organizer; that is to say, a musical organizer could potentially improve listening
and also performance skills (Andrews, 1989). Consequently, an investigation of the effect of
advance organizers on the students' performing and listening skills was also undertaken.
METHODOLOGY
The primary question asked in the study was: "Does the treatment group acquire
significantly more knowledge about musical concepts than the control group after advance
organizer instruction is implemented?" In this study, advance organizers were developed
(after Suchman, 1972) for ten musical concepts and integrated into the curriculum (of the
treatment group but not the control group) for study. The selected concepts were based on
3
curricular materials for a beginning instrumental class : i) instrumentation (Inst); ii) counting
(Coun); iii) note values (NotV); iv) slurring (Slur); v) key signature (KeyS); vi) meter (Metr);
vii) dynamics (Dyns); viii) harmony (Harm); ix) dotted note (DotN); and x) form (refer to
Appendix 1). Since the treatment and control groups did not complete the post-test section on
4
‗form,‘ this concept was omitted. The secondary question was: ―Is there a significant
difference between the treatment group and the control group on performance and listening
skills based on the musical concepts?‖
In this study, the quasi-experimental method was adopted using the untreated control
group pre-test and post-test design (Cresswell, 2002) with multiple concepts (after Andrews,
2008). Four intact grade 7 instrumental music classes were selected, two of which were
randomly assigned to receive advance organizer instruction. The Treatment group (Trt n=48
comprised of Trt1 n=24 + Trt2 n=24) was taught using advanced organizers. The control
group (Con n=54 comprised of Con1 n=28 + Con2 n=26) was taught using the traditional
teacher-directed approach (i.e., teacher as conductor). All four beginning instrumental classes
were taught by the same music teacher who had five years of instrumental teaching
experience. She was also coached in the use of advance organizers.
The instruments for this study – knowledge, performing and listening tests – were
developed by the principal author, a teacher educator with 15 years of experience teaching
instrumental music in schools, in collaboration with the music teacher. Before
implementation of the study, the tests were reviewed by a panel of teachers with instrumental
teaching experience and researchers with expertise in learning theory and psychology to
3
The four music classes used Learning Unlimited which is published by Hal Leonard.
4
―Form‖ was not adequately taught as several students missed school due to a flu virus.
220 Bernard W. Andrews
confirm content validity. Subsequently, the tests were piloted for reliability with a sample of
beginning instrumental students prior to the commencement of the study (Andrews, 1994).
Both treatment and control groups were pre-tested and post-tested on their knowledge of
each of the musical concepts using a multiple-choice written test which measured
comprehension at superordinate, comparative and subordinate levels of cognitive
differentiation in congruence with advance organizer theory (refer to Appendix II). Multiple
choice tests have been used previously by researchers undertaking advance organizer studies
(e.g., Chung, 2002; Kreiner, 1996).
A post-test on performance and listening skills was also administered. It was not feasible
to do a pre-test on performance and listening skills as the students were beginners without
prior instrumental experience, and they were unfamiliar with the musical concepts. Five of
the concepts were assessed using a performance test (counting, slurring, key signature,
dynamics and dotted notes), and the other four concepts were assessed with a listening test
(instrumentation, note values, meter and harmony). The performance test pieces were selected
from the band method in use (i.e., Learning Unlimited), and accuracy was measured when
students correctly played rhythms (counting), slurs, accidentals (key signature), dynamics,
and dotted notes. The listening test was developed by the music teacher and refined by the
researcher, and accuracy was measured when students correctly recognized instrumentation,
note values, meter and harmony.
The means of the subsections covering each of the nine musical concepts on the
knowledge pre-test and post-test, and on the performance and listening post-tests, were
calculated for both treatment and control groups. Two Group membership (Treatment,
Control) by two Time (Pre, Post) split-plot Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) with repeated
measures on the second factor were conducted to assess the effects of advance organizer
instruction on knowledge of each of the nine concepts because it is the most generally used
model in dealing with pre-test and post-test data (Cook & Campbell, 1979). Independent t-
tests were conducted to compare the two groups on their performance and listening skills for
the nine concepts.
Because we conducted multiple ANOVAs and t-tests (9 of each), use of conventional
alpha levels for each test might yield an unacceptably high Type I error rate across all of the
tests. Therefore, to maintain the overall Type I error rate at 0.05, an additive (Bonferroni)
inequality was used to determine the alpha level for each test. For m tests, the alpha level for
each test is given by the overall alpha level divided by m (Huberty & Morris, 1989). Thus
0.05 / 9 = 0.006 was the significance level used for each ANOVA and t-test.
RESULTS
Primary Question
―Does the treatment group significantly acquire more knowledge about musical concepts
than the control group after the advance organizer instruction is implemented?"
The means for both pre-test and post-test knowledge of the nine concepts for both
treatment group and control group are presented in Table 1. As indicated, the means for the
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 221
post-test are greater than those for the pre-test for both treatment and control groups, with
instrumentation, slurring, dynamics, harmony and dotted note being more pronounced.
Indeed, the main effect of Time was statistically significant for all nine concepts, indicating
an overall increase in knowledge from pre-test to post-test.
The primary question of interest, however, is addressed by the Group membership by
Time interactions. If a given advance organizer is more effective than the traditional teacher-
directed approach, then the increase in knowledge test scores from pre to post should be
greater for the treatment than control group. Results indicate that only the Group membership
by Time interactions for the concepts of instrumentation, dynamics and harmony were
statistically significant. For these three concepts, means indicate greater acquisition of
knowledge (i.e., increase from pre to post) in the treatment than control group (see Figures 1
– 3). For all other concepts, knowledge acquisition was not significantly different between
the treatment and control groups (see, e.g., Figure 4).
Table 1: Means (and standard deviations) of knowledge pre and post-tests by concept
for treatment and control groups.
Treatment Control
(n=48) (n=54)
Concept Pre Post Pre Post
*Inst 3.167 (1.117) 4.354 (1.139) 3.346 (1.118) 3.500 (1.129)
Coun 3.812 (1.179) 4.667 (1.191) 3.615 (1.255) 4.942 (0.873)
NotV 3.562 (1.367) 4.937 (1.099) 3.462 (1.488) 5.173 (1.098)
Slur 2.872 (1.569) 5.447 (0.855) 2.462 (1.488) 4.942 (1.162)
KeyS 3.149 (1.503) 4.298 (1.397) 2.731 (1.374) 3.885 (1.215)
Metr 3.457 (1.187) 4.543 (0.780) 3.481 (0.980) 3.981 (0.828)
*Dyns 2.261 (1.405) 5.239 (0.947) 2.423 (1.194) 4.442 (1.364)
*Harm 3.609 (1.437) 5.543 (1.005) 3.788 (1.319) 4.885 (1.003)
DotN 2.696 (1.245) 5.522 (0.863) 2.615 (1.157) 4.654 (1.083)
* Significant Group by Time interaction, p < .006
Table 2: Means (and standard deviations) of Listening (L) and Performance (P) skills
post-tests for treatment group and control groups*.
Secondary Question
―Is there a significant difference between the treatment group and control group on
performance and listening tests based on the musical concepts?‖
For performance and listening, the means on the nine concepts for the treatment group
are quite similar to those for the control group (see Table 2). Independent t-tests revealed no
statistically significant differences between the two groups on performance and listening
skills for any of the nine musical concepts studied.
DISCUSSION
Referring to Table 1, for the concept of instrumentation the mean knowledge post-test
score (4.354) was much greater than the mean knowledge pre-test score (3.167) in the
treatment group, while hardly any change was found in the control group from the pre-test
(3.345) to post-test (3.500). This significant group membership by time interaction is
illustrated graphically in Figure 1.
For both the concepts of dynamics and harmony, the mean knowledge scores increased
from pre-test to post-test for both the treatment and control groups, but the increase in the
treatment group was more pronounced. Figures 2 and 3 illustrate the significant group
membership by time interactions for dynamics and harmony, respectively. Thus, while
students were able to acquire knowledge of dynamics and harmony without an advance
organizer, those students who used an advance organizer appear to have acquired knowledge
at a greater rate.
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 223
For the rest of the concepts, no group membership by time interactions were obtained.
Although knowledge of each concept increased for both treatment and control groups, it did
so at almost the same rate. Take the concept of slurring as an example. For the treatment
group, the mean post-test knowledge score increased by 2.575 (i.e., 5.447 - 2.872), and for the
control group, the mean post-test score increased by 2.480 (i.e., 4.942 - 2.462) (see Table 1).
Figure 4 shows this homogeneous change over time; that is, both treatment and control
groups changed the same amount from pre-test to post-test.
The untreated control group with pre-test and post-test is a frequently used design and is
often interpretable. However, as with other quasi-experimental designs, one can never be
entirely sure that attribution of a causal relationship, or lack thereof, between independent and
dependent variables is correct: there are several threats to internal validity that may be
224 Bernard W. Andrews
played with a reed and consequently are classified as woodwinds. In this context, the advance
organizer offers the possibility of providing a framework for understanding the organization
of the instruments based on acoustical properties and historical usage.
The teaching of dynamics is a concept of moderate difficulty which requires an
understanding of contrasts (loud - soft) and gradations of sound (soft to loud; loud to soft),
and which requires an ability to translate these concepts into instrumental performance. This
process involves cognitive (recognizing the dynamic symbols), affective (interpreting
dynamic symbols within a particular context), and psychomotor (performing the passage
delineated by dynamic symbols) functions. A knowledge of dynamics is essential to the
effective musical interpretation of music. Consequently, advance organizer instruction could
potentially lead to improved musical interpretations by young instrumentalists.
Of the three concepts where a significant effect of advance organizer instruction was
found, teaching harmony is the most problematic for teachers of beginning instrumentalists.
Instrumentalists can "hear" harmony, "feel" harmony, "see" harmony (e.g., multiple parts in
sheet music and musical scores), but they cannot "hold" it like a musical instrument. Unlike
melody which is akin to speech, harmony has no equivalency in language. We read and speak
words and sentences, thereby expressing our thoughts sequentially in a linear fashion.
Similarly, students read and play notes and melodies, thereby expressing musical ideas which
move horizontally through time. For this reason, the concept of melody is grasped relatively
easily by young students. In contrast to melody, harmony is a vertical concept that involves
several notes played together and moving forward coherently (i.e., harmonic progression).
Young people simply do not have a frame of reference for harmony as they do for melody
(i.e., language). However, it is through the development of harmony that architectonic forms,
such as the symphony, concerto, tone poem and music drama, were brought to fruition. For
this reason, the study of harmony in the music curriculum is essential to understanding our
Western European heritage. Advance organizer instruction offers a unique opportunity for
teachers to help students understand this concept.
Knowledge of the three concepts – instrumentation, dynamics and harmony - that was
significantly affected by advance organizer instruction are common to students‘ lived
experiences. Young people see and hear musical instruments and listen to dynamics and
harmony on a regular basis; for example, when watching music videos, listening to their
IPod‘s or MP3 players, attending concerts, and when they are of age, going to nightclubs.
These three concepts require neither score reading ability nor performance ability for the
concepts to be understood; that is, they do not have to read music or play an instrument to
acquire knowledge of instrumentation, dynamics or harmony. Indeed harmony, the more
complex of the three, is intrinsic to human physiology.
Many young people are tuned-in to their IPods or MP3 players throughout a good part the
day and often tuned-out to what is going on around them, much to the annoyance of their
teachers and parents. Their evenings are often taken up with video games, complete with
music sound tracks, on an array of devices - computers, television, X Box, Sony Play Station,
226 Bernard W. Andrews
and now cell phones. In the music youth hear, dynamics and instrumentation support
harmony by intensifying and colouring listeners‘ responses, thereby keeping them involved in
the musical experience and extra-musical experience. Consequently, it appears to the observer
that young people‘s ears are ―glued‖ to their IPods and their eyes are ―glued‖ to video games.
In support of harmonic intent, dynamics further alerts the listener to what is in store, as
exemplified by the music of ‗horror‘ films, for instance. Dynamics can drive the music,
determine where it is heading, and thus contribute to the overall form of the work. Increases
and decreases in volume, supported by certain instrumental timbres, evoke entrained
responses from the listener. (Schneck & Berger, 2006, p. 237)
Why weren‘t knowledge of the other seven concepts – counting, note values, slurring,
key signature, meter and dotted note – significantly affected by advance organizer
instruction? All of these concepts represent new knowledge for most beginning instrumental
students (except for those few enrolled in private music lessons), and each requires an ability
to read music. Furthermore, all of these concepts are physically operationalized by playing a
musical instrument. For example, a student recognizes a curved line in music notation that
connects two notes as a slur, and then he or she plays the second of the two notes without
tonguing it. As the participants in the study were beginning instrumentalists, it is likely that
the inability to read music and play an instrument impacted on their learning of the seven
concepts. Alternatively, the concepts represent essential procedural knowledge for beginners
and consequently may not be amenable to advance organizer instruction. Acquisition of more
advanced knowledge of the seven concepts could be examined with advance organizers in the
second year of an instrumental program, when more complex note values, slurring patterns,
etc. are introduced.
The lack of significance of the impact of advance organizer instruction on knowledge of
meter may appear surprising when one considers that young people consistently experience
rhythm, alongside dynamics and harmony, on a regular basis. However, rhythm is a complex
concept involving the elements of beat (pulse of the music), meter (arrangement of strong and
weak beats), and pattern (duration of notes in a melody or chords in a progression). Meter is a
higher-order element than beat. Almost all popular music is written in 4/4 time; that is, a
series of four beats with a pattern of accents - strong-weak-medium-weak - which is
constantly repeated throughout the song. The meters introduced to beginning instrumentalists
in their first year of study included 2/4 and 3/4 which have recurring beat patterns of strong-
weak and strong-weak-weak, respectively. These meters are most commonly used in the
march (2/4) and the waltz (3/4). Since marches and waltzes are seldom experienced by
students outside of a school music program, it could explain why knowledge of the concept of
meter was not significantly improved through advance organizer instruction.
CONCLUDING COMMENTS
This study indicates that the advance organizer is an effective instructional method for
the teaching of instrumentation, dynamics and harmony to beginning instrumental music
students. The reason that no significance was found on the performance and listening skills
may be that the advance organizer places more emphasis on conceptual knowledge while
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 227
beginning band performance emphasizes the development of playing skills. Perhaps more
experienced instrumentalists could benefit from a conceptual framework in refining their
performance skills. However, due to the limitations of the quasi-experimental design used
here, further study is required to provide supporting evidence for the results shown by this
study. Additional research is also recommended to explore the use of advance organizers with
more experienced instrumentalists to examine the viability of advance organizers for
improving performance skill and interpretation.
REFERENCES
Andrews, B. W. (1989). The advance organizer in music instruction: An antidote for
mechanistic rote learning? Canadian Journal of Research in Music Education, 31 (1), 5-
11.
Andrews, B. W. (1991). Be prepared: Organize in advance! Canadian Music Educator, 33
(2), 20-21.
Andrews, B. W. (1994). Advance organizers in music instruction: Implications for
performance and listening skill acquisition. Reporting Classroom Research, 23 (3), 3.
Andrews, B. W. (1995). Advance organizers re-visited. C.M.E. Newsletter, 93, 2-3.
Andrews, B. W. (2008). Integrated Inquiry: Transforming research perspectives. In S.
Kouritzin, N. Piquemal, & R. Norman (Eds.), Qualitative research: Challenging the
orthodoxies in standard academic discourse(s) (pp. 169-181). New York, NY: Taylor
and Francis.
Ausubel, D. P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of
meaningful verbal material. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51 (5), 267-272.
Ausubel, D. P. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning. New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Ausubel, D. P., & Fitzgerald, D. (1961). The role of discriminability in meaningful learning
and retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 52 (5), 266-274.
Ausubel, D. P., & Fitzgerald, D. (1962). Organizer, general background, and antecedent
learning variables in sequential verbal learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 53
(6), 243-249.
Ausubel, D. P., & Youssef, M. (1963). Role of discriminability in meaningful parallel
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54 (6), 331-336.
Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D., & Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational psychology: A cognitive
view. (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Cames, E. R., Lindbeck, J. S., & Griffin, C. F. (1987). Effects of group size and advance
organizers on learning parameters when using computer tutorials in kinematics. Journal
of Research in Science Teaching, 24 (9), 781-789.
Callison, D. (2000). Key words in instruction: Organizers. School Library Media Activities
Monthly, 16 (5), 36-39.
Chung, J. M. (1999). The effects of using video texts supported with advance organizers and
captions on Chinese college students‘ listening comprehension: An empirical study.
Foreign Language Annals, 32 (3), 295-308.
228 Bernard W. Andrews
Chung, J. M. (2002). The effects of using two advance organizers with video texts for the
teaching of listening in English. Foreign Language Annals, 35 (2), 231-241.
Chung, J. M., & Huang, S. C. (1998). The effects of three aural advance organizers for video
viewing in the foreign language classroom. System, 26 (4), 553-65.
Chung, J. M., & Plass, J. L. (1996). Facilitating reading comprehension with multimedia.
System, 24 (4), 503-19.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation design and analysis issues for
field settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Corkhill, A. J., Bruning, R. H., & Glover, J. A. (1988). Advance organizers: Concrete versus
abstract. Journal of Educational Research, 82 (2), 76-81.
Cresswell, J. (2002). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative and mixed method
approaches. (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gordon, E. (1971). The psychology of music teaching. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Hanley, J. E., Herron, C. A., & Cole, S. P. (1995). Using video as an advance organizer to a
written passage in the FSL classroom. Modern Language Journal, 79, 57-66.
Herron, C. A. (1994). An investigation of the effectiveness of using an advance organizer to
introduce video in foreign language classrooms. Modern Language Journal, 78, 190-198.
Herron, C. A., York, H, Cole, S.P., & Linden, P. (1998). A comparison study of student
retention of foreign language video: Declarative versus interrogative advance organizer.
Modern Language Journal, 82, 237-247.
Huberty, C. J., & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus multiple univariate
analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 105 (2), 302-308.
Kreiner, D. S. (1996). Effects of advance questions on reading comprehension. Journal of
General Psychology, 123 (4), 352-364.
Langan-Fox, J., Waycott, J. L., & Albert, K. (2000). Linear and graphic advance organizers:
Properties and processing. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 4 (1), 19-34.
Lin, L., & Chen, T. (2006). Decreasing cognitive load for novice EFL learners: Effects of
question and descriptive advance organizers in facilitating EFL learners‘ comprehension
of an animation-based content lesson. System, 34, 416-431.
Luiten, J., Ames, W., & Ackerson, G. (1980). A meta-analysis of the effects of advance
organizers on learning and retention. American Educational Research Journal, 17 (2),
211-218.
O‘Bannon, B., Puckett, K., & Rakes, G. (2006). Using technology to support visual learning
strategies. Computers in the Schools, 23 (1/2), 125-137.
Schneck, D. J., & Berger, D. S. (2006). The music effect: Music physiology and clinical
applications. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Sidnell, R. (1973). Building instructional programs in music education. Englewood Cliffs,
N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Sledge, L. J. (1971). The application of selected writings by Gagné, Bruner and Ausubel to
music theory pedagogy. Ph.D. thesis, Florida State University.
Stone, C. L. (1984). A Meta-analysis of advance organizer studies. Journal of Experimental
Education, 51 (4), 194-199.
Suchman, R. J. (1972). A model for the analysis of inquiry. In H.J. Klausmeir, B. Joyce & M.
Weil (Eds.), Models of teaching. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 229
APPENDIX 1
Superordinate Level:
Comparative Level:
Many popular groups are also comprised of groups of instruments.
Subordinate Level:
Each instrument in a concert band belongs to the brass, woodwind or percussion families.
Your clarinet is made of wood, produces sound by vibrating a reed and produces different
notes by depressing keys. It is a woodwind. Your trumpet is made of brass alloy, etc.
2. Counting
Superordinate Level:
Music in our culture is measured, which means that we can count it. Most of our music is
in 4/4 time, which means there are four beats in each bar and each quarter note gets one beat.
We measure each bar by counting out four beats.
Comparative Level:
Counting music is similar to counting our money. Four quarters (25 cent pieces) equals
one loonie. If we have several quarters and wanted to know how many dollars we have, we
measure them into separate piles. To achieve this we would count
1 2 3 4 | 2 2 3 4 | 3 2 3 4 | 4 2 3 4 , etc.
Subordinate Level:
When we count notes in a bar we write the number of the beat under the note (i.e., 1 2 3
4). If the note receives more than one beat, we correct the numbers with a line (i.e., a half note
is counted 1-2 and a whole 1-2-3-4). If a note receives less than one beat, we use "&" (e.g., 2
eighths are counted 1 &).
3. Note Values
Superordinate Level:
Musical symbols, called notes, are used by musicians to represent sounds. The duration
of different sounds, from long to short, is represented by whole, half, quarter and eighth notes.
The relationship between different notes is a ratio of 2:1; that is, two eight notes are equal in
value to a quarter note, two quarter notes are equal to a half note, etc.
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 231
Comparative Level:
Money also has a ratio of 2:1. For example, two quarters (25 cent pieces) have the same
value as a 50 cent coin. Similarly, two quarter notes have the same value as a half note.
Subordinate Level:
Two eight notes are equal in value to a quarter note. We can feel eighth notes by running
and quarter notes by walking on the spot. Try saying ―alligator hungry‖ while running and
walking on the spot, and you will understand the relationship of eighth notes to quarter notes
(i.e., the 2:1 ratio).
4. Slurring
Superordinate Level:
Slurring notes is a way of connecting sounds so that a smooth, flowing line is possible.
Comparative Level:
Tonguing notes is like walking. Slurring notes is like skating. Staccato is like hopping.
Subordinate Level:
A slur is a curved line that connects two notes of different pitches. Tongue the first note
and move to the second note without tonguing. Keep the air moving.
5. Key Signature
Superordinate Level:
The key signature is the sign post at the beginning of the piece of music. It is located
beside the time signature and consists of one or more sharps, or one or more flats. It tells you
that certain notes must be altered when you play them.
Comparative Level:
A key signature is like a detour. When you travel with your parents and you encounter
construction, you must follow the signs and take an alternate route.
Subordinate Level:
A key signature consists of sharps or flats. The sharp indicates that a note must be raised
a semi-tone; the flat indicates that the note must be lowered a semi-tone.
6. Meter
Superordinate Level:
Meter is the grouping of beats in music. You are familiar with 4/4 time. Music can also
be written in 3/4 time where there are 3 beats to a bar and each quarter note receives 1 beat.
232 Bernard W. Andrews
Comparative Level:
Many things around us are organized with fours; for example, four wheels on a car, four
corners in a room, four stoplights at an intersection. Can you think of some items that are
organized in three's (e.g., men's three-piece suite, triangle, etc.)?
Subordinate Level:
3/4 meter consists of a strong beat and two weak beats. The feel of 3/4 is quite different
than 4/4 as it is lilting and flowing. The most common piece in 3/4 time is the waltz.
7. Dynamics
Superordinate Level:
Dynamics are the volume levels that musicians employ to convey their interpretation of a
piece. It is essential that all the members of a group pay attention to the dynamic markings so
that the appropriate musical effect is achieved.
Comparative Level:
Have you ever played with the volume dial on your radio? What is the effect on your
parents when you turn it up loud? Why is music in restaurants turned down so low?
Subordinate Level:
Dynamic markings in a piece tell us how loud or soft to play, or gradations in-between.
Forte which is written ƒ means to play loud with a full sound. Piano which is written p means
to play with a soft sound. Between the two extremes there is mƒ and mp: mezzo forte for
moderately loud and mezzo piano for moderately soft.
8. Harmony
Superordinate Level:
Harmony is created when two or more parts sound together in a pleasing manner.
Comparative Level:
Listen very carefully to the sounds around you. Close your eyes and listen to your
heartbeat, your breathing, the sounds in your classroom, and the sounds in the hall. Listen to
how all these sounds blend to forma soundscape to our everyday life.
Subordinate Level:
In music, one of the earliest types of harmony was called a round. When we play
different parts of the song all together, it sound pleasing and we obtain harmony. We can
achieve this by dividing the piece into four parts. Each part will start the song in sequence
after the previous part plays the opening bar. By playing the round in this way, we create
harmony.
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 233
9. Dotted Note
Superordinate Level:
A dot increases the value of a note by half its value. For example, if a quarter note
receives one beat, a dotted quarter note will receive a value of 1 + ½ beat.
Comparative Level:
In the military, a stripes and half stripes on your shoulder indicates that you are an
officer. The addition of a half stripe denotes a higher rank: one stripe for a second lieutenant;
one and a half stripes for a first lieutenant; two stripes for a captain; two and a half stripes for
a major, etc. With each half stripe, one also increases the value of one's pay by a half.
Consequently if the second lieutenant's salary is $20,000, the first lieutenant‘s salary would
be $30,000, etc.
Subordinate Level:
A dotted quarter note is equal to the value of a quarter note (1) + an eighth note (½) tied
together, or alternately, three eighth notes tied together (½ + ½ + ½ ). A quarter note is
counted 1-&-2 and is written with another eighth note or rest added to complete the second
beat, that is 1-&-2 &.
Superordinate Level:
Musical form is identified by contrasting sections which are labelled with capital letters
(e.g. A, B, C, etc.).
The simplest form ‗A‘ is one section and is best exemplified by a nursery rhyme or a one-
verse song, such as Frère Jacques. If there are two sections, the second section is labelled ‗B,‘
and it is labelled a two-part form, that is ‗AB.‘
Comparative Level:
Many things around us are organized into parts. For example, a house has a roof, a main
floor, and a basement; and our classroom consists of a floor, ceiling and walls. Can you think
of some more items with different parts? Can you think of something where two of the parts
are the same?
Subordinate Level:
‗ABA‘ form was created when the first section ‗A‘ was followed by a different section
‗B,‘ and then ‗A‘ was repeated. Composers initially used the term Da Capo a Fine (which
means to go back to beginning and play section ‗A‘) to lengthen a musical work. Also, it
saved them the work of re-writing all of section ‗A‘ again.
234 Bernard W. Andrews
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 235
236 Bernard W. Andrews
Back to the Future: The Effect of Advance Organizers in Music Instruction 237
238 Bernard W. Andrews
In: Recent Trends in Education ISBN 978-1-60741-795-8
Editor: Borislav Kuzmanović and Adelina Cuevas © 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Chapter 10
ABSTRACT
The field of Cognitive Science is dispelling some of the classical myths or
misunderstandings about how the brain learns, how brain development may be impacting
language acquisition and cognition. This is an exciting time to be an educator and to see
the incredible possibilities for the latest theories of how the brain develops and evolves.
No longer are we saddled with the idea that the brain is ‗fixed‘ by the time a person
reaches the age of three, that we must throw our hands up in despair if a child comes to
school not ready to learn. Through the most recent research, we know that our mind can
be enriched, expanded and developed toward several levels of sophistication. This
chapter draws upon the marriage of the latest brain based research and technologies as
they provide implications for early detection of cognition and learning difficulties. This
new direction in knowledge about the development of the brain has great potential for the
education of our children and the stability of our nation as a global competitor.
INTRODUCTION
In 1700 BC, Egyptians tinkered with the brain and were involved in brain research. Each
century since then has brought more information about how the brain functions with
advancing brain research. Theories have come and gone. Some were correct, other were not.
With the advancement of technology and more defined research, scientists and educators are
able to rethink and redefine brain research and its implications to educating children. This
chapter will explain briefly how the brain supports learning from a historical to a current
basis. Brain development as it relates to language and cognition will be examined by the
theories of Piaget, Bloom, Gardner, Siegler, and Sousa.
240 Leslie Wasserman
There are many theories in education as well as many theorists who have developed
theories only to have them disproved by other theorists. This is the way of education. As
more and more knowledge is gained, theories change. Interesting enough, several theorists
were correct in their time only to have been dismissed by the general thinking of their era.
Upon further investigation it is discovered that these theorists were really onto something and
were correct in their thinking. Piaget‘s theories were discounted before they were accepted
years later by the American educational system. This chapter will discuss theories throughout
history in the last century that has made an impact in early childhood education and language
acquisition.
understanding that the human mind was much more complex and intricate than previously
thought.
The theories of this time labeled people as right-brained and left-brained learners. Based
on this theory, the people who were considered to be left brained were much more logical,
rational, analytical, specific, and objective whereas the people who were considered to be
right-brained were much more random, intuitive, holistic, subjective and general.
During the 1990s, brain research exploded and educators were able to relate the
implications of the research to the curriculum that they taught to enable the students to learn
better. Constructivists had strongly influenced education and had caused a paradigm shift
with assessments. Language arts (Bruner, 1986), mathematics (Schifter, 1996), and science
(Yager, 1991) were also a part of the shift due to the hands-on, student centered,
constructivist style learning that is being taught in schools (Gibson and McKay, 2006).
Constructivists believe that people develop based on knowledge gained by social and physical
interactions with others and their environment (Piaget, 1956). Another constructivist,
Vygotsky, believed that knowledge is gained through interaction with social and cultural
interactions. The constructivist theories are basically about knowledge and the nature of it.
brain plan is both innate and localized. Man is the only species that is able to acquire
language which means language is innate.
The infants‘ brain is highly plastic. This means that it can be molded (Bates, 1999). Even
with a brain injury, a baby has the ability to bounce back and relearn much of what is
necessary to function properly (Bates). Bates conducted research about the infant brain and
language development. Bates‘ theory about how the brain organizes itself for language is as
follows:
1. Linguistic knowledge is not localized in a clear and compact form in either the infant
or the adult brain.
2. Left hemisphere injuries are not associated with aphasia if those injuries occur early
in life. Instead, the infant brain is highly plastic, permitting alternative ―brain plans‖
for language to emerge if the standard situation does not hold.
3. However, the infant brain is not a tabula rasa; it is already highly differentiated at
birth, and certain regions are biased from the beginning toward modes of information
processing those are particularly useful for language, leading (in the absence of focal
injury) to the standard form of brain organization for language.
4. The processing biases that lead to the ―standard brain plan‖ may be both innate and
localized, in both infants and adults, but they are not specific to language (p. 199).
According to Sousa (2006) there is still a debate whether educators should use the brain
research to their advantage in the classroom. This should not prevent educators from using
their new found knowledge and putting it toward enhancing the students‘ learning. Sousa
suggests that recent discoveries from brain research have yielded these results:
The human brain is constantly reorganizing itself with each new input of knowledge.
Revealed more about how the brain acquires spoken language.
Developed scientifically based computer programs that dramatically help young
children with reading problems.
Shown how emotions affect learning, memory, and recall (p. 5).
This new information that is being gathered about brain research can have an impact on
education and learning as more facts are uncovered with further testing.
Research has shown that the brains of girls and boys organize differently from very early
on and continues until their formative years that lead to different preferences for learning
(Sousa, 2006). More girls than boys are left-hemisphered. The left-hemisphere is wired for
empathy and the right-hemisphere is wired for understanding and building systems (Sousa).
This statement is not to be seen as a stereotype since it is not the case exclusively.
As the century continues, more brain research will bring about new and improved
information that can be used to make education and learning better. Is it possible that we can
even be able to pinpoint difficulties to the point of being able to fix them and see a decrease
of disabilities in students? One cannot know the range of knowledge gained from studies and
research but one would like to believe that it could be possible.
Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 243
1. Individual sounds.
2. Syllables.
3. Simple words often doubled syllables like ―dada‖. This is when the child first is said
to speak, because the sound he produces communicates an idea.
4. Understanding and saying words that are the names of the objects (nouns).
5. Understanding and saying words that refer to qualities of objects named (adjectives).
6. Understanding and saying words that refer to the relationship of objects named.
7. Explosion into language (verbs and the exact from of nouns and adjectives, including
prefixes and suffixes).
8. The forms for present, past, and future tenses of verbs, use of the pronoun as a word
that ―stands in place of‖ a name.
9. Construction of sentences with mutually dependent parts (p. 57).
Siegler (1978) believed that children‘s spoken language is based on four subtests
throughout the ―preoperational period‖ that tend to be representational rather than
transformational.
Even though children pass through these stages, it is their environment and specific
experiences that affect each child‘s development. The child‘s short term memory regulates
the speed at which the child‘s sequencing progresses. It has been discovered that ―memory
grows from one unit at age one to two to five units at age four to five years‖ (Siegler, 1978, p.
54).
The predetermined theory is a theoretical based position that language acquisition is
innate. The human brain is believed to be wired to master any language. This is the position
that brain based researchers have come to believe and have proved to be correct through their
many studies and research.
Recent research has indicated that spoken language is far more complex than thought
before. The Broca‘s area and Wernicke‘s area are responsible for spoken language as well as
several other neural networks that are scattered throughout the left hemisphere (Sousa, 2006).
Although the infant‘s brain can respond to many sounds, only the ones that are repeated will
be remembered. By the age of twelve months, the brain can distinguish the infant‘s native
language and ignores foreign sounds (Sousa, 2006). ―Babies begin to distinguish word
boundaries by the age of eight months‖ with words such as ―green house and greenhouse‖
even though people do not pause when speaking (Sousa, 2006, 181).
Boliek and Lohmeier (1999) wrote of a study conducted on infants by Dr. Andrew
Meltzoff to determine if Piaget‘s theory of development was correct. Meltzoff found that
babies at least twelve days old were able to imitate mouth movements of adults (Boliek and
Lohmeier). According to Piaget‘s theory, this does not take place until about one year of age.
The infants were able to self-correct the movement to match that of the adult and so Meltzoff
declared the lip and tongue protrusions and mouth movements to not be reflexive (Boliek and
Lohmeier).
According to Boliek and Lohmeier (1999), Meltzoff also disproved another of Piaget‘s
theories based on his study. He disproved Piaget‘s sensorimotor stage. Piaget has three stages
of sensorimotor development: primary, secondary, and tertiary. Meltzoff found that infants
intentionally explored their surroundings and were able to adapt to different conditions. This
theory challenges Piaget‘s theory that infant behavior is reactionary. Meltzoff‘s theory is
much more of a multimodality approach to learning (Boliek and Lohmeier, 1999). Meltzoff
believes the development of linguistic skills in young children can affect not only respiration
function, but also the overall verbal output.
Language is broken into two categories: words and grammar. These two components are
developed at different times and in different areas of the brain. Phase shifts (system
instability) might explain the observed increase in speech dysfluencies during a period of
rapid growth (Boliek and Lohmeier, 1999). According to the Boliek and Lohmeier, it is this
dynamic systems theory that holds the answers for much of the development that takes place
during infancy. In Eliot‘s (2001) research she determined that people are able to speak seven
hundred speech sounds in one minute of normal speech. It is during this development that the
plasticity of the brain continues. This learning window could be an excellent opportunity to
teach the child a second language since the brain is already wired for language acquisition.
The brain of a baby comes prepared to learn language. The babies are able to recognize
their mother‘s voice while they are inside the womb (Montanaro, 2001). As early as nine
Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 245
months of age, a child is able to understand sound patterns. As the baby is listening, the brain
is mapping the different sounds into categories (Cohen, 1999). It has been found that the
language center in the brain shows response when people talk to the infant beginning at birth
(Montanaro, 2001). The lack of interaction can cause delays or even mutism as the child
grows.
The window of opportunity for language is from birth until age ten (Slegers, 1997).
Montanaro (2001) states that there are two different stages involved in language acquisition:
prelanguage and linguistic. The first stage is prelanguage that begins before birth and lasts
until the age of ten or twelve months. Montanaro (2001) calls this period ―silence‖ (p. 2)
when babies are taking in everything they hear. The second stage is the linguistic stage that is
from the ages of twelve to thirty-six months. By the time a child is three years old 97% of
children are able to connect 2-3 words to form phrases and simple sentences (Slegers).
Montanaro states that it is this period that is a ―sensitive period for naming things‖ (p. 2). It is
important for the child to learn the correct terminology of words.
Children are born with equal sides of the brain (Slegers, 1997). The right side develops
first and grows faster. This is the side of the brain that deals with emotion. The left side of the
brain starts to grow later and is in charge of new learning (Slegers). The left hemisphere of
the brain has the information processing center that plays an important part in the
development of language skills (Jensen, 2005). The brain when viewed with an fMRI shows
that children who have normal language skills have lop sided brains (Slegers). That being the
case, it is not surprising that the children with language disorders have brain sides of equal
size. Both sides of the brain can work independently and can work together.
Between birth and the age of five, a child‘s experience aids in the development of
language skills in the brain (Jensen, 2005). Beginning around 19 months until around 31
months, the child will have a learning explosion of the most language thus far in his
development (Jensen). Jenson (2005) states that development of language can be described in
this manner:
1. Hearing it. The more words a child hears, the better. The highly fluctuating tonality
of ‗parentese‘ is helpful from birth to 12 months.
2. Speaking it. The more a child speaks, the better.
3. Hearing parents speak normally. Normal ‗grown up‘ talk is beneficial for babies at
any time after 6 month ( p. 24).
The function of the left hemisphere of the brain includes verbal functioning such as
talking, reading, writing, and understanding the speech of other people (Pink, 2006). The left
hemisphere is sequential and the right hemisphere is simultaneous (Pink). Pink describes it
this way that ―the right hemisphere is the picture, and the left hemisphere is the thousand
words‖ (p. 19). The left side handles what is said and the right side identifies how it is said
such as the facial expressions, eye contact, gesture, and intonation (Pink; Gardner, 2006).
With both sides of the brain functioning properly, the listener is able to understand the full
meaning of what is being said.
246 Leslie Wasserman
As the years passed, teachers continued to follow this easy to understand method to
engage their students in higher level thinking skills. In 2001, the taxonomy was revised using
the same six levels but changed the wording of three levels to verb forms and interchanged
the top two levels (See Figure 2). The revised taxonomy suggests a more open model (See
Figure 2). This loosening of the hierarchy was created due to what has been learned by brain
research that different areas of thinking can be used to solve the different parts of the problem
(Sousa, 2006).
It is important to provide extensive practice of the lower level skills so that the students
become comfortable with the questioning techniques and can master the techniques in order
to be able to move up to the other levels (Bloom, 1981). Apply the concepts when instructing
the children by having them process information in terms of the verbs such as analyze,
evaluate, and create to help them have a deeper understanding of the material that they can
attach meaning to it for retention (See Figure 2).
CLASSROOM TECHNIQUES
The teacher‘s knowledge about how the brain functions and learns will influence how the
material is taught. Each individual learns differently. It is up to the teacher to find out what
type of learner each student is and to teach the students accordingly. This is not to say that the
teacher should only teach in a hands-on manner if most of the class is kinesthetic learners, or
to teach using lecture for the auditory learners, or to show visual representation for the visual
Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 249
learners, this means that the teacher should have the flexibility to teach using a variety of
teaching styles (Sternberg, 2004; Gardner, 2006). The teacher can give the students a Multiple
Intelligence Inventory to find out what type of learners each student is to determine the type
of teaching styles to be used in the classroom (Gardner, 2006). Flexibility is important for
both the teacher and the students. Students also need to learn how to do things in new ways
and leave their comfort zone of their learning style in order to grow educationally. This
should be done occasionally to help the student develop their learning style. This type of
teaching may be different for some classroom teachers who believe that lecture is the most
effective way to teach and it may be difficult for these teachers to teach outside the box by
using varied teaching styles (Sternberg, 2004).
Assessment of the students can be a different issue with varying learning styles. Students
should be able to be assessed within their learning style. Teachers should be able to give
assessments that truly measure what the student has learned. A visual student may make a
PowerPoint, or a kinesthetic learner a diorama or something that he can make with his hands.
Some students may prefer the traditional aspect of assessment with a research paper. There
are so many ways the teacher can assess the students, from individual work to group work
such as: reenactments of the book, poster presentations, creating a movie, writing their own
story, creating an art portfolio, or any other kind of project that best meets the student‘s
learning style. By doing this, the teacher is able to see what the student has learned about the
topic. It is important for the teacher to let the students choose the activity for the assessment
within boundaries of the assignment.
Although cooperative learning groups are good for some students who may be less
extroverted, it can be difficult for the gifted student. Gifted students in cooperative learning
groups many times are the ones who are teaching the others in the group and are not being
challenged and are not learning themselves (Sternberg, 2004). Knowing this, the teacher
needs to be aware of what type of project the student chooses so that the assessment is not one
that is too easy or a too difficult assignment, or one that will not be successful for the student.
It is the teachers‘ responsibility to help the students find ways that best meet their learning
styles and best meet their individual needs for learning.
this practice for their school systems and a few states have joined in, as well. Reform is
needed to increase literacy skills of the very young and to be sure that each child is entitled to
a free education.
In the long run, having a free preschool program is less expensive to the school district
since it addresses many of the learning issues at a young age with intervention. If the students
do not receive intervention at an early age, they are more likely to have to receive special
services throughout their school years which can be a longer lasting expense to the school
district.
CONCLUSION
There are many different theorists who have theories that have stood the test of time:
Piaget, Bloom, Gardner, and Siegler. Sousa has made major contributions in the area of brain
development and research, especially for educators. Sousa writes about the newest studies and
research and compiles it in such a manner that it is user friendly and informative. By learning
how the brain learns, educators are able to understand how best to teach the children along
with understanding the child‘s developmental level the child is physically, mentally, socially,
and cognitively. The more knowledge an educator has about brain development, the better
able the educator is able to acquire and apply the knowledge, the better they can instruct the
children. New theories about language acquisition should be explored by the educators to
make the most of learning ―windows‖. This impact will increase the children‘s retention and
processing of language and give them success with language.
Brain functioning is interesting and amazing research. It is capable of so many different
skills that it is hard to know exactly what else the brain is capable of doing. Each brain is
unique and the potential for learning is enormous. If educators use the windows of
opportunity for learning, students will be capable of so much more processing and retaining
information than was once imagined. It is up to the educator to make the most of it for our
future‘s sake.
REFERENCES
Bates, E. (1999). Language and the infant brain. A companion to cognitive science, 32, 195-
205.
Bloom, B.S. (1981). All our children learning: A primer for parents, teachers, and other
educators. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Boliek, C.A. and Lohmeier, H. (1999). From the big bang to the brain, Journal of
Communication Disorders, 32, 4, 271-276.
Bruner, J. (1960). The Process of Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cohen, D.L. (1999). Good beginnings for all children: From brain research to action,
Elementary and Early Childhood Education, 3, 1-35.
Costelloe, M.J. (1967) (trans.). Maria Montessori‘s the discovery of the child. Notre Dame,
IN: Ballantine Books.
Eliot, L. (2001). Language and the developing brain, NAMTA Journal, 26, 2, 8-60.
Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 251
Chapter 11
ABSTRACT
This chapter is about the 'connectedness' of different ways of presenting health
information. Information for individual patients can be tailored via 'user-models' which
may include the patient's own medical record. There is evidence that tailoring
information in this way can make it more relevant and useable for patients and their
families. For example, in rigorous randoamised trials we have shown that tailored
information is more likely to be shown by cancer patients to their confidants. There may
be impact on psychological wellbeing, but this has been more difficult to show.
Traditional e-learning for students has well known advantages such as convenience
of time and being able to work at the student's own pace and review material in different
ways. However, the reduced 'connection' with the lecturer and other students may mean
that students are less motivated to engage with the material. We have been developing
simple ways of delivering live webcasting with a simultaneous chat room. User reaction
from students has been favourable but there are many practical aspects to improve.
Although there is evidence that computers can be used successfully for psychological
therapy and patient education approaches to this could be improved by inclusion of live
webcasting. For example, computerised cognitive behavioural therapy has proved
successful for anxiety and depression but there is also some evidence that this approach is
less successful if there is no human contact. A combination of live webcasting to provide
human contact and motivation together with tailored information education may be the
best way forward.
However, there are many unanswered questions about the use of these technologies
particularly amongst new user groups. For example, how does the use of asynchronous
methods such as email differ from synchronous methods such as chat rooms. Chat rooms
*
A version of this chapter was also published in E-Learning: 21st Century Issues and Challenges, edited by
Audrey R. Lipshitz and Steven P. Parsons, published by Nova Science Publishers, Inc. It was submitted by
appropriate modification in an effort to encourage wider dissemination of research.
254 Leslie Wasserman
may provide more ‗connection‘ but perhaps users need time to think. We will be
exploring such issues with groups of older people.
INTRODUCTION
Information for patients comes in many formats. Most patients get the majority of their
information face-to-face from health professionals. However, information from TV, the press,
books, leaflets, and of course the Internet plays an increasing role. Information may be
‗pushed‘ by professionals – in the form of (for example) an anti-smoking or safe sex
campaign, or may be ‗pulled‘ by patients seeking more information about their condition.
Information may be sought to help in making a joint decision with the health care provider, or
may be sought simply for ‗peace of mind‘. Information can be targeted at particular groups by
the language and imagery used and by the medium and timing of the output. Health promoters
use the services of marketing experts to help develop images and phraseology suitable for
their target audiences. Charities and other providers of condition specific information develop
and test their booklets, leaflets, and web pages with focus groups and interviews amongst the
target populations. However, information cannot only be targeted at particular groups but also
tailored to the individual patient. This chapter is about studies that have sought to take that
further step from targeting a particular group of patients to tailoring the information for
individuals.
Tailoring is just one way of trying to ‗connect‘ better with the user. Another way is
through the use of synchronous methods such as webcasting and in this chapter we describe
our experience of live webcasting with simultaneous chat room both for students and for
patients.
their confidants confirmed that patients were more likely to show booklets including, and
tailored by, personal information, to their confidants.
This approach of giving patients access to their own medical record and using the
information to tailor further explanation about their condition can be extended to virtually all
groups. In another study we successfully took this approach with patients with schizophrenia
in a day centre in the Gorbals in Glasgow [5]. We had no major problems in sharing the
medical record and tailoring information for this more difficult-to-engage group.
Information for patients can be tailored in a number of ways. For example, Grasso [6]
attempted to develop a conversational method to put counter arguments to users about their
dietary choices. In her approach the arguments presented to users were tailored according to
their previous responses.
Others, such as Marsden [7], have used online questionnaires. She recruited female
university students and asked them to complete online questionnaires about their dietary,
exercise, and other lifestyle choices. She compared the impact of tailored versus general
emails sent to the students encouraging behaviour that might help prevent subsequent
development of osteoporosis. She used Prochaska and Di Clemente‘s [8] stages of change
model to choose which type of message to send.
Kroez, Werkman, and Brug [9] recently reviewed the use of tailored information in
dietary advice. Most of the studies used online questionnaires about food, exercise, other
lifestyle issues such as whether the user smoked to generate advice or a health plan as a letter
or a leaflet. Most studies found that those with personalized advice increased their daily
servings of fruits and vegetables.
Another way of tailoring is to make better use of the information already seen onscreen
or given via leaflet. Many websites now record the pathway that users have followed. This
information could be used to tailor information. This approach was pioneered by Mellish and
others in Edinburgh in a novel experiment in museums in which a handset was to give audio
information about the exhibits being viewed. The aim was that the handset would send the
users position to a computer which could then generate an explanation such as ‗This exhibit
that you are looking at is similar to the one that you have just seen in that. In comparison to
the XXX exhibit that you saw before on the second floor this piece. In the end, the main
system was delivered through a standard web interface, but the idea could be revisited [10].
The information needs of patients change over time. A person newly diagnosed with
cancer requires different information to someone who has been living with cancer for some
time and had multiple treatments. Information for patients needs to be built up over time. In
face to face dialogue with a health professional, they professional will partly remember what
has already been discussed but also confirm this to gradually help the patient to build an
understanding of the situation. If this is to be done in some form of automated dialogue the
computer program needs to adapt to current questions or information requests and build upon
what has been said before. Dialogue systems are well described by Bickmore and Giorgino
[11] with illustrations from their work with Fitrack exercise advisor [12] and other systems.
Lastly, these approaches to tailoring information normally assume some ‗user model‘ by
which to select and modify the information presented. One of the most well known consumer
websites which constructs a dynamic user model is Amazon (www.amazon.com). Most
readers will know how this site, based on the user‘s current choice, suggests books that other
readers who have made the same choice, have gone on to buy. The same approach can of
256 Leslie Wasserman
course be made with health information and can be particularly useful where there is a very
large amount of information for which a ‗dynamic‘ index could be constructed, tailored to the
particular user.
In general, tailoring helps people connect with the information. A systematic review of
tailored information in a range of settings and conditions has explored the effects of using
computers to tailor printed information to the individual patient, with positive effects on
patient satisfaction, knowledge, and anxiety [13].
The other approach has been to develop a chatroom within which we embed the
videostream. The web chat software used was ajaxchat (http://www.ajaxchat.org), which is
free software, licensed under the Gnu Public Licence (GPL). Ajaxchat is written in PHP, a
web scripting language, and uses the MySQL relational database to store messages and data.
Among the features of ajaxchat are fast response time, use of avatars, personal messaging,
emoticons, and inline URLs. We modified the ajaxchat programme code to embed the
webcast stream. The ajaxchat method has a limitation in the length of the message that can be
posted to the chat room compared to GoodMood WIP. We also have had some concerns
about the bandwidth required on the server. However, it currently has many other advantages.
GoodMood requires Active X and some of our non-University users do not have
administrative rights on their computers to install this. We have also had problems with
firewalls for GoodMood that we have not had with ajaxchat. We have so far not reproduced
the useful ajaxchat features of avatars, list of signed-on members, and parallel chatrooms,
within GoodMood WIP. The main advantage of the ajaxchat approach is that it is free of
charge and open source. We continue to pursue the use of both methods so as to identify and
use the most useful features of each.
We have used live webcasting with chatroom for a number of events for our own
University students and others. For example, in early May 2007 we ran a one day online e-
health workshop. We had 15 participants from Malaysia, Pakistan, Iran, Germany, Scotland
and England and three presenters. Comments from participants included: ‗all in all an
enjoyable learning experience for me, with lots of potential‘, ‗the potential for web casting is
enormous‘, ‗I much liked the discussion and its momentum. I also liked that you did not use
voice as a medium for communication, but used text based chat for this purpose‘, ‗I‘ve never
done a chatroom ever, so being able to chat with many people in real time online was really
interesting and beneficial‘.
Various practical issues about delivery arose and these are being addressed. They include
the need for practice and guidance for the facilitators in this new medium, getting the right
balance between presentation and discussion, and deciding which elements need to be live
and which can be pre-recorded and made available beforehand.
support in a public library the package was less successful. We concluded that human contact
was an important feature of use of CCBT. Others have found the same [18].
We are currently working with Williams, Martinez, Prestwich and others (University of
Glasgow), authors of www.livinglifetothefull.org.uk, a self help cognitive behaviour website
for people with anxiety or depression. We are exploring different ways in which we can
improve the connection between patients, website and therapist. For example, live
webcasting, in which a therapist appears live on the patient‘s computer and leads a chat room
discussion with patients typing their anonymous questions and comments, should provide
more ‗connection‘ than a simple email. However, it requires that patients log on to their
computer at a certain time and that they have both the technical ability and the requisite
hardware to participate. Furthermore, live webcasting will be more costly than a simple email.
Methods need to be easy to use so as to remain accessible to the widest population possible
(accepting that the target population is already limited to Internet users) and the trade-off
between extra cost and possible additional effectiveness needs to be examined. In our current
research we aim to compare different types of motivational support and their acceptability to
users, the feasibility of long term implementation and maintenance of these interventions, and
how they impact on ‗connectedness‘.
Unlike our webcasting for students (above) in which we used webcasting ministudios, in
this new work, therapists will be working from their office or home and the videostream will
be ‗captured‘ on a webcam. We have so far encountered a number of problems in pursuing
this approach including interruptions in the video stream. This occurred during multicasting
(sending video to more than one viewer) and was due to network bandwith limitations. Some
have these have been overcome by using a streaming media server, such as Microsoft Media
Server, which receives video being ―pushed‖ from the therapist's computer and rebroadcasts it
to the viewers. This results in a more stable video output because the streaming server has a
larger dedicated network bandwidth as well as greater computing power. But as of yet, we
have not found a totally robust solution suitable for use with novice Internet users,
particularly those with anxiety or depression.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, tailoring information and live webcasting are both ways in which we can
help patients and students connect more both with the information and the therapist or
lecturer. Ways of combining these approaches should be a productive line of research but
there are both practical and theoretical issues to be addressed.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Dave Hurrell, Zoe Portman, Adrian Vranch, Matt Newcombe,
Gail Wilson, Faye Doris, Fraser Reid and others who have helped with this work.
REFERENCES
[1] Bental, D.S., Cawsey, A.J., Jones, R. (1999). Patient Information Systems that Tailor
to the Individual. Patient Education and Counseling, 36, 171-180.
[2] Jones, R.B., Pearson, J., McGregor, S., Cawsey, A., Barrett, A., Atkinson, J.M.,
Craig, N., Gilmour, W.H., McEwen, J. (1999). Randomised trial of personalised
computer based information for cancer patients. BMJ, 319, 1241-1247.
[3] Jones, R.B., Pearson, J., Cawsey, A.J., Bental, D., Barrett, A., White, J., White, C.A.,
Gilmour, W.H. (2006). Effect of different forms of information produced for cancer
patients on their use of the information, social support, and anxiety: randomised trial.
BMJ, 332, 942 – 948
[4] Zigmond, A.S., Snaith, R.P. (1983). The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta
Psychiatrica Scan, 67, 361-370.
[5] Jones, R., Atkinson, J.M., Coia, D.A., Paterson, L., Morton, A.R., McKenna, K.,
Craig, N., Morrison, J., Gilmour, W.H. (2001). Randomised trial of computer-based
education for patients with schizophrenia, BMJ, 322, 835-840.
[6] Grasso, F., Cawsey, A., Jones, R.B. (2000). Dialectical argumentation to solve
conflicts in advice giving: a case study in the promotion of healthy nutrition in
International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 53 (6), 1077-1115.
[7] Marsden, J. (2003). Primary prevention of osetoporosis in young British women: a
comparison of electronic stage-based tailored messages versus non-tailored
intervention. University of Glasgow. PhD Thesis.
[8] Prochaska, J.O., DiClemente, C.C., Norcross, J.C. (1992 Sep). In search of how
people change. Applications to addictive behaviors. Am Psychol.47(9), 1102-14.
[9] Kroeze, W., Werkman, A., Brug, J. A. (2006). Systematic Review of
Randomized Trials on the Effectiveness of Computer-Tailored Education on Physical
Activity and Dietary Behaviors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 31 (3), 205-223.
[10] O'Donnell, M., Knott, A., Mellish, C., Oberlander, J. (2001). ILEX:The architecture
of a dynamic hypertext generation system. Natural Language Engineering, 7, 225-
250.(More information at http://www.hcrc.ed.ac.uk/ilex/)
Demystifying Outdated Theories about How the Brain Works 261
[11] Bickmore., and Giorgino. Some novel aspects of health communication from a
dialogue systems perspective. In Proceedings of the AAAI fall symposium on dialogue
systems for health communication. Washington, DC. (Downloaded from
http://www.misu.bmc.org/~bickmore/dshc/bickmore.pdf August 2007)
[12] Bickmore, T., Gruber, A., Picard, R. (2005). Establishing the computer-patient
working alliance in automated health behavior change interventions. Patient
Education and Counseling, 59 (1), 21-30.
[13] Treweek, S.P., Glenton, C., Oxman, A.D. (2002). Computer-generated patient
education materials: Do they affect professional practice? A systematic review.
Journal American Medical Information Association, 9 (4), 346-358
[14] Jones, R., Skirton, H., McMullan, M. (2006). Feasibility of combining e-health for
patients with e-learning for students using synchronous technologies. Journal of
Advanced Nursing, 56 (1), 99-110.
[15] Kaltenthaler, E., Shackley, P., Stevens, K., et al. (2002). Computerised Cognitive
Behavioural Therapy for Depression and Anxiety. Report commissioned by NHS
R&D HTA Programme on behalf of NICE.
[16] White, J., Jones, R,, McGarry, E. (2000). Cognitive behavioural computer therapy for
the anxiety disorders: A pilot study. J Mental Hlth, 9, 505-516.
[17] Jones, R. B., Kamarzaman, Z., Naven, L. M., Morton, W. R., Marriott, C., Craig, N.,
and Gilmour, W. H. (2001). Cognitive behavioural computer therapy for anxiety:
difficulties in carrying out a randomised trial and lessons learned. University of
Glasgow. Submission to NICE.
[18] Gellatly, J., Bower, P., Hennessy, S., Richards, D., Gilbody, S., Lovell, K. (2007).
What makes self-help interventions effective in the management of depressive
symptoms? Meta-analysis and meta-regression. Psychol Med, 11, 1-24. (published on-
line early).
[19] Horvath, A.O., Greenberg, L.S. (1989). Development and validation of the Working
Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 36, 223–233.
[20] Bordin, E.S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the
working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 16, 252–260.
[21] Tracey, T.J., Kokotovic, A.M. (1989). Factor structure of the Working Alliance
Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 1, 207–210.
[22] Knaevelsrud, C., Maercker, A. (2007). Internet-based treatment for PTSD reduces
distress and facilitates the development of a strong therapeutic alliance: a randomized
controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry, 7:13 doi:10.1186/1471-244X-7-13.
[23] Knaevelsrud, C., Maercker, A. (2006). Does the Quality of the Working Alliance
Predict Treatment Outcome in Online Psychotherapy for Traumatized Patients? J Med
Internet Res, 8(4), e31 <URL: http://www.jmir.org/2006/4/e31/>
[24] Reid, F.J.M., Reid, D.J. (in press).The expressive and conversational affordances of
mobile messaging. Behaviour & Information Technology.
[25] Jones, R., Tweddle, S., Hampshire, M., Hill, A., Moult, B., and McGregor, S. (2000).
Patient-led learning for the clinical professions in fulfilling the information needs of
patients. Published by NHS Information Authority, June 2000. Ref 2000-IA-280.
Available from http://www.nhsia.nhs.uk/informatics/ pages/resource_informatics/
patient_led_learning.pdf.
262 Leslie Wasserman
[26] Wilson, E.J. (1995). Use of a public access health information system by student
nurses. Master of Public Health Thesis, University of Glasgow.
[27] Budtz, S., and Witt, K. (2002). Consulting the Internet before visit to general practice.
Patients‘use of the Internet and other sources of health information. Scand J Prim
Health Care, 20(3), 174-176.
INDEX
age, xiv, xv, 8, 20, 21, 22, 28, 37, 41, 42, 43, 44, 46,
A 47, 48, 50, 51, 53, 58, 61, 72, 109, 118, 120, 135,
139, 175, 176, 180, 181, 192, 199, 200, 239, 240,
acceleration, 43, 94, 104
241, 243, 244, 245, 249
access, xii, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 14, 15, 16, 17,
agent, 14, 120
18, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 32, 35, 38, 255,
agents, 22, 117
256, 257, 259, 262
aging, 63, 64
accessibility, xii, 1, 2, 9, 10, 13, 14, 15, 17, 18, 19,
aid, 8, 15, 19, 47, 54, 103, 136, 186, 193, 194, 249
22, 23, 28, 29, 30, 32, 33, 36, 37
AIM, 5
accommodation, 27, 109
air, 228
accountability, 9, 30
Airlines, 15, 29
accounting, 3, 78
Albert Einstein, 72, 105
accreditation, 7, 187
Alberta, 173
accuracy, 25, 27, 58, 144, 220
alertness, 16
achievement, 8, 16, 20, 121, 127, 150
alpha, 220
ACM, 30, 32, 33, 38
ALT, 36
acoustic, 46
alternative, 2, 8, 9, 14, 18, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 49, 85,
acoustical, 225
86, 121, 134, 197, 202, 211, 242
acquisition of knowledge, 158
alternatives, 176, 177
activation, xii, 39, 40, 44, 45, 46, 49, 57, 60, 63, 66
alters, 66
acute, 16, 191
Alzheimer disease, 109
ADA, 13, 14, 15, 16, 26, 27, 29
Amazon, 255
adaptation, 113, 114, 116, 124, 125
ambiguity, 53, 87
ADHD, 29
amendments, 13
administration, 225
American Sign Language, 25
administrative, 3, 27
Americans with Disabilities Act, 7, 13, 29, 37
administrators, 10, 21, 29, 186
AMS, 66
adolescence, 133, 241
Amsterdam, 62, 66, 151, 152, 211
adolescents, 66, 152, 154, 189, 217, 233
anatomy, 111, 112, 156, 164, 165, 173
adult, xi, 8, 20, 21, 31, 36, 41, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 49,
animals, 49, 109, 116, 118, 119, 123, 126
53, 55, 58, 59, 126, 180, 242, 244
antibiotics, 124
adult education, 20, 21
anticoagulation, 177
adult population, 180
anti-smoking, 254
adulthood, 47, 53, 58, 64, 109
anxiety, 21, 158, 159, 161, 162, 163, 164, 165, 168,
adults, xi, 1, 2, 8, 16, 19, 21, 25, 31, 41, 46, 53, 109,
169, 180, 183, 184, 218, 219, 253, 254, 256, 257,
118, 123, 180, 242, 244, 259
258, 260, 261
advocacy, 20
anxiety disorder, 261
Africa, 82, 190
aphasia, 242
appendix, 97
264 Index
application, xiii, 3, 5, 6, 9, 31, 73, 80, 98, 101, 131, base rates, 184, 185
232, 246 beams, 90, 92
arbitrary associations, 218 behavior, xi, 20, 56, 67, 114, 117, 124, 136, 138,
Argentina, xiii, 71, 72, 73, 105, 106 148, 158, 163, 240, 244, 261
argument, 41, 100, 111, 112, 114, 128, 260 behavioral disorders, 33
Aristotelian, 103, 110, 112, 125 behaviorists, 240, 248
Aristotle, 108, 111, 112, 126, 127 belief systems, 126
Arizona, 14, 30, 36 beliefs, 73, 109, 118, 119, 122, 126, 129, 248
articulation, 25 bell, 3, 227
artistic, 77, 82 benchmark, 137
assessment, 5, 20, 22, 25, 32, 52, 144, 145, 157, 171, benefits, xi, xii, 2, 3, 14, 21, 26, 38, 58, 61, 63, 69,
174, 180, 191, 192, 249 73, 135, 147, 149, 157, 158, 165, 167, 171, 176,
assignment, 5, 22, 48, 153, 249 177, 178, 184, 185, 187, 259
assistive technology, 25, 30 bereavement, 180
Assistive Technology Act, 30 Best Practice, 28
assumptions, 11, 13, 109 bias, 48, 61, 117, 119
asthma, 192 Bible, 112
asymmetry, 78 bilingual, xii, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48,
asynchronous, 4, 5, 6, 12, 22, 253, 259 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61,
asynchronous communication, 12, 22 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68
Athens, 107, 127 bilingualism, xi, xii, 39, 40, 41, 49, 53, 54, 61, 63,
athletes, 32 64, 67
atmosphere, 11, 94, 98, 180 binding, 17
atomic clocks, 80, 81 biomass, 202
atomic theory, 81 birds, 114
atoms, 80, 94 birth, xiv, 25, 42, 56, 75, 97, 127, 155, 241, 242,
atrial fibrillation, 177 244, 245
attachment, 256 blame, 177
attention, 9, 21, 22, 28 blocks, 52, 248
Attention Deficit Disorder, 19, 28 blood, 108
attitudes, 35, 101, 157, 166, 171, 172, 241 bonds, 97
audio, 255 boredom, 165
auditing, 27 Boston, 15, 32, 38, 65, 66, 194, 232, 250
Australia, 188 bottom-up, 57
autism, 16 boys, 139, 242
autonomy, 128, 181, 188 Braille, 14, 18, 27
availability, 9, 18, 27, 256 brain, xv, 16, 19, 28, 66, 239, 240, 241, 242, 244,
awareness, 21, 28, 40, 44, 50, 51, 55, 60, 63, 64, 67, 245, 246, 248, 250, 251
118, 137, 153, 176, 187, 218 brain development, xv, 239, 250
brain functioning, 245
brain functions, 239, 248
B brain injury, 16, 19, 28, 242
brainstorming, 148, 164
babies, 40, 42, 191, 244, 245 brass, 216, 225, 226, 227
bachelor‘s degree, 20, 98 Brazil, 73
back, 20, 27, 90, 94, 137, 162, 163, 164, 166, 167,
breast, 254
168, 192, 231, 242
breast cancer, 184, 186, 187, 188, 190, 191, 192,
background information, 217
193, 194
bacteria, 124
breast carcinoma, 188
bandwidth, 5, 256, 257, 258
breathing, 230
barrier, 9, 18
Britain, 119
barriers, 9, 10, 13, 14, 17, 18, 22, 23, 26, 28, 29, 48,
British children, 119
181, 182, 184, 185, 187, 189
Buenos Aires, 71, 73, 82, 98, 106, 213
base rate, 184, 185
Index 265
building blocks, 61 classical, xv, 50, 79, 80, 82, 83, 85, 88, 96, 97, 102,
buses, 8 115, 239
classification, 30, 112
classroom, xiii, xiv, 5, 10, 14, 19, 20, 27, 32, 34, 35,
C 38, 71, 96, 98, 103, 104, 105, 132, 151, 155, 156,
162, 169, 212, 217, 230, 231, 232, 233, 242, 245,
call centers, 186
248
Canada, 31, 175, 189, 190, 194
classroom teacher, 249
cancer, 177, 179, 184, 186, 188, 190, 191, 192, 193,
classroom teachers, 249
194, 253, 254, 255, 258, 259, 260
classrooms, 4, 12, 20, 23, 34, 151, 152, 197, 199,
cancer care, 188, 192
212, 232
cancer screening, 186, 190, 194
clients, 176
cancer treatment, 185, 187, 191, 194
climate change, 202, 203, 204, 205, 207, 208, 209
carbon, 36
clinical, 261
carcinoma, 188
clinician, 176, 177, 178, 179, 180, 182, 187, 188
carefulness, 124
closure, 144
caregivers, 56, 193
clouds, 117
case study, 31, 32, 58, 211, 260
clusters, 46, 57
cast, 23, 31
CMC, 11, 12
casting, 5, 257
Co, 29, 251
categorization, 144
coaches, 184
category b, 42
Cochrane, 184, 185, 186, 189, 193
Catholic, 112
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 193
causal relationship, 139
codes, xii, 39, 42, 43, 53, 60, 161
causality, 93, 110
coding, 161
CBT, 257, 258
cognition, xv, 30, 40, 41, 50, 55, 56, 59, 61, 127,
Census, 42, 61, 68
151, 152, 212, 219, 239, 248
cerebral palsy, 24
cognitive, 253, 257, 258
CERN, 95
cognitive abilities, 24, 40, 41, 200
certification, 2, 3, 6, 20, 187
cognitive biases, 57
cervical, 254
cognitive capacities, 61
cesium, 80
cognitive capacity, 134
channels, 12
cognitive development, xii, 39, 40, 53, 54, 55, 59,
Chat rooms, 253
60, 63, 65, 67, 116, 122, 202, 203, 208, 246, 251
chemotherapy, 193
cognitive dimension, 59
childbirth, 169
cognitive domains, 54
childhood, xii, xiii, 39, 40, 41, 49, 53, 54, 55, 61, 64,
cognitive effort, 153
107, 108, 109, 110, 118, 189, 240, 241, 243, 248,
cognitive function, 42, 54, 62
249, 251
cognitive impairment, 9
children, xii, xiii, xv, 2, 8, 16, 19, 20, 24, 30, 34, 35,
cognitive level, 58, 181, 210
38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51,
cognitive load, 134, 232
52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 63, 64, 65,
cognitive performance, 58
66, 67, 107, 108, 109, 116, 117, 118, 119, 123,
cognitive perspective, 154
126, 127, 151, 152, 176, 180, 189, 190, 191, 192, cognitive process, xii, 39, 40, 42, 50, 53, 54, 59, 61,
239, 240, 242, 244, 245, 246, 248, 249, 250 64, 136, 148, 149, 182, 218
chronic fatigue syndrome, 9
cognitive processing, xii, 39, 50, 59, 61, 64, 218
circulation, 108
cognitive science, 40, 41, 250
citizens, 14
cognitive system, 44, 50, 56, 60
citizenship, 3, 198
cognitive tasks, 58
civil rights, 2, 13, 14, 30, 35, 37
cognitive tool, 40, 61
civil society, 31
coherence, 127, 132, 142, 145
classes, xv, 3, 11, 26, 27, 42, 73, 83, 98, 103, 139,
cohort, 160, 194
200, 210, 215, 219
266 Index
collaboration, 4, 6, 11, 12, 21, 25, 33, 34, 36, 173, confounding variables, 219
191, 220 confusion, 82
college campuses, 23 Congress, vii, 13, 15, 16, 37
college students, 9, 29, 121, 126, 232 congruence, 220
colleges, 3, 6, 8, 14, 15 Connecticut, 21, 31
colon, 177, 184 connectionist, 56, 57, 59, 66
colon cancer, 177, 184 connectionist models, 57
Colorado, 7 consensus, 57, 126, 178, 186, 190
colorectal cancer, 193 consent, 14, 177, 178, 187, 188, 190, 192
colors, 52 consolidation, 156, 164, 171
Columbia, 125 Constitution, 14, 37
Columbia University, 125 constraints, xiii, 48, 107, 110, 115, 127, 181, 187,
comfort zone, 248 225
commerce, 15, 27 construct validity, 220
communication, xiv, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14, 18, 22, 24, 26, construction, 49, 103, 198, 211, 229
31, 34, 35, 56, 62, 155, 158, 159, 175, 176, 182, constructivist, 158, 170, 171, 241, 250, 251
185, 187, 188, 189, 191, 192, 193, 243, 257, 261 constructivist learning, 251
communication skills, xiv, 155, 158, 159, 182, 189, contextualization, 74, 77, 198
192, 193 continuity, 72, 85, 194
communicative intent, 56, 132 contractors, 6
communities, xii, 1, 2, 11, 12, 34, 36, 38, 118, 190 control, xv, 13, 21, 33, 40, 44, 45, 46, 49, 50, 51, 52,
community, 11, 12, 13, 14, 20, 21, 26, 34, 41, 42, 77, 53, 54, 56, 57, 59, 60, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 110,
78, 79, 81, 101, 186 132, 137, 138, 139, 141, 142, 145, 146, 147, 176,
competence, xiii, 20, 58, 131, 134, 136, 138, 148, 177, 180, 183, 215, 219, 220, 221, 222, 223, 224,
153, 176 225, 256
competency, 173, 197 control group, xv, 138, 139, 147, 183, 215, 219, 220,
competition, 46, 52, 66, 68, 167 221, 222, 223, 224, 225
competitor, xv, 239 controlled trials, 157, 184
complexity, xii, 39, 40, 49, 55, 56, 59, 63, 65, 86, 99, Convention on the Rights of People with Disabilities,
111, 112, 176, 180, 198, 199, 246 17
compliance, xii, 2, 7, 16, 24, 28, 180, 187 conversion, 12
complications, 155 cooperative learning, 20, 22, 33, 249
components, xiii, 4, 11, 20, 26, 43, 44, 45, 57, 68, Coping, 137, 141
79, 114, 131, 134, 142, 178, 244, 258 coping model, 137, 141
composition, 150, 152, 153 correlation, 53
comprehension, 46, 53, 56, 63, 68, 83, 134, 150, 217, cortex, 50
220, 232, 233, 246 cost-effective, 185
computer skills, 9 costs, 17, 20, 26, 27, 49, 56, 184, 187, 191, 193, 194,
computer software, 184 256, 257, 259
computer-mediated communication (CMC), 11 counseling, 20, 21, 25, 182
computers, 253, 256, 257 course content, 4, 10, 24, 27, 28
computing, 258 course work, 8
concentration, 21, 167 Court of Appeals, 15
conception, xiv, 72, 75, 77, 78, 82, 85, 99, 102, 128, courts, 14
195, 196 coverage, 14
conceptualization, 96 creativity, 199
concrete, 15, 68, 85, 216, 218 credentials, xi
conductor, 19, 78, 219 credit, 3, 7, 21, 24, 26, 28
confidence, 20, 158, 161, 163, 165, 170, 180 critical analysis, 79, 83
conflict, 11, 60, 64, 97, 109, 110, 120, 122, 126, 158, critical thinking, 73, 157, 197, 208, 248
176, 177, 180, 182, 183, 184, 185, 188, 189, 190, criticism, 77, 79, 86, 111
191, 194 cross-sectional, 161
conflict resolution, 64 cues, 47, 54, 60, 144
Index 267
indeterminacy, 47 insurance, 27
Indiana, 15, 32 integration, 13, 28, 29, 66, 168, 196
indication, xiii, 131 intellectual development, 240
indicators, 142, 177 intellectual property, 18
individual differences, 152, 225 intelligence, 250, 251
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 8, 16, intensive care unit, 189
32, 33, 37 intentional behavior, 117
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement intentionality, 59, 108
Act, 16, 32 intentions, 121
induction, xv, 195 interaction, xii, 3, 4, 5, 9, 12, 28, 34, 37, 40, 41, 43,
inequality, 220 56, 61, 75, 79, 82, 96, 134, 159, 178, 182, 211,
inertia, 79 222, 223, 241, 243, 244, 256
infancy, 62, 63, 68, 118, 180, 244 interaction effect, 223
infants, 40, 42, 43, 44, 47, 48, 49, 53, 57, 60, 64, 65, interactions, 4, 41, 55, 59, 61, 125, 145, 146, 222,
66, 68, 189, 242, 244, 245 224, 225, 241, 248
infections, xi interactivity, 11, 12
infinite, 61 interdependence, 64
information age, xiv, 175 interdisciplinary, 152
information and communication technology, 18, 35 interface, 12, 41, 48, 50, 51, 255
information exchange, 180 interference, 46, 47, 53, 66, 68, 218
information processing, 21, 242, 245 internal processes, 149
information retrieval, 158, 170 internal validity, 225
information sharing, 180 Internet, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, 22, 24, 26,
Information System, 260 27, 28, 31, 34, 36, 37, 38, 184, 186, 216, 254,
Information Technology, 29, 30, 31, 32, 36, 37, 38 256, 258, 259, 261, 262
informed consent, 177, 178, 187, 188, 190 Internet Protocol, 5
infrastructure, 5, 187 internists, 192
inheritance, 117 interpersonal skills, 157
inherited, 111 interpretation, 17
inhibition, 49, 52, 53, 57, 59, 63, 64, 66 interrelationships, 133
inhibitory, 49, 52, 53, 54, 57, 59, 60, 66, 67 interstate, 27
injuries, 242 interstate commerce, 27
injury, vii, 16, 19, 28, 242, 259 interval, 144, 225
Innovation, 1, 150 intervention, xiii, 65, 131, 134, 135, 138, 139, 145,
input, 9, 12, 23, 25, 28 147, 149, 157, 163, 177, 185, 193, 249, 260
insecurity, 165 interview, 161
insight, 160 interviews, 77, 159, 161, 170, 254
inspiration, 82 intonation, 245
instability, 244 intrinsic, xiv, 47, 95, 117, 155
instinct, 56 invasive, 188, 191
institutions, 8, 10, 15, 16, 23, 34, 161, 186 inventories, xii, 39, 43, 55
institutions of higher education, 15 investment, 187
instruction, xiii, xv, 5, 7, 16, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 29, ions, 56
36, 51, 107, 109, 110, 121, 124, 126, 131, 134, Iran, 257
135, 136, 137, 140, 145, 147, 148, 149, 150, 151, Ireland, 23, 33
152, 153, 172, 211, 215, 217, 218, 219, 220, 221, isolation, 4, 25, 122
222, 225, 226, 231
instructional methods, 16, 19
instructional practice, 147 J
instructional techniques, 147
JAMA, 190, 192, 193
instructors, 3, 4, 5, 9, 10, 19, 22, 27, 28, 30, 31, 157,
Jamaica, 17
212
Japan, 66, 190
instruments, 86, 88, 96, 99, 191, 216, 225, 226, 227
Japanese, 67
272 Index
Java, 3 learners, xi, xii, 2, 3, 8, 10, 11, 12, 21, 22, 24, 29, 32,
job skills, 21 64, 66, 68, 135, 153, 154, 216, 217, 232, 241,
jobs, 3, 9, 21 248, 256
Jordan, 21, 38 learning difficulties, xv, 239
judge, 50, 144 learning disabilities, 9, 12, 21, 23, 29, 31, 35, 38,
judges, 142 135, 151, 152, 154, 233
judgment, 181, 246 learning environment, 9, 10, 11, 12, 61, 169, 200,
Jun, 66, 194 259
Jung, 3, 33 learning process, 11, 79
junior high school, 198 learning skills, 21
learning styles, 165, 249
learning task, 68, 137, 170
K left hemisphere, 242, 244, 245
legislation, 16
K-12, xii, 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, 8, 15, 19, 24, 31, 34, 36, 38
Leibniz, 85, 86
Kant, 81, 114
leisure, 18
kappa, 145
liberal, 24, 116
Keynes, 233
lifelong learning, 197
kinematics, 98, 231
lifespan, 61
kinesthetic, 248, 249
lifestyle, 255
knowledge acquisition, 156
likelihood, 13, 117
knowledge construction, 211
limitation, 257
knowledge transfer, 194
limitations, 9, 13, 56, 179, 180, 182, 210, 226
linear, 82, 217, 226
L linguistic, xi, xii, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 46, 48, 50, 51,
53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 59, 60, 61, 63, 65, 68, 142,
labour, 156 143, 151, 244, 245
Lafayette, 32 linguistic information, 46
language, xii, xv, 8, 11, 16, 18, 22, 24, 25, 39, 40, linguistic processing, 51, 60
41, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 53, 54, linguistic representation, 62
55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, linguistically, 56, 68
68, 69, 83, 85, 114, 126, 127, 134, 138, 217, 226, links, 28, 54, 59, 137, 256
232, 239, 240, 241, 242, 243, 244, 245, 248, 249, listening, xv, 159, 215, 217, 218, 219, 220, 222, 226,
250, 254, 257, 258 231, 232, 244
language acquisition, xii, xv, 39, 40, 41, 42, 48, 49, literacy, 9, 19, 22, 51, 125, 138, 139, 149, 153, 187,
54, 55, 57, 59, 60, 61, 62, 66, 68, 239, 240, 243, 249
244 literature, xi, 259
language development, xii, 39, 40, 41, 42, 46, 53, 54, loans, 25
56, 59, 60, 61, 62, 242, 243 local government, 14, 15, 16, 22
language impairment, 16 location, 3, 6, 15, 26, 52
language processing, 41, 50, 51, 54, 59, 63, 66 logging, 139
language proficiency, 58 London, 37, 67, 125, 126, 127, 128, 129, 152, 155,
language skills, 245 171, 172, 173, 211, 212, 213, 250
laparotomy, 177 long period, 79, 112, 160
laryngeal cancer, 254 long-term retention, 217
law, xii, 2, 3, 15, 22, 24, 29, 30, 33, 37 Los Angeles, 14, 34, 37, 126
laws, xii, 2, 10, 13, 14, 18, 22, 32, 33, 74, 76, 80, 82, Louisiana, 7
83, 85, 88, 94, 112, 114 love, 243
laws of motion, 76 low-level, 132
LEA, 64, 137 lymph, 193
leadership, 33, 250 lymph node, 193
Index 273
palliative care, 189 philosophy, xii, 71, 73, 74, 81, 108, 110, 111, 122,
paradigm shift, 241 125, 126, 128, 129
paradox, 93, 94, 173 Phoenix, 7
parallelism, 66 phone, 22, 24, 27, 246
parameter, 120 phonemes, 46
parental involvement, 20 phonological, 42, 43, 44, 46, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53,
parents, 22, 36, 176, 180, 189, 190, 191, 229, 245, 54, 55, 57, 59, 60, 63, 64, 65, 67
250 phonological codes, 43, 44, 53, 60
Paris, 129 phonology, 65, 68
participant observation, 159 photoelectrical, 80
particles, 75, 80, 94, 95 photographs, 119
partnership, 176, 178 physical education, 19, 24
passenger, 91 physical interaction, 241
passive, 179, 184, 185, 191 physical sciences, 125, 200, 211
pathologists, 65 physical therapy, 20, 25
pathways, 41, 55, 186, 188, 248 physicians, 176, 180, 181, 182, 186, 189, 192, 193
patient rights, 178 physicists, 79
patient-centered, 178, 189, 190 physics, 80, 211, 212, 213, 217
patients, xi, xiv, 109, 128, 175, 176, 177, 178, 179, physiological, 115
180, 181, 182, 183, 184, 186, 187, 188, 189, 192, physiology, 156, 164, 165, 172
193, 194, 253, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260, Piagetian, 250
261 pilot study, 194, 211, 257, 261
payroll, 3 planning, xiii, 8, 16, 19, 20, 21, 23, 131, 132, 133,
PBL, xi, xiv, xv, 155, 156, 157, 158, 159, 160, 161, 134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 140, 141, 142, 145, 147,
162, 163, 164, 165, 166, 167, 168, 169, 170, 171, 149, 150, 151, 152, 157, 171, 183, 184, 187, 193,
172, 173, 174, 195, 196, 197, 198, 199, 200, 205, 197
209, 210, 212 plants, 109
pedagogical, xii, xiii, 71, 72, 83 plastic, 82, 227, 242
pedagogy, 24, 98, 232 plasticity, 244
pediatric, 180, 189, 190, 192 platforms, 11
peer, 20, 34, 134, 138, 166 Plato, 30, 108, 110, 111, 127
peer review, 166 play, xiv, 102, 175, 216, 225, 226, 228, 229, 230,
peer tutoring, 20, 34 231, 241, 248, 259
peers, xii, 3, 8, 11, 14, 21, 25, 26, 39, 41, 42, 43, 44, pluralistic, 116
47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 53, 55, 56, 57, 58, 60, 61, 102, pluralistic approach, 116
135, 137, 147, 148, 162, 163, 166 Poincaré, 79, 81, 86
Pennsylvania, 7 polar bears, 123
perception, 43, 66, 67, 68, 110, 180 policy initiative, 2
perceptions, 126, 156, 159, 160, 172, 173, 174, 184, policy makers, 186
188, 190, 192 political power, 13
performers, 199 politicians, 74
permit, xii, 1, 2, 9, 12, 26 politics, 9
perseverance, 177 polling, 256
personal, 255, 257 poor, 41, 122, 123, 166, 233, 249
personal life, 177 population, xiii, 12, 13, 19, 41, 61, 68, 107, 113, 114,
personal values, 184 124, 160, 179, 180, 258
personality, 157 portfolio, 22, 249
persons with disabilities, 8, 10, 14, 15, 17, 18, 22, 37 portfolio assessment, 22
Peru, 210, 212 Portugal, 195, 200, 202, 210
pharmacology, 165 positive correlation, 210
Philadelphia, 95 positive relation, 50
philosophers, 74, 108, 111, 112, 113, 115 positive relationship, 50
philosophical, 74, 81, 85, 96, 99, 125, 159 postsecondary education, 36
276 Index
reasoning, xiii, 50, 67, 73, 107, 108, 110, 118, 127, responsibility for learning, xiv, 155, 171
151, 157, 159, 163 responsiveness, 190
reasoning skills, 73 restaurants, 15, 229
rebelliousness, 72 retail, 8, 9
recall, 58, 242 retardation, 16, 34
reception, 217 retention, xiv, 155, 198, 215, 217, 231, 232, 233,
recognition, 12, 13, 23, 25, 57, 64, 66, 67, 68, 102, 246, 247
225 rhetoric, 134
recollection, 218 rhythm, 42, 67, 216, 227
reconcile, 217 rhythms, 220
recreation, 6, 18 right hemisphere, 245
recruiting, 8, 50 risk, 102, 177, 184, 188, 191, 210
rectilinear, 75, 76, 93 risk perception, 184
reduction, 28, 254 risks, 177, 178, 184
reference frame, 75, 76, 82, 83, 85, 86, 87, 88, 89, rote learning, 157, 217, 218, 231
91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 99, 100, 102, 103, 104 routines, 165
refining, 226 rural, 2, 32, 33, 193
reflection, 21, 148, 153, 170, 197, 259 Russian, 46, 67
reflectivity, 151
regression, 261
regular, 5, 23, 25, 58, 148, 151, 152, 171 S
regulation, 21, 135, 136, 137, 138, 140, 141, 142,
safeguards, 25, 138
148, 149, 152, 153, 154, 171
safety, 7
regulations, 24
salaries, 32
Rehabilitation Act, xii, 1, 2, 13, 15, 26, 35
salary, 230
reinforcement, 149
sales, 32
rejection, 109
sample, 72, 139, 145, 179, 200, 220
relational database, 257
sampling, 210
relationship, xii, xiv, 39, 40, 50, 54, 81, 98, 99, 102,
SAS, 45
122, 125, 139, 148, 161, 171, 195, 196, 197, 198,
satellite, 80, 256
200, 202, 203, 205, 210, 228, 243, 251, 258
satisfaction, 159, 168, 176, 178, 180, 181, 183, 184,
relationships, 33, 40, 50, 57, 58, 81, 84, 210, 247
185, 187, 192, 193, 256
relativity, 72, 76, 79, 82, 85, 87, 88, 95, 100, 104
scaffolding, 74, 135, 136, 149
relevance, 8, 162, 169, 170, 198, 258
scandalous, 101
reliability, 142, 161, 201, 220
scarcity, 48
religion, 112, 126
scheduling, 14, 186
religiosity, 109, 119, 123
schema, 11, 133, 216
religious belief, 121, 126
schemas, 45, 132, 136
religious beliefs, 121, 126
schizophrenia, 255, 260
Renaissance, 82
school, xi, xii, xiii, xiv, xv, 3, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9, 13, 14, 15,
repetitions, 143
16, 20, 21, 22, 24, 25, 27, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37,
reproduction, 112
38, 41, 51, 71, 72, 73, 74, 97, 98, 101, 102, 103,
research and development, 18
107, 109, 117, 118, 119, 132, 138, 139, 143, 148,
resentment, 163
149, 151, 153, 154, 155, 173, 192, 195, 196, 197,
resistance, 109, 124, 126
198, 200, 201, 202, 204, 205, 208, 209, 210, 212,
resolution, 64, 196
213, 216, 217, 218, 219, 239, 240, 241, 249
resource allocation, 40
school activities, 20
resources, xiii, 3, 4, 10, 14, 20, 22, 24, 26, 33, 40, 46,
schooling, 138, 203
47, 48, 50, 56, 59, 63, 131, 132, 133, 158, 184,
science education, 83, 95, 102, 125, 198, 201, 211,
187
212, 251
respiration, 244
science teaching, 196
response time, 257
scientific community, 77, 78, 79, 81, 101
responsibilities, 7, 20, 161
scientific knowledge, 81, 83, 103, 122
278 Index
temporal, 63, 81, 85, 89, 90, 93 transfer, xiv, 10, 14, 151, 155, 194, 217, 219
Tennessee, 10, 14, 37 transference, 169
tension, 165, 170, 177 transformation, 76, 104, 110
test data, 220 transformations, 75, 76, 77, 79, 80, 91, 95, 104
test scores, 171, 224 transfusion, 110
text messaging, 5, 259 transgression, 72
textbooks, xii, 10, 71, 73, 77, 78, 83, 88, 103, 123, transition, xi, 8, 16, 19, 20, 21, 31, 32, 35, 46, 80,
124, 248 113, 158, 165
theology, 111, 112, 128 transitions, 46, 189
theoretical, 260 translation, 26, 48, 49, 66, 141
theory, xii, 2, 10, 11, 34 transmission, 3, 12
therapeutic, 258, 261 transplantation, 189
therapeutic relationship, 258 transportation, 6, 14
therapists, 158, 173, 258, 259 traumatic brain injury, 16, 19, 28
therapy, 20, 24, 25, 173, 177, 184, 253, 257, 258, travel, 9, 80, 84, 91, 93, 94, 229
261 treaties, 17
thinking, 11, 74, 83, 114, 126, 127, 129, 132, 136, trees, 109, 123
137, 138, 141, 143, 148, 149, 151, 163, 197, 211, trial, 44, 145, 188, 189, 191, 192, 193, 257, 260, 261
218, 240, 241, 246, 248, 251 triggers, 152, 156, 170
third party, 26 trust, 11, 180
Thomas Kuhn, 113 tuition, 28
Thomson, 30, 112, 129 tumors, 116
thorns, 116 Tunisia, 37
threat, 205 tutoring, 20, 24, 34
threatening, 163 twins, 93
threats, 225 two-way, 12
three-dimensional, 90 Type I error, 220
threshold, 58, 67 typology, 190, 201
thresholds, 61
tics, 19
time, 4, 5, 10, 12, 17, 21, 23, 26, 27, 28, 253, 254, U
255, 256, 257, 258, 259
UK, 23, 33, 257
time constraints, 187
uncertainty, 158, 159, 161, 163, 177, 180, 191
time consuming, 163, 164
undergraduate, 153, 172, 211, 212
timing, 225, 254
undergraduates, 121
title, 37
UNESCO, 18
Title III, 15, 27
unification, 116
toddlers, 245
uniform, 75, 76, 93
Tongue, 228
United Kingdom, 131, 190
top-down, 48, 57
United Nations, 8, 9, 13, 17, 37
total product, 133
United States, 4, 13, 34, 41, 186, 190, 194, 248
tracking, 22, 46, 63
univariate, 232
trade, 6, 14, 15, 37, 258
universe, 74, 77, 89, 110, 111
trade-off, 258
universities, 3, 6, 8, 9, 14, 15, 21
tradition, 82, 108
university education, 72
training, xi, xiv, 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 7, 13, 16, 19, 20, 21,
university students, 255
24, 28, 29, 30, 32, 33, 37, 68, 73, 98, 135, 139,
updating, 184, 187
144, 148, 150, 171, 175, 181, 182, 187, 189, 192
uplink, 256
training programs, 30
upload, 23
traits, 108, 110, 114, 115, 116, 121, 124
urology, 193
trans, 250
users, 5, 6, 12, 16, 22, 24, 254, 255, 256, 257, 258
transcript, 26
transcripts, 23
Index 281
watches, 85, 99
V water, 109, 123
web, 3, 4, 6, 7, 9, 14, 15, 16, 22, 23, 26, 27, 28, 33,
vaccination, 177, 184
34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 89, 246, 251, 254, 255, 257
vacuum, 75, 79, 85, 101
web pages, 4, 9, 16, 23, 254
validation, 97, 201, 261
web-based, 4, 6, 15, 27, 36, 38
validity, 72, 78, 80, 122, 201, 220, 225
webcasting, 253, 254, 256, 257, 258, 259, 260
values, xv, 11, 82, 177, 178, 182, 183, 184, 185, 187,
websites, 4, 9, 14, 15, 28, 29, 31, 255
215, 219, 220
wellbeing, 113, 253
variability, 58, 180
Western Europe, 226
variables, 134, 231
wheelchair, 8
variance, 225
windows, 241, 250, 256
variation, xiii, 55, 62, 68, 107, 114, 115, 159, 180,
wireless, 12
221, 224
women, 169, 172, 177, 184, 186, 188, 190, 194, 260
vegetables, 255
wood, 227
vehicles, 202
word frequency, 57
vein, 55
word naming, 49
vibration, 80
word processing, 27
Victoria, 14, 31
word recognition, 57, 64, 66, 67, 68
video, 256, 258
workers, xi, 147
virtual reality, 4, 6
Workforce Investment Act, 15
virtual world, 6
working memory, 58, 66, 67, 152, 153
virus, 219
workload, 158
visible, 59, 151
World War, 82
vision, 9, 19, 22, 27, 241
World Wide Web, 4, 9, 16
visual environment, 54
worldview, 111
vocabulary, xii, 39, 56, 60, 61, 246
writing, xiii, 12, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 63, 131, 132,
vocational, xii, 1, 2, 20, 21, 36
133, 134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 139, 140, 141, 142,
vocational education, 20, 36
143, 144, 145, 147, 148, 149, 150, 151, 152, 153,
vocational training, xii, 1, 2
154, 160, 231, 245
voice, 6, 23, 28, 42, 56, 65, 212, 244, 257, 259
writing process, xiii, 131, 132, 134, 135, 137, 138,
VoIP, 23
140, 141, 142, 144, 145, 147, 148, 151, 152
voting, 17
writing tasks, 135, 138, 139, 142, 147
Vygotsky, 208, 213, 241
WWW, 36
W Y
W3C, 9, 24, 38
yield, 42, 61, 208, 220, 258
walking, 228
young adults, 31, 53, 259
Wall Street Journal, 34, 38
younger children, 119
Washington, 261