Influence of Language Load On Speech Motor Skill in Children With Specific Language Impairment
Influence of Language Load On Speech Motor Skill in Children With Specific Language Impairment
Research Article
Purpose: Children with specific language impairment (SLI) assessed phonetic accuracy, speech movement variability,
show particular deficits in the generation of sequenced and duration.
action: the quintessential procedural task. Practiced imitation Results: Children with SLI produced more variable
of a sequence may become rote and require reduced articulatory movements than peers with typical
procedural memory. This study explored whether speech development in the high load condition. The groups
motor deficits in children with SLI occur generally or only in converged in the low load condition. Children with
conditions of high linguistic load, whether speech motor SLI continued to show increased articulatory stability
deficits diminish with practice, and whether it is beneficial to over 3 practice sessions. Both groups produced
incorporate conditions of high load to understand speech generated sentences with increased duration and
production. variability compared with repeated sentences.
Method: Children with SLI and typical development Conclusions: Linguistic demands influence speech motor
participated in a syntactic priming task during which they production. Children with SLI show reduced speech motor
generated sentences (high linguistic load) and, then, practiced performance in tasks that require language generation but
repeating a sentence (low load) across 3 sessions. We not when task demands are reduced in rote practice.
S
pecific language impairment (SLI) has classically speech motor deficits may be broadly distributed and general
been defined as a disorder that is constrained to or may be specific to particular aspects of performance.
language, particularly to morphosyntax (Leonard, The latter option may involve demands related to sequen-
2014). However, over the last several years, it has become tial movement (Goffman, 1999; Hsu & Bishop, 2014;
evident that children diagnosed with SLI experience Lum, Gelgic, & Conti-Ramsden, 2010; Tomblin, Mainela-
other deficits related to nonlinguistic cognitive processing Arnold, & Zhang, 2007) or cognitive–linguistic processing
(Ellis Weismer, Plante, Jones, & Tomblin, 2005; Miller (Goffman, 2004; Heisler, Goffman, & Younger, 2010;
et al., 2006) and to action (Bishop, 2002; Hill, 2001; Vuolo, Maner, Smith, & Grayson, 2000). Furthermore, although
Goffman, & Zelaznik, 2017). In the speech motor domain, children with SLI do not always have overt speech motor
children with SLI show difficulties with the production of deficits, previous research has shown that they commonly
multiword phrases (Brumbach & Goffman, 2014) and weak– demonstrate more variable articulation across multiple
strong stress patterns (Goffman, 1999, 2004). Motor and productions of a sentence (Brumbach & Goffman, 2014). It
is the objective of the present work to determine whether
a
children with SLI demonstrate a deficit in speech motor
The University of Iowa, Iowa City skill that broadly influences performance or that is con-
b
Callier Center for Communication Disorders, Behavioral and Brain
strained to particular components. For example, higher lin-
Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas
guistic load tasks may be impaired, and lower linguistic load
Note that data were collected while Lisa Goffman was with the
or practiced tasks may be unaffected. In the context of the
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue
University, West Lafayette, IN.
current study, we define linguistic load on a continuum, with
tasks with increased formulation demands (such as word
Correspondence to Meredith Saletta: [email protected]
and sentence generation) as relatively high load and those
Editor-in-Chief: Sean Redmond
with lower formulation demands (such as word and sen-
Editor: Jan de Jong
tence imitation) as relatively low load.
Received February 16, 2017
Revision received July 5, 2017
Accepted November 16, 2017 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0066 of publication.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • 1–15 • Copyright © 2018 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1
Downloaded From: https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a University College London User on 03/12/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Analytic Approaches to the Assessment 1999). Quantifying utterance duration can provide insight
of Speech Motor Skill into the influence of processing load on speech produc-
tion but, perhaps, in a different way from those revealed
Articulatory Variability by direct assessment of articulatory movement stability.
Speech motor skill may be measured via articulatory
kinematics. Kinematic measures provide a direct assess-
ment of the movements of the articulatory structures that Influence of Load on Articulatory
are used for speech production. One central measure is Variability and Duration
articulatory variability. The spatiotemporal index (STI) is
one measure that has been used to analyze articulatory In both adults and children, imitation and repetition
variability across repeated utterances (Smith, Goffman, paradigms have been used to assess how relatively high
Zelaznik, Ying, & McGillem, 1995). Younger children levels of language processing may influence the lower level
demonstrate more variable oral speech movements than adults components of speech production. Findings on the basis
(Goffman & Smith, 1999; Green, Moore, Higashikawa, & of imitation paradigms have provided evidence that multi-
Steeve, 2000; Nip & Green, 2013; Smith & Goffman, 1998), ple linguistic levels influence speech production.
and adults who stutter or who have Parkinson’s disease
demonstrate more variable oral speech movements than Articulatory Variability
healthy adults (Kleinow & Smith, 2000; Walsh & Smith, In the production of sentences, Maner, Smith, and
2011). Most relevant to the current study, children with Grayson (2000) showed that young children used increased
SLI demonstrate more variable oral speech movements articulatory variability when imitating utterances that
than children with typical development (TD; Brumbach & were embedded in longer and more complex sentence frames,
Goffman, 2014; Goffman, 1999). However, there is a dual as compared with imitating the same phrase in isolation.
nature of high variability. For example, Green and Nip Kleinow and Smith (2000) asked adults who stutter to
(2010) discuss how young children may demonstrate tran- produce phrases in isolation and then the same phrases
sient episodes of increased variability during times of rapid embedded in sentences with low and high syntactic com-
development or the emergence of new speech skills, pre- plexity (defined as conjoined compared with embedded
sumably because the child is in a period of reorganization clauses that were matched for length). In their study, adults
in pursuit of the best coordinative approach. In these who stutter produced utterances with higher variability
cases, increased variability in development may actually than normally fluent adults and were particularly suscepti-
be a positive or adaptive strategy. ble to increases in syntactic complexity.
In the production of lexical stress, Goffman (1999,
Speech Duration 2004) explored children’s variability and modulation
Analyses of duration have also been used to infer ar- of articulatory movement (i.e., whether weak syllables
ticulatory skills in young children (classically studied by are produced with relatively short and small amplitude
Eguchi & Hirsch, 1969). In the acoustic domain, utter- and/or variable articulatory movements), both in content
ance durations are often used as a proxy for motor skill (Goffman, 1999; Goffman & Malin, 1999) and function
or processing load. By measuring duration, researchers can (Goffman, 2004) words. Articulatory movement measures
determine whether it takes longer for a particular age group revealed production differences as a function of linguistic
or population to produce given nonwords, words, or sen- context (or load), with content words produced differently
tences, with duration differences used to infer processing from function words and iambs produced differently
load. Sevald and Dell (1994) provide a model for the ways from trochees. Surprisingly, iambs were produced with
in which processing and planning affect utterance duration. more articulatory stability than trochees, suggesting that in-
Previous findings have demonstrated that children are creased load may lead to increased rather than decreased
slower than adults (Grigos & Patel, 2007; Smith & Goffman, stability. Finally, when considering lexical–semantic load,
1998; Smith & Zelaznik, 2004; Walsh & Smith, 2002) and Heisler, Goffman, and Younger (2010) and Heisler and
that individuals with speech and/or language disorders are Goffman (2016) demonstrated that adding a lexical repre-
slower than those with typical speech and language (Strand sentation (visual or functional) in a novel word–learning
& McNeil, 1996). Durations may change when task de- task resulted in decreased articulatory variability in children
mands, such as those related to syntactic complexity or with TD and children with SLI.
length, increase (Brumbach & Goffman, 2014; Dromey &
Benson, 2003; Maner, Smith, & Grayson, 2000; Sadagopan Duration
& Smith, 2008). However, duration and articulatory vari- Utterance durations are often used as a proxy for
ability are not necessarily correlated and thus may reveal processing load. In young children, Goffman (2004) found
different underlying production processes (Brumbach & that function words were produced with shorter dura-
Goffman, 2014; Goffman & Smith, 1998; Maner, Smith, & tions than weak syllables of content words. Children also
Grayson, 2000). Most relevant to the current study, chil- use duration to distinguish between declaratives and in-
dren with SLI are slower at speech production, at least in terrogatives (Grigos & Patel, 2007). Adults, but not chil-
some tasks, compared with children with TD (Goffman, dren, reduce durations when producing sentences that are
Note. SLI = specific language impairment; TD = typical development; SES = socioeconomic status; CMMS = Columbia Mental Maturity
Scale; SPELT = Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; EVT = Expressive Vocabulary
Test; BBTOP = Bankson-Bernthal Test of Phonology; MABC = Movement Assessment Battery for Children; CELF Preschool-2 = Clinical
Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool–Second Edition.
“Pat the Bunny” (Kunhardt, 2001). Next, to assure famil- sentence “Mom pats the puppy” 10 times consecutively.
iarity with the subjects and objects included in the experi- This is similar to prior tasks used to assess speech motor
mental sentences, photographs of these were presented and variability in children (Smith & Goffman, 1998; Smith &
labeled. Finally, the child completed three practice trials Zelaznik, 2004).
demonstrating his or her understanding of the priming par-
adigm before beginning the experimental task. The sen-
tences and their accompanying illustrations were presented Data Processing
via PowerPoint at a comfortable loudness level. If a child The kinematic data from the target sentences “Mom
produced errors in target sentences (such as substituting pats the puppy” and “Mom pats the baby” were processed
“dog” for “puppy” or producing a syntactic error), the ex- in customized MATLAB routines (The Mathworks, 2009).
aminer would remind the child of the correct production and When processing the kinematic data, some productions
encourage him or her to reattempt the sentence. During the could not be used, including those that contained extra or
first experimental session, the children engaged in the scaf- missing syllables or phonemes, interruptions in the speech
folded priming task. Imitated sentences were not analyzed. signal, extra opening and closing of the lips, or IREDs
missing from the cameras’ view. Incorrect but consistent
productions were analyzed if they outnumbered correct
Sentence Repetition Task productions, that is, if the child said “Mom petted the
At the end of the first and during the second and puppy” eight times and “Mom pats the puppy” twice, the
third sessions, the child was instructed to repeat the target eight consistent productions were used.
Figure 2. Extracted movement sequences from a child with SLI’s productions of the sentence, “Mom pats the puppy.” The left column represents
productions elicited by priming; the right column represents repeated productions. The top two panels represent the raw movement records.
The middle two panels represent the same records, now time normalized and amplitude normalized. The bottom two panels represent the
standard deviations of the normalized records, the sums of which compose the spatiotemporal index (STI) values for lip aperture, or the lip
aperture index (LA index). SLI = specific language impairment.
Table 3. Children’s word choice errors and syntactic errors while completing the priming task.
Group Puppy: nontarget word choice Puppy: syntactic error Baby: nontarget word choice Baby: syntactic error
PrimePuppy – Rep1 8.9278 1.0045 136 8.888 < .0001 < .0001
PrimePuppy – Rep2 10.7400 1.0045 136 10.692 < .0001 < .0001
PrimePuppy – Rep3 11.1486 1.0045 136 11.099 < .0001 < .0001
TD, PrimeBaby – SLI, PrimeBaby −3.1811 1.6822 128 −1.891 0.0609 0.1218
TD, PrimePuppy – SLI, PrimePuppy −4.2722 1.6822 128 −2.540 0.0123 0.0492
TD, PrimeBaby – TD, PrimePuppy −1.1522 1.4205 136 −0.811 0.4187 0.6090
SLI, PrimeBaby – SLI, PrimePuppy −2.2433 1.4205 136 −1.579 0.1166 0.2073
TD, Rep1 – SLI, Rep1 −3.5767 1.6822 128 −2.126 0.0354 0.0944
TD, Rep2 – SLI, Rep2 −1.5133 1.6822 128 −0.900 0.3700 0.5920
TD, Rep3 – SLI, Rep3 −1.1361 1.6822 128 −0.675 0.5007 0.6162
TD, Rep1 – TD, Rep2 0.7806 1.4205 136 0.549 0.5836 0.6669
SLI, Rep1 – SLI, Rep2 2.8439 1.4205 136 2.002 0.0473 0.1081
TD, Rep1 – TD, Rep3 1.0006 1.4205 136 0.704 0.4824 0.6162
SLI, Rep1 – SLI, Rep3 3.4411 1.4205 136 2.422 0.0167 0.0536
TD, Rep2 – TD, Rep3 0.2200 1.4205 136 0.155 0.8772 0.8772
SLI, Rep2 – SLI, Rep3 0.5972 1.4205 136 0.420 0.6748 0.7198
Note. FDR = false discovery rate; SLI = specific language impairment; TD = typical development.
this, we again observed the pairwise comparisons between children with SLI between the first and third sessions,
group and repetition session (see Table 4). There was not although the size is small, p = .02, FDR = .05. This indi-
a significant effect of group for any of the three repetitions, cates that, in conditions of low formulation demands,
p = .04, .37, .50, and FDR = .09, .59, .62, for Sessions 1–3, children with SLI show similar articulatory variability to
respectively. The effect of session was significant only for their peers with TD. Importantly, with imitation practice,
Figure 3. Speech stability by group and task. SLI = specific language impairment; STI = spatiotemporal index; TD = typical development.
PrimePuppy – Rep1 0.4572 0.0690 136 6.629 < .0001 < .0001
PrimePuppy – Rep2 0.4861 0.0733 136 6.636 < .0001 < .0001
PrimePuppy – Rep3 0.5117 0.0725 136 7.061 < .0001 < .0001
TD, PrimeBaby – SLI, PrimeBaby −0.0339 0.1620 34 −0.209 .8356 .9244
TD, PrimePuppy – SLI, PrimePuppy −0.2456 0.1805 34 −1.361 .1826 .3651
TD, PrimeBaby – TD, PrimePuppy 0.0333 0.1003 136 0.332 .7402 .9244
SLI, PrimeBaby – SLI, PrimePuppy −0.1783 0.1003 136 −1.777 .0778 .2073
TD, Rep1 – SLI, Rep1 −0.0167 0.0985 34 −0.169 .8666 .9244
TD, Rep2 – SLI, Rep2 0.0200 0.0881 34 0.227 .8219 .9244
TD, Rep3 – SLI, Rep3 0.0689 0.0894 34 0.771 .4463 .7933
TD, Rep1 – TD, Rep2 0.0106 0.0304 136 0.348 .7286 .9244
SLI, Rep1 – SLI, Rep2 0.0472 0.0304 136 1.555 .1222 .2792
TD, Rep1 – TD, Rep3 0.0117 0.0304 136 0.384 .7015 .9244
SLI, Rep1 – SLI, Rep3 0.0972 0.0304 136 3.201 .0017 .0068
TD, Rep2 – TD, Rep3 0.0011 0.0194 136 0.057 .9545 .9545
SLI, Rep2 – SLI, Rep3 0.0500 0.0194 136 2.575 .0111 .0355
Note. FDR = false discovery rate; SLI = specific language impairment; TD = typical development.
inquired whether practice in imitating the sentence (that the interaction term was not statistically significant because
is, as the task transitions between an emphasis on high planned analyses of interest were the pairwise compari-
formulation demands to a task comprising lower formula- sons, we evaluated all of the pairwise comparisons while
tion demands) would cause the two groups to converge. adjusting the p value with a suitable Type I error adjust-
We address each of our predictions below. ment, FDR. As predicted, the mean variability score was
significantly different between SLI and TD in the priming
condition, but the group means were not significantly
The Speech Motor Deficit in Children With SLI different in the repetition condition. In addition, practice
First, we predicted that children with SLI would facilitated motor learning in children with SLI.
demonstrate higher speech movement variability and lon- Our findings indicate that articulatory variability
ger utterance durations. Our findings indicate that the and utterance duration decreased by Session 3 of practice
SLI group produced more variable speech movements in for children with SLI, indicating motor learning with prac-
both tasks, but that there were no group differences in tice even in children with potential deficiencies in speech
duration. We also found that both groups had more vari- motor learning. The TD group did not show this differ-
able movements and longer utterance durations in the con- ence. This indicates that, as the demands of the experi-
dition of high formulation demands than in the condition mental task diminished, the two groups converged, and
of low formulation demands. This indicated that the re- children with SLI no longer showed differences from their
duced demands associated with imitation facilitated stable peers with TD.
and relatively fast articulatory movements in both groups. These discoveries contribute to characterization of
Second, we predicted that group differences would the motor deficit in children with SLI. Our comparison of
be higher when formulation demands were higher, in that speech production in conditions of high formulation de-
children with SLI would be especially susceptible to this mands versus conditions of motor practice/rote recitation
manipulation. Specifically, both groups would demonstrate revealed that children with SLI do not have a generalized
higher variability in the condition of high formulation speech motor deficit. Actually, the differences between
demands, but with motor practice and lower formulation speech motor production in children with SLI versus chil-
demands, group differences would diminish. Even though dren with TD appeared predominately in the condition