Post-Stroke Insomnia
Post-Stroke Insomnia
com/scientificreports
A substantive body of evidence has documented a close relationship between stroke and sleep disordered breath-
ing1. However other changes in sleep after stroke are less well researched2. According to a recent meta-analysis3
examining the literature on sleep physiology after stroke, the majority of these studies are conducted in inpatient
populations, often lack an adequate control group and frequently do not include an adaptation night. The pol-
ysomnographic, (PSG)-derived evidence on sleep in stroke is therefore limited.
Physiological (polysomnographic, PSG) sleep studies are critical as this methodology affords the comprehen-
sive and objective characterization of sleep parameters as well as the diagnosis of organic sleep disorders (includ-
ing sleep apnea). Moreover, the theoretical conceptualization of the processes involved in sleep, and its relation
to physical and mental health, has been largely derived from PSG studies in neurologically healthy volunteers.
Physiological sleep studies in patients therefore represent a necessary step to draw on the wealth of knowledge
obtained from basic sleep research. In addition, it is clear that sleep difficulties are a common problem in society,
and their prevalence and severity is increasing with age. Calibrating the observations made in stroke patients
against otherwise healthy participants is therefore important as it will allow the consideration of these findings in
the context of care pathways and public health initiatives on sleep more generally.
1
School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK. 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical
Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany. 3University Hospital
of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Bern, Switzerland. 4Sleep Wake Epilepsy Center, Neuroscience Center, Bern,
Switzerland. 5Department of Neurology, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany. 6Department
of Neuroradiology, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg,
Germany. 7Department of Neurology, University Hospital Bern, Bern, Switzerland. Correspondence and requests for
materials should be addressed to A.S. (email: [email protected])
Sleep difficulties and daytime sleepiness are frequently experienced in those with stroke. For example,
questionnaire- and actigraphy-based studies report chronic sleep difficulties in 34–67% of patients4,5, with 48%
satisfying the criteria for insomnia6. 17–34% of patients with stroke further report disturbed daytime function-
ing due to excessive sleepiness4,7,8 and fatigue (30–69%)9–13. Daytime electroencephalographic (EEG) measures
in chronic motor stroke further indicate greater prevalence of slower frequencies in resting state wake EEG14, a
phenomenon often interpreted as a marker of sleep deprivation15. Together these findings suggest that sleep and
daytime sleepiness represent a continued problem for those with stroke. Critically, the subjective experience of
sleep problems does not always map onto objective physiological sleep parameters. For example, in the study by
Herron14, the EEG findings would suggest greater daytime sleepiness, however, the objective sleepiness ratings
were similar to those of controls.
The considerations summarized above highlight the need for the acquisition of PSG data and physiological
measures of daytime sleep propensity in patients in the chronic stroke phase of stroke and age-matched controls
using two gold standard methodologies, polysomnography and the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT).
In the present study 22 community dwelling patients with first ever stroke at least 12 months prior to the data
collection and 22 sex and aged matched controls were tested in a protocol comprising two consecutive nights of
PSG conducted in the controlled environment of a sleep laboratory, followed by the MSLT after night two. The
study enrolled patients with right-hemispheric stroke and sustained motor deficits to ensure a homogeneous
cohort and to keep potential confounding factors to a minimum. For practical and ethical reasons, the motor
deficit was mild since patients had to be able to dress and eat independently, and be comfortable within the labo-
ratory setting. Physiological sleep characteristics indicative of sleep continuity and architecture, and the spectral
EEG composition, were extracted from the PSG recorded on the second night. In line with best practice standards
in PSG research, the first night served as an adaptation night and its data were not analysed. Daytime sleepiness
measures comprised sleep latency and wake efficiency, as well as vigilance performance. In addition to the physi-
ological data obtained through PSG and MSLT, participants also completed a sleep diary and wore actiwatches for
two weeks prior to the first night in the sleep laboratory, and completed a number psychological scales, including
the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; see methods for details).
Importantly these measures were obtained from patients and control partipants living independently in the com-
munity so that potential sleep difficulties in those living with the consequences of stroke could be compared to
those living in similar age-typical situations, without the additional physiological and psychological challenges
arising from having had, and living with the consequences of, a stroke.
Results
Subjective Sleep. Significant differences in global PSQI scores suggested poorer sleep in patients than
controls (6.9 ± 3.1 vs 5.1 ± 2.8, F1,38 = 3.68, p < 0.05 with ŋp2 = 0.09). Thereby patients were more likely to be
categorized as poor sleepers (14/19 vs 10/21) when using the ≥ 5 PSQI criterion (odds ratio: 3.08, p = 0.049).
Patients also reported significantly poorer sleep efficiency than controls (80.1 ± 10.3% vs 89.9 ± 10.8%, F1,31 = 7.1,
p < 0.01; ŋp2 = 0.186) in their sleep diaries. However, diary reports for total sleep time (patients: 422.6 ± 47.7 mins;
controls: 433.3 ± 63.4 mins), number of wake periods (patients: 0.87 ± 9; controls: 0.9 ± 0.7), and daytime sleep
(patients: 5.68 ± 8.7 mins; controls: 4.9 ± 9.4 mins) were not different between the groups.
PSG. No significant group differences were observed for the apnea hypopnea index (AHI: patients: 9.8 ± 7.0;
controls: 7.6 ± 7.5, F1,38 = 0.94, p = 0.37, ŋp2 = 0.024) and periodic limb movements (patients: 12.08 ± 14.78, con-
trols: 8.04 ± 10.13, F1,38 = 0.94, p = 0.31, ŋp2 = 0.026).
Sleep stage parameters are shown in Table 1. Patients had significantly longer sleep latencies (23.9 ± 17.5
vs 15.9 ± 9.0 minutes; F1,38 = 4.43, p = 0.035, ŋp2 = 0.083) and poorer sleep efficiency (72.0 ± 11.9 vs 79.1 ± 9.0;
F1,38 = 4.70, p = 0.018, ŋp2 = 0.110). At the same time patients had significantly more WASO (102.7 ± 44.4 vs
72.1 ± 40.8 minutes, F1,38 = 5.15, p = 0.014, ŋp2 = 0.119) and more % wake than controls (20.6 ± 9.2 vs 14.7 ± 7.7%;
F1,38 = 4.82, p = 0.017, ŋp2 = 0.113). Lesion size did thereby not correlate with these variables.
The remaining variables showed a numerical pattern of group differences pointing towards poorer sleep in
patients, however these numerical differences were not significant. The spectral analysis revealed significantly
greater gamma power during NREM sleep in patients than contols (F1,38 = 3.52, p = 0.034, ŋp2 = 0.085).
MSLT. The data is illustrated in Fig. 1. Wake efficiency showed main effects for session (F1.38 = 5.95,
p = 0.01, ŋp2 0.135) and group respectively (F1.38 = 4.91, p < 0.01, ŋp2 0.114); the session x group interaction was
non-significant (p = 0.89). N2 latencies showed a significant session effect (F3,114 = 4.2, p < 0.01) but no group
effect or interaction. However, visual inspection of the means suggested a tendency for longer N2 latencies in
patients towards the end of the day. We therefore calculated the ANOVA across the timepoints 11am, 1 pm, 3 pm
and 5 pm. This revealed a main effect of group (F1,38 = 3.9, p < 0.05) with significantly longer N2 latencies for
patients at 1 pm, 3 pm, and 5 pm. Subjective sleepiness ratings (KSS) ranged from 2.2 to 3.7 for patients and 2.2 to
3.5 for controls and were not significantly different at group level.
The group x session ANOVA of the vigilance performance data (TAP) revealed a main effect session
(F1,38 = 2.82; p = 0.021; ŋp2 = 0.069) for errors, but no main effect group or interaction, and no effects for omis-
sions. Subsidary analysis through planned t-tests further indicated similar errors in the two groups for the 10am
session (t = 3.41, p = 0.37) but significantly poorer performance in patients for the noon (t = 1.76, p = 0.04), 2 pm
(t = 1.82, p = 0.04) and 4 pm sessions (t = 2.13, p = 0.02). For omissions, patient performance was significantly
poorer the 2 pm session only (t = 2.00, p = 0.026).
Table 1. mean and standard error of sleep stage characteristics; *time in minutes.
Discussion
The present study examined physiological characteristics of sleep and daytime sleepiness in chronic stroke
patients compared to age-matched healthy controls living in the community. Results from a 2-night PSG proto-
col followed by the Multiple Sleep Latency Test revealed that compared to controls, patients differ in measures
of sleep continuity, sleep architecture, and spectral EEG characteristics, and are less likely to fall asleep during
the day as indicated by the MSLT. In detail, the sleep EEG indicates longer sleep latencies and lower sleep effi-
ciency. This was reflected by more time awake since sleep onset and greater % wake. At the same time, total sleep
time was similar between the groups. Nocturnal EEG power was greater for patients in higher frequency bands.
During the day patients fell asleep less easily but were more prone to errors in a vigilance task. These group
differences were observed despite similar levels of sleep disordered breathing and periodic limb movements in
both groups. In addition patients consistently reported poorer sleep efficiency in their sleep diaries and were
more likely to be categorized as poor sleepers on the PSQI. However, subjective measures of fatigue and day-
time sleepiness were similar between the two groups, highlighting a discrepancy between sleepiness perception
and physiological sleepiness measures. Importantly, the present study found that sleep efficiency was reduced in
patients while total sleep time was similar to controls. Total sleep time represents a purely physiological meas-
ure of sleep continuity, while sleep efficiency represents a superimposition of physiological and behavioral fac-
tors. This suggests that the lesions caused by the stroke have not impacted the synchronized slow oscillations in
the cortico-thalamo-cortical feed-back loop relevant for effective sleep-wake regulation16, and hence renders
the explanation of a lesion-induced ineffective sleep-wake regulation less likely. However, this interpretation is
most likely only true for mild strokes, and further research is clearly necessary to ascertain the proposition that
poorer sleep in chronic stroke may, at least in a subset of patients, occur without damage to sleep-wake regulating
networks.
To the best of our knowledge the present study is the first PSG-MSLT study in community-dwelling patients
with a chronicity >12 months. Our findings extend and substantiate the existing data for sustained sleep difficul-
ties in chronic stroke by providing objective physiological evidence in a well-controlled sample. Taken together
these findings suggests that normal sleep homeostasis, i.e. increasing daytime sleepiness after poor nighttime
sleep, is compromised in patients. Reduced sleep continuity and increased power in higher frequency bands at
night, combined with greater wake efficiency during the day, are typical characteristics of insomnia17. Our results
may therefore be interpreted as reflecting a form of insomnia.
The group differences observed in the present study were obtained by contrasting a relatively healthy stroke
population (i.e. relatively low levels of depression, no sleep-affecting drugs) with a group of healthy ‘normal
sleepers’ rather than ‘good sleepers’. The latter is often used in sleep research with the rational that good sleep
is the healthy state and should hence be used for benchmarking sleep in other groups. Here we chose not to go
down this route to avoid an overestimation of group differences. The recruitment strategy for controls further
had an element of recruitment bias such that those more conscious about sleep (e.g. because their sleep is not as
good as they would like) were more likely to notice flyers and respond to them. While this argument also holds
in principle for patients, the fact that they were actively contacted via the clinic and given information about the
study outright makes a self-selection bias towards poor sleepers less likely for patients than controls. The group
differences observed in the present study are therefore likely to be more pronounced in the wider population.
The present findings were obtained from patients living independent lives in the community rather than
patients residing in rehabilitation or other care settings at the time of the experiment. This is an important aspect
of our study because it makes the two study groups more comparable and hence less prone to group-specific con-
founding factors. Because both groups were of good psychological health, and both had low levels of depression, it
is unlikely that the poor sleep observed in patients can simply be explained by differences in mental health status.
In the present study all patients had a right-hemispheric lesion. The sleep staging was therefore conducted for
electrode C3 over the left (nonlesioned) hemisphere. While generally the non-lesioned hemisphere is considered
to be ‘healthy’, the possibility of compensatory changes in homologous areas and/or the wider cortex is genuine,
and these changes might have affected the EEG signal and hence the PSG parameters. However, the changes
observed for the PSG parameters were not uniform but specifically affected certain aspects of sleep physiology.
Moreover, the objective indicators for an insomnia-like disorder was corroborated by the sleep diaries and the
ratings on the PSQI. We would therefore argue that any differences in the EEG signal caused by the lesion are
unlikely to represent an epiphenomenon but are functionally relevant to sleep. This argument is underpinned by
the absence of significant correlations between lesion size and sleep parameters.
Figure 1. Top 2 panels shows makers of sleep propensity obtained from the MSLT for patients and controls.
The bottom panel shows the error rates obtained in the TAP.
One model frequently used to explain the pathomechanisms of insomnia is the hyperarousal model18. This
model notes the importance of physiological and psychological factors in the disease and treatment mechanisms
of insomnia, and provides a possible explanatory framework for the present findings. Specifically, we propose that
the insult induces physiological and psychological changes that are prone to trigger disturbed sleep in the early
phases of stroke recovery. These early sleep disturbances are well documented3,19,20. Moreover, depression and
fatigue are highly prevalent in the acute and postactue phase21,22, and both aspects can negatively affect and/or
interact with sleep23–25. Perpetuating factors arising from psychological processes, such as maladaptive behaviors
and dysfunctional beliefs, as well as physiological aspects including lesion characteristics and medication, can
maintain these early sleep difficulties and lead to a persistent sleep disorder in the chronic state as predicted by
Spielman’s 3 P model26. Should these assumptions be correct it would follow that early psychological intervention
could reduce the likelihood of sustained sleep difficulties in the chronic phase of stroke. The potential role for
psychological or bhavioural factors in disease maintenance is supported further by our observation that sleep
efficiency but not total sleep time is reduced in patients. Both measures are reflective of sleep continuity. However,
while total sleep time represents a purely physiological measure, sleep efficiency represents a superimposition of
physiology and bedtime behaviour.
An alternative explanation to the one offered above could be a general reduction of sleep pressure resulting
from lower levels of physical activity in patients. This, however, is unlikely to fully explain the findings. Firstly,
because sleep efficiency was poorer in patients than controls, their tendency to fall asleep during the day should
be higher than in controls. But the contrary is the case: patients have poorer sleep efficiency at night and greater
wake efficiency during the day. Secondly the patients were relatively well recovered and lived an active life, a
notion corroborated by the subscores on the stroke impact scale. However, the actigraphy data, obtained in the
fortnight prior to the experiment, revealed significantly lower activity counts for patients than controls (199 ± 94
vs 315 ± 146; p < 0.01). While one could argue that actigraphy data in a person with hemiparesis is a questionable
proxy for the level of physical activity, it is a genuine possibility that patients were less physically active, and that
this might have negatively impacted their sleep homoestasis. However, the laboratory situation controlled critical
zeitgebers, sleep-wake pattern and, to some extent, physical activity. Reduced sleep pressure therefore offers a
limited explanation for the present data. The issue, however, highlights the contribution of health behaviors to the
pathomechanism of post stroke insomnia, and hence potential pathways for non-pharmacological intervention.
In addition to the arguments above it is further noteworthy that actigraphy counts in patients with hemipare-
sis are typically calibrated against the less-affected limb, and this was not done in the present study. Measures from
the affected limb are likely to underrepresent movement, which further strengthens the argument that sedentary
lifestyle alone is unlikely to be an exhaustive explanation for the poor sleep efficiency observed in our study.
A further possible contributor to poorer sleep efficiency could be greater pain or discomfort in patients. While
this possibility cannot be ruled out entirely, it is an unlikely explanation given that patients in the present study
had mild hemiparesis and were screened for excessive pain.
While the present study was well matched and shows no significant differences for any of the critical demo-
graphic variables, it is true that numerically the stroke group contains more men, is older and has higher BMI.
All these factors are typically associated with more disrupted sleep. On statistical grounds there is no room to
argue that the sleep-related group differences were due to any of these factors, as none of them were significant.
However, the possibility that the combined effect of the numerical differences in these three parameters might
have contributed to the sleep difference between the groups, although unlikely, cannot be entirely excluded.
The latest NICE guidelines for stroke rehabilitation issued by the National Health Service in the UK do not men-
tion sleep27. This omission stands in contrast to the recognized importance of sleep for physical and mental health28.
We therefore argue that sleep is a concern in stroke and hence should be given greater consideration in stroke care.
For example, poor sleep increases risks, such as the likelihood for cognitive failures or falls29–31. These risks, and the
impact thereof, are likely to be aggravated in older persons with a disability (i.e. in this case stroke). In addition,
age-related comorbidities, such as hypertension, and sleep are strongly linked28,32–34. Sleep disordered breathing
represents the biggest single risk factor for recurrent strokes35. 29% of stroke survivors suffer clinical levels of depres-
sion36, a condition strongly associated with poor sleep37. In addition, sleep is conducive to skill learning38,39, while
motor learning is known to be reduced in insomnia40,41, and hence likely to mediate rehabilitation outcome3,42–44. A
better understanding of sleep in patients long after the incident, and, more importantly the mechanisms leading to
these sleep difficulties might therefore represent an opportunity to improve quality of life and long-term outcome.
Conclusion
The present study provides evidence for an insomnia-like sleep disorder in the chronic phase of stroke. Given
the importance of sleep for daytime function, psychological wellbeing and health, as well as the proposed role in
neurorehabilitation, these findings highlight the need to encapsulate sleep in stroke care provision. The patho-
mechanisms proposed here may not only be relevant to stroke but also other chronic medical conditions.
Methods
Participants. 21 patients completed the study but two were excluded from further analysis because of techni-
cal problems (i.e. 19 patients; 13 males, 9 females; aged 63.5 ± 8 years). All patients had mild left-sided upper-limb
hemiparesis following right-hemispheric first-ever stroke 13–108 months prior to the study (mean chronicity
26.9 months). Recovery, assessed at the time of study, was good as indicated by a mean global recovery score of
80.3 out of 100 on the Stroke Impact Scale, SIS. Clinical details and selection criteria are listed in Table 2. All par-
ticipants were non-smokers and did not consume more than 2 units of alcohol per day. Patients taking sleep-af-
fecting medication were excluded from the study.
Patient volunteers were recruited via a database of patients previously treated in the Depatment of Neurology
at the University of Freiburg Medical Center. Those fulfilling the selection criteria (see Table 2) were invited by
letter to contact the sleep laboratory. Interested candidates were telephone-screened before completing a full
medical and psychological screening conducted by two of the authors (D.E. & S.F.).
The control group comprised 21 age (±5 years) - and sex-matched healthy volunteers, recruited through
advertisements in local newspapers, flyers in public places, word of mouth and websites. The protocol for tele-
and pre-screening, and the inclusion/exclusion criteria were similar to those for patients. Group statistics of
demographic variables are provided in the lower part of Table 2. Participant enrollment and the study overview
are presented in Fig. 2. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) recordings were obtained during routine diagnostics
and used to extract lesion charateristics (Fig. 3).
The study was approved by the University of Freiburg Medical Center Ethics Committee, and conformed
to the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained prior to the study. Participants received
financial reimbursement for participation.
Scales and Questionnaires. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI45), the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index
(PSQI46) and the Motor Activity Log (MAL47) were administered at screening. Subjective sleepiness during the
day was assessed using the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale48 (KSS). A regular sleep wake pattern was ensured through
sleep diaries and actigraphy obtained for two weeks prior to the study49.
Polysomnography and Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT). The protocol is illustrated in Fig. 4. PSG
was recorded during the two sleep laboratory nights to standardized procedures of the sleep laboratory in the
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy at the University of Freiburg Medical Center50, using a 24-channel
Inclusion criteria
Paresis of left upper limb
Unilateral right-hemispheric stroke
Time post-stroke >12 months
Premorbid right-hand dominant
Exclusion criteria
Inability to clearly communicate/understand instructions
Mini Mental State exam ≤25
Inability to feed, wash or dress independently
Sleep disorder determined through sleep interview
AHI or PLMS >15
Neurological or psychiatric comorbidities
Seizures 6 months before participation
Hemineglect
Shift work or jet lag 4 months before participation
Regular consumption of recreational drugs
Sleep-affecting medication 2 weeks before participation
Patient characteristics Mean ± SD Range
Time post-stroke (months) 26.9 ± 21.7 13/108
Global SIS score 80.3 ± 12.4 50/100
SIS Strength 65.8 ± 18.6 25.0/100.0
SIS Memory 59.6 ± 17.6 14.3/85.7
SIS Emotion 62.5 ± 23.7 22.2/100.0
SIS Communication 74.4 ± 20.9 28.6/100.0
SIS ADL 81.6 ± 20.3 40.0/100.0
SIS Mobility 72.3 ± 23.4 33.3/100.0
SIS Hand Function 63.7 ± 30.5 20.0/100.0
SIS Social Participation 65.7 ± 23.7 18.8/100.0
Lesion volume*, ml 26.9 ± 41.0 0.8/158
Patients Controls F /Χ2 p-value ηp2
sex (m/f) 13/6 14/7 0.014 0.906
family status (in relationship/ living alone) 18/1 16/5 2.69 0.101
Age 63.5 ± 8.0 59.2 ± 8.6 2.57 0.117 0.063
BMI 26.9 ± 3.6 25.3 ± 4.3 1.62 0.211 0.041
years in education 10.1 ± 1.6 10.4 ± 1.7 0.37 0.546 0.010
PSQI 6.9 ± 3.1 5.1 ± 2.8 3.68 0.063 0.090
BDI 5.8 ± 5.2 3.4 ± 4.1 2.64 0.113 0.065
AHI 9.8 ± 7.0 7.6 ± 7.5 0.945 0.37 0.024
PLMS (SPT) 12.08 ± 14.78 8.04 ± 10.13 1.03 0.31 0.026
Table 2. Selection criteria, patient characteristics and demographics. Abbreviations: SIS = Stroke Impact Scale;
ADL = Activities of daily living; AHI = Apopnea-hyponea index; PLMS = Periodic limb movement syndrome;
*imaging data of 17 patients were available.
video-polysomnography system (Leonardo Polygraph, Sagura Medical Equipment, Germany). The first night
served as an adaptation night and was not analysed. The setup included the EEG channels Fz, Cz, Pz, C3, C4, P3,
P4, FP1, FP2, F3, F4, T7, T8, A1 and A2 as reference, submental electromyogram (EMG), vertical and horizontal
electrooculogram (EOG) and electrocardiogram (ECG).
The MSLT used a similar set up. The protocol comprised five 20-minute sessions (9am, 11am, 1 pm, 3 pm,
5 pm), where participants lay in bed in a dim room while the EEG was recorded. In-between these sessions, the
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale and the sustained attention subtest of the Test of Attention Performance (TAP51) were
acquired at 10am, 12 am, 2 pm and 4 pm.
Off-line Analysis. PSG data were 0.5 and 70 Hz bandpass filtered and resampled at 200 Hz for offline analy-
sis. Data collected between lights off in the evening and lights on in the morning were segmented into 30 second
epochs and scored manually at the C3-A2 derivation by accredited sleep technicians using AASM criteria (http://
www.aasmnet.org/practiceguidelines.aspx). Sleep parameters were calculated as described in52 and comprised
measures of sleep continuity, sleep architecture, and REM characteristics as specified in Table 2. For the MSLT, N2
latency and wake efficiency, defined as the ratio between time awake and sleep opportunity (20 minutes respec-
tively), were extracted for further analysis.
Figure 3. Lesion overlap obtained from diffusion-weighed images (1.5 T Siemens Avanto) acquired during
routine diagnostics in the acute stroke phase. Using SPM8 (http://sourceforge.net/projects/spmtools/) lesions
were delineated with MRIcron (http://www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricron/index.html) and normalized into
standard MNI-space (the Z-coordinates are provided). Color bar indicates the number of overlapping lesions.
Figure 4. Flow chart of protocol. Participants arrived at the laboratory in the afternoon of the adaptation
night for a full medical check-up and were given dinner at 7 pm. PSG wire-up started thereafter. The montage
on the adaptation night included a nasal airflow sensor to determine OSA. This sensor was not used in the
experimental night because it is often uncomfortable for participants. In the morning after the adaptation night,
participants were given breakfast and asked to return to the laboratory for the experimental night for 6.30, were
given dinner and wired up. In the morning after the experimental night, the PSG was removed. Participants
were given breakfast before the EEG was wired up for the MSLT starting at 9 am.
Spectral analysis was performed on 4-second windows with 0.49 Hz resolution similar to52. The average power,
referenced to average reference, was calculated for delta (0.1–3.5 Hz), theta (3.5–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), sigma
(12–24 Hz), beta 1 (16–24 Hz), beta 2 (24–32 Hz), gamma (32–48 Hz), and computed for all sleep stages com-
bined, as well as separately for N2 and REM.
Statistics. ANOVAs were calculated for group comparisons. T-tests and Chi square statistics were used for
demographic variables as appropriate. Correlations examined the association of lesion volume and sleep. Unless
otherwise stated, significance levels for group comparisons refer to one-sided testing to reflect the direction-
ality of the hypothesis and to take account for the clear indication of poorer sleep in patients as reflected in
questionnaire-based research.
References
1. Bassetti, C. L. et al. Sleep and stroke. Exp. Neurol. 16, 1–4 (2011).
2. Bassetti, C. L. et al. Neurology and psychiatry: Waking up to opportunities of sleep.: State of the art and clinical/research priorities
for the next decade. Eur. J. Neurol. 22, 1337–1354 (2015).
3. Baglioni, C. et al. Polysomnographic characteristics of sleep in stroke: A Systematic review and meta- analysis. PLoS One 11 (2016).
4. Schuiling, W. J., Rinkel, G. J. E., Walchenbach, R. & de Weerd, A. W. Disorders of sleep and wake in patients after subarachnoid
hemorrhage. Stroke. 36, 578–582 (2005).
5. Sterr, A., Herron, K., Dijk, D.-J. J. & Ellis, J. Time to wake-up: sleep problems and daytime sleepiness in long-term stroke survivors.
Brain Inj 22, 575–579 (2008).
6. Palomaki, H. et al. Complaints of poststroke insomnia and its treatment with mianserin. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 15, 56–62 (2003).
7. Campos, T. F. et al. The sleep-wake cycle in the late stage of cerebral vascular accident recovery. Biol. Rhythm Res. 36, 109–113
(2005).
8. Vock, J. et al. Evolution of sleep and sleep EEG after hemispheric stroke. J. Sleep Res. 11, 331–338 (2002).
9. Choi-Kwon, S., Han, S. W., Kwon, S. U. & Kim, J. S. Poststroke fatigue: characteristics and related factors. Cerebrovasc Dis 19, 84–90
(2005).
10. Ingles, J. L., Eskes, G. A. & Phillips, S. J. Fatigue after stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80, 173–178 (1999).
11. Park, J. Y. et al. Functional outcome in poststroke patients with or without fatigue. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 88, 554–558 (2009).
12. Schepers, V. P., Visser-Meily, A. M., Ketelaar, M. & Lindeman, E. Poststroke fatigue: course and its relation to personal and stroke-
related factors. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87, 184–188 (2006).
13. Hinkle, J. L. et al. Poststroke Fatigue: Emerging Evidence and Approaches to Management: A Scientific Statement for Healthcare
Professionals from the American Heart Association. Stroke 48, e159–e170 (2017).
14. Herron, K., Dijk, D. J. D. D.-J., Dean, P., Seiss, E. & Sterr, A. Quantitative electroencephalography and behavioural correlates of
daytime sleepiness in chronic stroke. BioMed Res. 2014 (2014).
15. Brunner, D. P., Dijk, D. J. & Borbély, A. A. Repeated partial sleep deprivation progressively changes in EEG during sleep and
wakefulness. Sleep 16, 100–13 (1993).
16. Krone, L. et al. Top-down control of arousal and sleep: Fundamentals and clinical implications. Sleep Med. Rev. 31 (2017).
17. Baglioni, C. et al. Sleep changes in the disorder of insomnia: A meta-analysis of polysomnographic studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 18,
195–213 (2014).
18. Riemann, D. et al. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med Rev 14, 19–31 (2010).
19. Leng, Y. et al. Sleep duration and risk of fatal and nonfatal stroke: a prospective study and meta-analysis. Neurology 84, 1072–9
(2015).
20. Hermann, D. M. & Bassetti, C. L. Role of sleep-disordered breathing and sleep-wake disturbances for stroke and stroke recovery.
Neurology 87, 1407–16 (2016).
21. Wu, S., Mead, G., Macleod, M. & Chalder, T. Model of understanding fatigue after stroke. Stroke 46, 893–8 (2015).
22. Choi-Kwon, S. & Kim, J. S. Poststroke Fatigue: An Emerging, Critical Issue in Stroke Medicine. Int. J. Stroke 6, 328–336 (2011).
23. Buysse, D. J. et al. Sleep, Fatigue, and Medical Training: Setting an Agenda for Optimal Learning and Patient Care. Sleep, https://doi.
org/10.1093/sleep/26.2.218 (2003).
24. Theorell-Haglöw, J., Lindberg, E. & Janson, C. What are the important risk factors for daytime sleepiness and fatigue in women?
Sleep, https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/29.6.751 (2006).
25. Irwin, M. R. et al. Sleep loss exacerbates fatigue, depression, and pain in rheumatoid arthritis. Sleep, https://doi.org/10.5665/
sleep.1742 (2012).
26. Spielman, A. Assessment of insomnia. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 6, 11–25 (1986).
27. Dworzynski, K., Ritchie, G., Fenu, E., MacDermott, K. & Playford, E. D. Rehabilitation after stroke: summary of NICE guidance.
BMJ 346, f3615 (2013).
28. Riemann, D. et al. The neurobiology, investigation, and treatment of chronic insomnia. Lancet Neurol. 14, 547–558 (2015).
29. Stone, K. L. et al. Sleep Disturbances and Risk of Falls in Older Community-Dwelling Men: The Outcomes of Sleep Disorders in
Older Men (MrOS Sleep) Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62, 299–305 (2014).
30. Cauley, J. A. et al. Hypoxia During Sleep and the Risk of Falls and Fractures in Older Men: The Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Sleep
Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62, 1853–1859 (2014).
31. Paudel, M. L. et al. Sleep Disturbances and Risk of Hospitalization and Inpatient Days Among Older Women. Sleep 40, (2017).
32. Bassetti, C. L., Milanova, M. & Gugger, M. Sleep-disordered breathing and acute ischemic stroke: diagnosis, risk factors, treatment,
evolution, and long-term clinical outcome. Stroke. 37, 967–972 (2006).
33. Bassetti, C. L. & Hermann, D. M. Sleep and stroke. Handb. Clin. Neurol. /Ed. by P.J. Vinken G.W. Bruyn 99, 1051–1072 (2011).
34. Hermann, D. M. & Bassetti, C. L. Sleep-related breathing and sleep-wake disturbances in ischemic stroke. Neurology 73, 1313–1322
(2009).
35. Birkbak, J., Clark, A. J. & Rod, N. H. The effect of sleep disordered breathing on the outcome of stroke and transient ischemic attack:
a systematic review. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 10, 103–108 (2014).
36. Ayerbe, L., Ayis, S., Wolfe, C. D. & Rudd, A. G. Natural history, predictors and outcomes of depression after stroke: systematic review
and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 202, 14–21 (2013).
37. Baglioni, C. et al. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: A meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. Journal
of Affective Disorders 135, 10–19 (2011).
38. Rasch, B., Pommer, J., Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. Pharmacological REM sleep suppression paradoxically improves rather than
impairs skill memory. Nat Neurosci 12, 396–397 (2009).
39. Diekelmann, S., Buchel, C., Born, J. & Rasch, B. Labile or stable: opposing consequences for memory when reactivated during
waking and sleep. Nat Neurosci 14, 381–386 (2011).
40. Landmann, N. et al. The reorganisation of memory during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 18 (2014).
41. Nissen, C. et al. Impaired sleep-related memory consolidation in primary insomnia–a pilot study. Sleep 29, 1068–1073 (2006).
42. Siengsukon, C. & Boyd, L. A. Sleep enhances off-line spatial and temporal motor learning after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair
23, 327–335 (2009).
43. Siengsukon, C., Al-Dughmi, M., Al-Sharman, A. & Stevens, S. Sleep Parameters, Functional Status, and Time Post-Stroke are
Associated with Offline Motor Skill Learning in People with Chronic Stroke. Front Neurol 6, 225 (2015).
44. Duss, S. B. et al. The role of sleep in recovery following ischemic stroke: A review of human and animal data. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.nbscr.2016.11.003 (2017).
45. Beck, A., Steer, R. & Brown, G. Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). … Beck Depress. Invent. (1993).
46. Buysse, D. J. et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28,
193–213 (1989).
47. Uswatte, G., Taub, E., Morris, D., Vignolo, M. & McCulloch, K. Reliability and validity of the upper-extremity motor activity log-14
for measuring real-world arm use. Stroke 36, 2493–2496 (2005).
48. Akerstedt, T. & Gillberg, M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52, 29–37 (1990).
49. Ancoli-Israel, S. et al. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep 26, 342–392 (2003).
50. Voderholzer, U., Al-Shajlawi, A., Weske, G., Feige, B. & Riemann, D. Are there gender differences in objective and subjective sleep
measures? A study of insomniacs and healthy controls. Depress. Anxiety 17, 162–172 (2003).
51. Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsleistung (TAP). (2007).
52. Feige, B. et al. Does REM sleep contribute to subjective wake time in primary insomnia? A comparison of polysomnographic and
subjective sleep in 100 patients. J. Sleep Res. 17, 180–190 (2008).
Author Contributions
A.S.: study lead M.K.: statistical analysis, write up C.N.: theoretical conception, data interpretation and write up
D.R.: data interpretation, write up D.E.: recruitment, medical assessment, data collection (sleep) S.F.: recruitment,
medical assessment, data collection (sleep) B.F.: analysis support (data management and quality assurance) R.U.:
MRI data collection and lesion analysis H.U.: MRI data collection and lesion analysis C.W.: MRI data collection
and lesion analysis.
Additional Information
Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.