Western Michigan University
ScholarWorks at WMU
Dissertations Graduate College
8-1990
Causes of Juvenile Delinquency in Jordan
Ayed A. Irfaifeh
Western Michigan University
Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations
Part of the Educational Assessment, Evaluation, and Research Commons
Recommended Citation
Irfaifeh, Ayed A., "Causes of Juvenile Delinquency in Jordan" (1990). Dissertations. 2063.
https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations/2063
This Dissertation-Open Access is brought to you for free
and open access by the Graduate College at
ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion
in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of
ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please
contact
[email protected].
CAUSES OF JUVENILE DELINQUENCY IN JORDAN
by
Ayed A. Irfaifeh
A Dissertation
Submitted to the
Faculty of The Graduate College
in partial fulfillment of the
requirements for the
Degree of Doctor of Philosophy
Department of Sociology
Western Michigan University
Kalamazoo, Michigan
August 1990
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CAUSES OF JUVENILE DELINQUENCY IN JORDAN
Ayed A. Irfaifeh, Ph.D.
Western Michigan University, 1990
This dissertation sought to examine the extent to which a set
of social bonding factors were helpful in preventing delinquency in
the Jordanian society. A sample of 147 juveniles (male eleventh-
graders) from seven public high schools in the Tafielah Governate
and its localities completed a self-reported survey. Their answers
were useful in ascertaining whether or not Hirschi's (1969) social
control theory was indeed applicable cross-culturally, especially in
a society dissimilar to the United States. Within the theoretical
confines of Hirschi's theory, the descriptive data suggest that
Jordanian youth have similar cultural beliefs, values, attitudes, and
affiliation to the convention order of the general society.
Based on their admissions of delinquency or non-delinquency, the
respondents were assigned either to the deviant or non-deviant cate-
gory. Four indices were constructed to measure the four elements of
social bonding: (1) attachment to significant others, (2) commitment to
conventional activities, (3) involvement in conventional activities, and
(4) belief in the conventional order.
The four hypotheses of the dissertation were tested and the data
suggested that the Jordanian youth do indeed maintain strong ties to
the conventional order which, in a measure, is some support of Hir-
schi's assertion that a collective conscious exists regarding order
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
maintenance. However, the sample reported significant involvement in
delinquent activities as 76.2% (n = 112) fell into the delinquent
category and 23.8% (n ~ 35) fell into the nondelinquent category.
In sum, the analysis of the data revealed that while Jordanian
youths preserved their ties to the conventional order, the ties were
not sufficient enough to constrain their delinquency involvement.
This finding casts serious doubt on the adequacy of Hirschi's theory
in an attempt to explain juvenile delinquency in a cross-cultural con-
text.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
INFORMATION TO USERS
The most advanced technology has been used to photograph and
reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the
text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and
dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any
type of computer printer.
The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the
copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality
illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins,
and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete
manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if
unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate
the deletion.
Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by
sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and
Cuutinuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each
original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in
reduced form at the ba~k of the book.
Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced
xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white
photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations
appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly
to order.
U·M·I
Untversity Mtcroftlms International
A Bell & Howelllnformatton Company
300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor. Ml48106-1346 USA
3131761-4700 8001521-0600
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Ordel" Number 9101302
Causes of juvenile delinquency in Jordan
Irfaifeh, Ayed Awad, Ph.D.
Western Michigan University, 1990
U·M·I
300 N. Zeeb Rd.
Ann Arbor, MI48106
_" __________________________
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I honestly cannot acknowledge all the persons associated with
Western Michigan University who have contributed so much to my per-
sonal development, but I must be content with recognizing those whose
special efforts and concern were most rewarding to my academic growth.
A debt of gratitude is owed to Dr. Lewis Walker, as Chair of my
Dissertation Committee. His guidance, support, advice, experience,
time, and criticism in the writing of this dissertation was so cru-
cial. As a professor, Lew Walker is outstanding; as a friend, he is
a rare example. My appreciation and gratitude also go to Dr. Peter
Renstrom for his patience and constructive criticism during the writ-
ing of this dissertation. My appreciation for and debt to is owed to
Dr. Judy Riley, who was always willing to offer her advice, experience,
time, and criticism in the writing of this dissertation. Gratitude is
also felt for the support and professional training I received from
the Department of Sociology, W.M U. I am indebted to Ron Kramer,
Morton Wagenfeld, Subhash Sonnad, Paul Friday, Ellen Page-Robin, and
David Chaplin.
A big thank you is owed to my friend and colleague Anora Ackerson
for her support, encouragement, editing, and providing a stimulating
educational environment. My gratitude is individually extended to my
friends Diab Albadyneh, Riad Wriakat, Alison Sears, and the eleventh
grade students in the Tafielah Governate and its localities. I am
most grateful .to Karen Rice for typing my dissertation. Finally, my
ii
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Acknowledgements--Continued
family support during the long years is unparalleled. Thank you all.
Ayed A. Irfaifeh
iii
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS............................................... ii
LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii
CHAPTER
I. INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
The Country. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
Judicial System...................................... 5
Court of Cessation................................ 5
Courts of Appeal. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
Courts of First Instance.......................... 6
Magistrates' Courts............................... 6
Religious Courts. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
Juveniles and Law.................................... 7
Theoretical Formulation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11
Attachment........................................ 14
Commitment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Involvement....................................... 16
Belief. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
Study Rationale and Statement of the Problem......... 17
II. LITERATURE REVIEW.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
The Social Rationality Model......................... 23
Subcultural Theories. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
Cultural Disorganization Theory................... 25
Social Learning Theories.......................... 26
iv
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table of Contents--Continued
CHAPTER
Exchange Theory............. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
The Social Solidarity Model.......................... 32
Containment Theories.............................. 32
Bonding Theories. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34
Research Hypothesis.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
Summary.............................................. 73
III. RESEARCH METHODS..................................... . . . 75
The Setting: The City of Tafie1ah.. ... . . .. . . . . . .. . . . 75
The Sample........................................... 77
Methodology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
Questionnaire......... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
Pretest........................................... 79
The Questionnaire Administration.................. 80
Measurement Problems and Description
of Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
Measurement of Delinquent Behavior................ 84
Data Analysis Procedures.......................... 93
IV. FINDINGS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
Profile of the Sample................................ 94
Comparison to Hirschi's Causes of Delinquency........ 98
Attachment to School, ............................. 103
Attachment to Peers ............................... 110
Commitment to Conventional Activities............. 114
Involvement in Conventional Activities ............ 119
Belief. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table of Contents--Continued
CHAPTER
Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130
Test of the Research Hypotheses ...................... 132
Hypothesis 1. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134
Individual Tests. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
Hypothesis la................ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137
Hypothesis lb..................................... 137
Hypothesis lc. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139
Hypothesis ld. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143
Summary of First Hypothesis .......................... 145
Hypothesis 2. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145
Summary of Second Hypothesis ......................... 148
Hypothesis 3..... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148
Summary of Third Hypothesis.......................... 151
Hypothesis 4. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
Summary of Fourth Hypothesis ......................... 154
Summary of the Chapter............................... 156
V. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION ............................... 159
Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160
Discussion of Findings ............................... 162
Limitations of This Study.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166
Conclusion.............. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
Future Study. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168
APPENDICES
A. Research Instruments ..................................... 170
BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 214
vi
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
LIST OF TABLES
1. The Sample Distribution According to School's Name ............ 94
2. The Respondents' Distribution According to Their Age .......... 95
3. Self-Reported Delinquency by Identification With Father:
"Would you Like to be the Kind of Person Your Father is?" ..... 99
4. Self Reported Delinquency by Perceived Academic Ability:
"How Do You Rate Yourself in School Ability Compared With
Other Students in Your School?" ............................... 103
5. Self-Reported Delinquent Acts by Attitudes Toward School:
"In General, do you Like or Dislike School?" .................. 105
6. Self-Reported Delinquent Acts by Concern for Teacher's
Opinions: "Do You Care What Teachers Think of You?" .......... 108
7. Self-Reported Delinquency by Friends, Contact With the
Police: "Have any of Your Close Friends Ever Been
Picked up by the Police?" ..................................... 110
8. Self-Reported Delinquency by Identification With Best
Friends: "Would you like to be the Kind of Person
Your Best Friends Are?" ....................................... 112
9. Self-Reported Delinqt:.ency by Smoking: "Do You Smoke?" ........ 115
10. Self-Reported Delinquency by Perceived Importance of
Good Grades: "How Important is Getting Good Grades to
You Personally?" .............................................. 118
11. Percent Committing One or More Delinquent Acts by
Time Devoted to Homework ...................................... 120
12. Self-Reported Delinquency by Involvement in School-
Related Activities: "Are You Active in
School Activities?" ........................................... 122
13. Percent Committing One or More Delinquent Acts
By Hirschi's Belief Items ..................................... 124
14. Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and Non-
delinquents on Attachment to Significant Others Index ......... 135
15. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on Attachment Indices. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
vii
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
List of Tables--Continued
16. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on Mother's Attachment Index (Item by Item) ................... 138
17. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on Father's Attachment Index (Item by Item) ................... 140
18. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
011 Attachment to School Index (Item by Item) .................. 142
19. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on Attachment to Peers Index (Item by Item) ................... 144
20. Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and
Nondelinquents With Respect to Commitment to
Conventional Activities ....................................... 146
21. T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
With Respect to Commitment to Conventional Activities
(Item by Item) ................................................ 147
22. Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and
Nondelinquents With Respect to Involvement in
Conventional Activities Index ................................. 149
23. Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and Non-
delinquents With Respect to Involvement in Conventional
Activities (Item by Item) ..................................... 150
24. Results of t-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and
Delinquents With Respect to Belief in the Conventional
Order ......................................................... 152
25. Results of t-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and
Delinquents With Respect to Belief in the Conventional
Order (Item by Item).. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153
viii
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
The subject of this dissertation is juvenile delinquency in
Jordan. Juvenile delinquency as a social phenomenon is a popular
topic in criminological literature. Youth crimes, especially among
males are viewed to be a major social problem both in developed and
developing societies throughout the world. Radzinowicz and King
(1977) have stated that "no natural characteristics, no political
regime, no system of law, police, justice, punishment, treatment
or even terror, has rendered a country exempt from crime. In fact
scarcely any can claim to have checked its accelerating momentum"
(p. 30).
Criminological theories have been developed and tested in
the main by either American criminologis~s or European criminolo-
gists. Western ideas concerning crime and juvenile delinquency
have a considerable international prestige, thus the American
theories of crime as well as textbooks are used all over the world.
Midgley, (1977) and Chambers and Inciardi, (1971) have pointed out
that although Western ideas and concepts have been used and trans-
planted freely, studies designed to replicate established Western
theories of crime causation cross-culturally are relatively rare in
the Third World and do not exist in some countries like Jordan.
In Third World countries, juvenile delinquency studies are
mostly descriptive and include only a few attempts to comparatively
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
2
test basic theoretical propositions about crime. Most of these have
shown the rapid increase in juvenile delinquency to be attributed to
processes of urbanization, migration, decline in family control,
faulty socialization, conflict between youth and adults, the decline
in family roles because of increasing education and literacy, and the
decline in moral standards of the societies (Chambers & Inciardi,
1971).
Clinard and Abbott (1973) pointed out a number of "asymmetries"
found in developing countries which may contribute to the crime pro-
blems. They contend the imbalance between the: (a) highly modernized
urban areas; (b) so-called "primitive" cities; and (c) backward rural
areas is responsible for the increase in the rural-to-urban migra-
tion, which in turn breeds the growth of slums and high urban un-
employment. Consequently, the imbalance between population growth
and the inability of the economy to create jobs lead to despairingly
high unemployment and dependency areas--the conditions which breed
crime.
The Country
The official name of the country is the Hashemite Kingdom of
Jordan which officially came into existence under its present name in
1947 and was enlarged in 1950 to include the West Bank as a result of
the Jericho Conference. Jordan occupies a land mass of 37,738 square
miles. It is bounded on the north by Syria, on the northeast by Iraq,
on the east and south by Saudi Arabia, and on the west by the West
Bank and Israel.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
3
The state of Jordan has received a massive influx of population.
Some 460,000 people were residents of the West Bank when Jordan was
enlarged to become the Hashemite Kingdom of Jordan, but besides these
people, an additional 350,000 to 500,000 refugees from Palestine had
already fled into the West Bank area and TransJordan while the 1948 war
was going on. When the armistice was signed in 1949, these refugees
were never allowed to return to their lands in Israel. Thus, the
population had risen 130%, from 375,000 in 1946 to at least 1.1 million
in 1949 (Beaumont, Gerald, & Wagstaff, 1976).
By 1950, the population of Jordan, including refugees, was ris-
ing steadily at the rate of about 3% annually. Consequently, the
population rose to approximately 2.1 million in 1967, just prior to
the June war. Following the war of June 1967, when Israel took over
Jordan's West Bank region, another 200,000 refugees fled from the area
to the East Bank of the Jordan and so made the economic situation in
the country even worse (Beaumont et al., 1976).
Jordan is one of the small Middle Eastern countries with very
weak natural resources and a population estimated in 1983 at 2,495,000
(East Bank only), mostly concentrated in the industrial cities or the
urban centers. Amman is the capital of the country with a population
of 744,000, the city of Zarka has 255,500 residents, and Irbid 131,200
(Fisher, 1986). Forty-four percent of the population are urban
dwellers; 40% are youth, 52% are below the age of 15; 750,000 are
between the ages of 7-18 years. In addition, the labor force stood at
400,000 in 1977, with 20% in agriculture, 20% in industry, and 60%
in trade and services. The illiteracy rate is 35% (Ministry of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
4
Planning, 1986).
Socioeconomic development in Jordan has been hampered by a
variety of difficulties and challenges. The most serious ones are
Jordan's limited natural resources and the diverse effects of five
centuries of Othman's rule, two wars with Israel, and the cut of
external Arabic funds since 1979. The forced emigration of Pales-
tinians generated serious problems with respect to emergency relief,
the provision of employment opportunities and socioeconomic services.
At the present time, the country is facing an economic crisis; the
foreign debt exceeds 8.1 billion dollars.
Along with these crises, tens of thousands of Jordanians lost
their jobs in the Gulf countries and came home looking for jobs and
houses. Social work began in Jordan at the instigation of religious
organizations and charitable groups which provided financial assist-
ance as well as in-kind aid to the needy among the population. As
Fisher (1986) correctly pointed out, of the three or four hundred
thousand inhabitants, only about one-fifth were town dwellers, and
these were confined to four small cities ranging in population from
10,000 to 30,000 inhabitants, thus assuring that the early years of
Jordanian history would be peaceful. As a result of the two wars,
1948 and 1967, and the emigration of Circassians from the Caucasus of
Russia who settled in Jordan as refugees during the 19th and 20th
centuries, combined with the emigration of Armenians and Palestinians,
urban centers grew rapidly to the present urban population of 60%.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5
Judicial System
Jordanian law is based on Islamic law (80% of the population
are Sunni Muslims) for both civil and criminal matters. In the years
of Othman's rule, certain aspects of continental law were introduced,
especially French commercial law, and civil and criminal procedures.
Due to the British occupation of TransJordan from 1917-1947, the
TransJordanian area has adopted, either by statute or case law, much
of the English common law. Prior to 1962, there were no juvenile
courts or juvenile laws in Jordan. Its judicial system was based on
five types of courts in Jordan: (1) The Court of Cessation, (2) the
Court of Appeals, (3) the Court of First Instance, (4) the Magistrates'
Court, and (5) the Religious Court.
Court of Cessation
The Court of Cessation consists of seven judges who sit in full
panel for exceptionally important cases. In most appeals cases,
however, only five members sit to consider the arguments. It is the
highest court of justice to hear applications in the nature of habeas
corpus, mandamus, and certiorari, i.e., dealing with complaints of
citizens against abuses of governmental authority (Fisher, 1986).
Courts of Appeal
There are two Courts of Appeal, each consisting of three judges,
whether for hearing of appeals or for dealing with Magistrates Courts'
judgments in chambers. Appellate review of the Courts of Appeal ex-
tends to those judgments rendered in the Courts of First Instance, the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6
Magistrates Courts, and Religious Courts (Fisher, 1986).
Courts of First Instance
Courts of First Instance are courts of general jurisdiction in
all matters civil and criminal except those specifically allocated to
the Magistrates Courts. Three judges sit in all felony trials, while
two judges sit for misdemeanor and civil cases (Fisher, 1986).
Magistrates' Courts
There are 14 Magistrates Courts, which exercise jurisdiction in
civil cases involving no more than 250 JD (approximately 300 U.S.
dollars), ar1d in criminal cases involving maximum fines of 100 JD
(approximately 120 U.S. dollars), or maximum imprisonment of one year.
Religious Courts
There are two types of religious courts: The Sharia Courts
(Muslims) and th~ Ecclesiastical Courts (Eastern Orthodox, Greek
Melkite, Roman Catholic, and Protestants). Jurisdiction extends to
personal or family matters, such as marriage, divorce, alimony,
inheritance, etc., for the Muslim community. When a dispute involves
persons of different religious communities, the civil courts have
jurisdiction in the matter unless the parties agree to submit to the
jurisdiction of one or the other of the Religious Courts (Fisher,
1986).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
7
Juveniles and Law
Prior to the 1950s, due to the social forces of the society that
governed and regulated it, the extended family or the clan assumed
full responsibility for its members, especially the young ones. In
Durkheim's (1897/1951) words, in the mechanical Jordanian society, where
the division of labor is minimal, people are generally involved in
similar activities; consequently, people tend to develop similar ideas,
values and goals, as well as a very strong collective conscience.
With respect to juvenile delinquency, before 1962 there were no
juvenile laws, or juvenile courts, or even juvenile institutions except
two homes, 80 capacity each, which were established in Jordan to serve
children between the ages of 12-18. Broadly speaking, children over 12
years were incarcerated in prisons with adult criminals. The children
below the age of 12 were either adopted by relatives or sent to social
or charitable organizations.
Special juvenile laws were first enacted in 1962, later modified
in 1968 and 1987, especially the laws concerning moral offenses such
as rape. The Juvenile Department, according to the Juvenile Law,
Article 24, 1968, (The Ministry of Work and Social Development, 1978)
was established to provide the following services:
1. To prevent juvenile delinquency and crime by
establishing rehabilitative institutions to
house convicted individuals aged 12-18 years,
and provide care, guidance, training, education,
and a trained staff to supervise these
institutions.
2. A juvenile, according to the criminal law is any
individual aged 7-18 years who has committed an
act which violates the Jordanian criminal law.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8
3. To prevent juvenile delinquency phenomenon by
establishing preventive juvenile institutions
for homeless children, orphans, and those at
high risk for delinquency, and provide
sufficient care.
4. To provide some care for juveniles living with
their families (under probation) and provide
counseling and supervision to their families as
well.
5. Special care to be directed to prevent female
delinquency by providing care in the "womens'
house" and follow-up after their release from
the institution. (p. 57)
With regard to delinquency, according to the juvenile laws of
1968, the law does not provide a clear definition of juvenile delin-
quency or a juvenile delinquent. Yet it does establish the age limits
for defining types of juvenile offenders, and sets forth those mea-
sures which may be taken against a juvenile convicted of one offense
punishable by law (Irfaifeh, 1988).
The Jordanian law has several features which will be familiar to
Western criminologists and practitioners. According to Irfaifeh (1988)
they are as follows:
1. All juvenile cases are to be held behind closed
doors.
2. All juvenile misbehavior during the period from
7-18 years of age will not appear in the
individual's record in the future.
3. No juvenile offender will be identified by name,
address, photograph, school, or anything else
which would point to his/her identity.
4. All juveniles have the right to counseling in
the "observation center" prior to the trial.
5. In felony cases, there is no fixed sentence for
each offense; it is up to the discretion of the
judges.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
9
6. Sentence is also up to the discretion of the
probation officer's report. If the juvenile
offender is viewed as a good person, sfhe might
be released after the judges sign the papers.
7. If a juvenile is accused of having committed
more than one offense, s(he will be brought to
trial only for the most serious offense and will
be sentenced only with regard to that offense.
8. Probation instead of incarceration is an
occurrence more in actuality than in philosophy.
(p. 5)
In summation, Jordanian law, Article 24 of 1968, reflects most
of the modern perceptions of protection, treatment, and rehabilita-
tion in dealing with juvenile offenders. In 1987, the Juvenile Law
was modified concerning offenses related to honor and morality, such
as rape, adultery, sodomy, as well as capital offenses such as murder
or homicide. These changes represent a tougher policy in dealing
with these issues. Unfortunately, the law did not go into detail
about the sentencing process.
Since the establishment of juvenile system in Jordan in the
1960s, the authorities adopted a preventative rehabilitative model
in corrections in setting up this institution. Thus, the Social De-
fense Department divides its institutions mostly into two major cate-
gories: (1) the preventative institutions, and (2) the rehabilita-
tive institutions. With respect to the prevention institutions, the
aims are to house orphans and homeless children, as well as indivi-
duals who might drift into delinquency. The rehabilitative institu-
tions have the goals of providing care, supervision, guidance, voca-
tional training, and academic education for those adjudged to be
delinquents.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
10
For juveniles, there are 12 instit.utions operating to serve the
country with a total capacity of 605 residents. Of the 12 institu-
tions, two are used to house the female offenders: one serves females
aged 7-18 years with a capacity of 25 individuals, and the second is
for females aged 15-45, with the same capacity as the first. The
other nine institutions serve male offenders from 7-18 years-of-age.
By the end of 1979, the Ministry of Work and Social Development,
through the Social Defense Department, had established a new insti-
tution with a capacity to serve 130 dangerous offenders and recidi-
vists. This new institution is divided into three sections:
(1) the department of closed detainees, (2) partial detention, and
(3) reception center (Irfaifeh, 1988). Accompanying the five-year
plan for economic and social development (1986-1990), the establish-
ment of this institution reflects the recent get tough policy of the
Social Defense Department concerning recidivists and dangerous
offenders.
The establishment of juvenile laws and juvenile institutions
both predate the establishment of the juvenile courts in Jordan. The
first juvenile court in the world was established in 1899 in Chicago,
Illinois. The first juvenile court in Jordan was established in the
capital of Amman in 1980. Currently, three juvenile courts are
operating in the country. The Ministry of Work and Social Develop-
ment had to ask the Ministry of Justice for judges and, in order to
qualify these new judges, they were sent abroad to learn about
juvenile delinquency. Also, special seminars were conducted, not
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11
only for judges but also for other counselors and employees in the
Ministry.
According to Touq's study (1980), discrepancy betwe~n the
principles of juvenile law and their actual application is very
obvious: delay of due process, absence of cooperation between the
courts and the Social Defense Department, and distrust between judges
(i.e., some of them are unqualified) and probation officers. There
are judges who prefer that juvenile cases be solved either informally
or through the police department. Touq explained this preference by
describing the overcrowding of the court system and the lack of
experience of the judges. Also, he pointed out that police depart-
ments solve approximately 15 juvenile cases monthly. It appears that
only serious cases have the chance of reaching the juvenile courts,
and minor offenses are either solved informally, as in the family
context, or in the police stations.
Theoretical Formulation
Durkheim, an advocate of comparative sociology, emphasized
the importance of testing theoretical propositions in a variety of
social settings, and felt that the following issues should be consi-
dered carefully: First, a theory should be tested in a single cul-
ture at one point in time, such as in the United States; second, in
societies generally similar such as Canada or the European countries
which have comparable cultural, economic, and technological condi-
tions; and third, in completely dissimilar societies (Clinard &
Abbott, 1973).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
12
Following Durkheim's suggestion, many criminologists have
replicated Western theories. Among the better known cross-cultural
studies which have utilized established Western theories of crime
are Wolfgang and Ferracuti's (1967) theory of the subculture of
violence, Downes' (1966) attempt to replicate Cloward and Ohlin's
theory of differential opportunity in the East End of London; Wein-
berg's (1964) use of a differential association theory in Ghana;
Defleur's (1967) analysis of juvenile gangs in Argentina in terms
of subcultural ideas; Friday's (1970) replication of differential
association and differential opportunity in Sweden; and Abbott's
(1971) application of a differential association in Uganda. Abbott
also tested Miller's theory regarding focal concerns, Cloward and
Ohlin's differential opportunity theory, and Midgley's (1977) repli-
cation of differential opportunity in South Africa.
One of the most significant theoretical contributions in the
study of delinquency in recent years has been the development of
social control theory by Hirschi (1969). Krohn and Massey (1980)
and Agnew (1985) agree that "Hirschi's theory has quickly be-
come one of the dominant theories of delinquency. It is the guid-
ing force behind much recent research on delinquency, as well as
occupying a prcininent place in most delinquency texts. Moreover,
the policy implications of the theory are beginning to receive
serious attention" (p. 47).
Hirschi's (1969) theory has been replicated by several scho-
lars (Caplan & Leblanc, 1985; Hepburn, 1977; Hindelang, 1973; Jensen,
1972; Johnson, 1979; Krohn & Masssey, 1980; Matsueda, 1982;
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
13
Wiatrowski, Griswold & Roberts, 1981). The most successful studies
in explaining delinquency and crime are derived from the control theory
of Hirschi, who, according to Shoemaker (1984), is able to explain from
25% to 50% of the variance in delinquency. Wiatrowski et al., (1981),
explained 32.5% of the variance. While that leaves much variation to
be explained, .data suggest that Hirschi's theory provides a better
explanation of delinquency than other leading theories (Cernkovich,
1978; Eve, 1978; Hepburn, 1977; Hindelang, 1973; Johnson, 1979; Knox,
1981).
Frazier (1976) pointed out that Hirschi's work enjoys "a dual
distinction among the contributions to control theories. First, it
it is the clearest statement available on the variations in the con-
trol approach; second, it presents some of the most compelling data
to be found in support of control theory; and third, it is more
congruent with the Durkheimian formulation than any of the more
recent variants of the control thesis" (p. 65). According to Durk-
heim's criteria, control theory has met the first two conditions. It
has been tested in the U.S. by many scholars and it has also been
tested in a different setting in Canada. Thus, the third--a dis-
similar setting--is waiting for initiation.
Prior to the present study, however, Hirschi's theory has not
been tested in the third setting, a dissimilar society. But before
we proceed to the next section, it is necessary to explain the theo-
retical aspects of Hirschi's bonding theory. He proposed an alter-
native explanation of deviant behavior to theories of strain or cul-
tural deviance which attempted to account for too much delinquency
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
14
quency (Briar & Piliavin, 1965; Matza, 1964). In other words, both
strain and cultural theories have failed to explain maturational re-
form, the nondelinquent boy in a high delinquency area, or the many
conforming activities of delinquency adolescents, among others.
Rather than positing extraordinary circumstances which push an ado-
lescent into deviance, Hirschi (1969) maintains that "the delinquent
is a person relatively free of the intimate attachments, the aspira-
tions, and the moral beliefs that bind most people to a life within
the law" (p. i). The four bonding components as specified by Hirschi
are: (1) th~ affective attachment to significant others, (2) a
rational commitment to conventional activities, (3) involvement in
in conventional activities, and (4) belief in the personal legitimacy
of the law.
Attachment
In Hirschi's bonding theory, attachment is defined in terms of
affection, respect and sensitivity to the opinions of others, or it
is the respect between an individual and significant others. "If a
person does not care about the wishes and expectations of other peo-
ple, . . then he is to that extent not bound by the norms. He is
free to deviate" (Hirschi, 1969, p. 18). Ultimately, the more an
individual is affected by, and the more he respects these significant
others, the more he is attached to them. Of significant others,
parents are viewed as the most important in determining either a
positive or negative outlook on conventional values.
Antecedents to Hirschi's notion of attachment are found in
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15
Reckless' (1961) idea of inner containment and Nye's (1958) internal
control. Both are simply explanations of an individual internaliz-
ing the views and beliefs held by significant others. Their concept
of attachment of children to their parents is an essential part of
delinquency research, and is generally supportive of Hirchi's posi-
tion. Empey (1978) also pointed out correctly that strong attachment
leads an individual to an easy internalization of social norms, while
the opposite or weak attachment leads an individual to social aliena-
tion. This point has been supported by Reiss (1951), Morris (1964),
Glueck and Glueck (1950), Toby (1957), and others.
Commitment
The notion of commit~~nt refers to the extent to which an
individual is dedicated to the conventional order. In this instance,
Hirschi pointed out that both aspirations and expectations of achiev-
ing higher education, and a high-status occupation were not related
to delinquency. A commitment to this rationality is demonstrated by
conforming behavior and the utilitarian notion that more is lost by
engaging in delinquency activities than is gained. In sum, commit-
ment reflects the cost factor involved in engaging in antisocial
behavior. Hirschi's notion of commitment is similar to Toby (1957)
and Briar and Piliavin's (1965) reactions that those with a higher
stake in conformity (investment) will be more likely to resist deviant
activities. This contradicts assertion of the cultural and strain
theorists' (Cloward & Ohlin, 1960; Merton, 1937) contention that
deviant behavior is fostered by frustrated ambition and blocked
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16
opportunities (legal or illegal).
Involvement
The assumption here is that an individual who has consider-
able time invested in the pursuance of conventional activities does
not have much time left to commit or engage in deviant activities.
In other words, engagement in conventional activities should absorb
time otherwise available for nonconforming activities. However,
according to Hirschi's findings (1969) "involvement in conventional
activities was not as important as the theory predicts in prevention
of delinquency" (p. 230).
Belief
The last element of Hirschi's bonding theory is belief in con-
ventional values and norms. He argues that belief in the legitimacy
of the moral order will prevent or inhibit delinquency. On the
other hand, the absence of such belief encourages it. If the indi-
vidual believes he should not behave criminally, he probably will
not. As a result, Hirschi (1969) suggested that "delinquency is
not caused by beliefs that require delinquency, but is rather made
possible by the absence of effective beliefs that forbid delin-
quency" (p. 198).
In sum, the four elements of the bond are the focal points of
Hirschi's theory. Further elaboration of his work will be presented
in the literature review chapter. The rationale for using social
control theory in general, and Hirschi's viewpoint in particular,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
17
will be presented in the next section of this chapter.
Study Rationale and Statement of the Problem
Many criminologists have focused attention on delinquency as a
future provider of different types of criminals. In fact, most con-
victed offenders have prior juvenile records, and the life histories
of many criminals show they started a life of crime as delinquents.
The study of juvenile delinquency has always been dependent on offi-
cial data as a source for analysis. The findings of some of these
studies have been misleading because the volume, class, and role
concentration of misconduct in police data are misleading (Arnold,
1983, pp. 42-43) quoted in Al-langawi (1987, p. 58).
In Jordan, the official statistics have shown an increase in
crimes committed by juveniles in the last two decades: first, crimes
against property increased from 677 in 1972 to 1,247 in 1982; second,
aggravated assault crimes increased from 47 in 1972 to 411 in 1982;
third, homicide crimes increased from 13 in 1972 to 27 in 1982; and
fourth, sexual crimes such as rape, increased from 53 to 306 in that
same period (Ministry of Interior Affairs, 1986). According to
Touq (1980), "juvenile crimes increased 64% between the years 1972-
1977 and also they have increased 142% between the years 1965-1977"
(p. 22).
Furthermore, between the years 1975-1986, 39,503 juveniles
either entered or had contact with the criminal justice system. In
the period 1975-1979, 13,201 juveniles committed acts prohibited by
criminal law. Between 1980-1986, another 26,302 juveniles entered
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
18
the delinquency population. Of the 39,503, 95% of the offenders
were males, and 5% females, and the peak age for juvenile offenders
in Jordan was between 12-15 years (Irfaifeh, 1988).
Touq (1980), a Jordanian psychologist, conducted a massive and
comprehensive study of juvenile delinquency in Jordan (using official
records), and concluded that crimes committed by juveniles over the
last three decades have increased dramatically. He also predicts
that juvenile offenses will increase in the future as well. In offi-
cial Jordanian statistics, serious crimes are the most frequently
reported, with minor crimes usually being solved informally even be-
fore being reported to police departments. As a result, most docu-
mented crimes are very serious. Touq (1980) has estimated that the
police departments solve 15 reported cases monthly in the capital,
while others either go undetected or are solved through family net-
works.
Because of the weaknesses associated with official statistics,
many criminologists rely on self-reported delinquency as a sub-
stitute for official records. Arnold (1983) did an offense-by-
offense check to find out the accuracy of a group's self-reported
delinquencies in comparison with their official delinquent records.
He concluded that 75% of the acts were accurately reported (cited
in Al-Langawi, 1987, p. 13). The comparisor. led him to conclude that
it is reasonable to assume that illegal acts are more memorable or
easy to recall than r0utine behavior or legal acts, and that recent
acts are more easily remembered than long past acts. Therefore, the
present writer has chosen in this study to test Hirschi's (1969)
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19
control theory, in a dissimilar setting (Jordan), through the use
of self-reported delinquency involvement.
The present study will extend the replication of Western ideas
to a Third World country (Jordan), and it also will widen the scope
of studying unreported delinquency in Jordan. Thus, two questions
will arise: (1) Why control theory? and (2) Why Hirschi's exten-
sion of the theory? With respect to the first part of the question,
it is simply that control theory seems more consistent with the data
on crime than either strain or cultural deviance theories. These lat-
ter theories account for such delinquency (Briar & Piliavin, 1965;
Hirschi, 1969; Matza, 1969). They have failed to explain matura-
tional reform, the non-delinquent boy in a high-delinquency area, or
the many conventional activities of delinquent youth. Regarding the
second part of the question, which deals with the bonding theories,
Hirschi's extension is the most thoroughly developed and complete and
it has best withstood empirical examinations (see Chapter II). Also,
given the nature of Hirschi's bonding theory, its emphasis on the
family and the conventional order, and its obvious testability, it
is considered an appropriate theory for a better understanding of
delinquency in the Jordanian society.
Durkheim (1897/1951) proposed "that abnormally high or low
levels of social integration (altruism and egoism respectively) and
of social regulation (fatalism and anomie respectively) generate
high suicide rates. A society is integrated to the extent that its
members are morally bonded to each other through interaction, a
commitment to common societal goals, and sharing a collective
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
20
conscience or culture. We are moral beings to the extent that we are
social beings" (p. 64).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER II
LITERATURE REVIEW
In this chapter, a general and brief introductory statement will
be provided about control theories in general. In addition, a highly
selected serie~ cf replicated studies of Hirschi's (1969) theory will
be emphasized, and the hypothesis of the study will be presented.
Control theories have unique and different ways of addressing
the problem of deviant bel1avior. Ultimately, society is regulated to
the extent that social control is exerted over the individuals by cus-
toms, tradition, mores, rules, laws, and other social constraints.
On the other hand, the less integrated and less regulated the society,
the higher the suicide rates. For Durkheim, "ineffective social con-
trol explains deviant behavior" (cited in Howell, 1977, p. 13).
Some criminologists, as we will see, have developed theories or
models of juvenile delinquency and criminal behavior based on Durk-
heim's notion of social control. According to Howell (1977),
three major theoretical perspectives on deviant behavior could be
listed under the social control model: control theories and societal
structural disorganization theories result directly from Durkheim's
model, while cultural disorganization is closely related. The common
denominator of these three perspectives rests on the basic ideas that
deregulation and malintegration are crucial to an explanation of
deviant behavior, but each emphasizes different aspects of ineffect-
ive social control (p. 13).
21
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
22
Social control theories assert that deviant behavior occurs
when an individual's moral bonds to the conventional order are weak,
broken, absent or neutralized (Friday & Hage, 1976; Hirschi, 1969;
Matza, 1964; Nye, 1958; Reckless, 1961; Reiss, 1951; Toby, 1957).
Social structural disorganization theories assert that the frustrated
desire to conform to the conventional order causes non-conformity
(Cloward & Ohlin, 1960; Cohen, 1955; Merton, 1937). With respect to
cultural disorganization theory, deviant behavior is a result of the
desire to conform to cultural values which are in conflict with those
of the dominant order (Miller, 1958; Shaw & McKay, 1942; Sutherland &
Cressey, 1970). In this context, deviant behavior is caused by pro-
per socialization within a deviant social group (for more details,
see Hirchi's, 1969, Chapter I).
The three perspectives, however, have implicitly introduced the
two major concepts of "rationality and solidarity" (Minor, 1975,
p. 45). The sources of conformity or nonconformity can be found in
either one of them. We believe that individuals are rational in
their calculations to conform or not to conform to the conventional
order. In this respect, structural disorganization, learning theory,
and exchange theory, fit the rationality model, with the objective of
either fulfilling desires and needs by rationally choosing illegiti-
mate means, or rejecting the middle class cultural values and conform-
ing to their own subcultures. On the other hand, control theories
fit the solidarity model maintaining that social control is achieved
through socialization and maintained through social constraints as
mentioned earlier. Whenever the social bond is weak or absent,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
23
individuals are more likely to commit crimes but not necessarily.
In other words, when the integration or solidarity is weak, deviant
behavior is highly expected but not necessary. However, control
theory is the purest type of social control theory unlike social
structural disorganization theory.
According to the above discussion, then we can classify the var-
ious theories which deal with social control under two major rubrics:
(1) The social rationality model which subsumes the subcultural
theories, the social disorganizaitonal theory, the social learning
theories, and exchange theories (especially Homans, 1961), and (2)
The social solidarity model which benefits from containment theory,
bonding theory and role relationship theory.
The Social Rationality Model
Subcultural Theories
According to Hirschi (1969) "social structural disorganization
theories of delinquent behavior have been characterized as the re-
sult of 'good answers to a bad question.' It parallels the Hobbesian
question, why do men obey the rules of society? The good answer is
desire, while the Hobbesian answer is fear of the consequences of
violating the laws. On the other hand, the Hobbesian individual is
antisocial and immoral, whereas the concept of man which underlies
structural disorganization theory is homoduplex suggesting that the
individual has a self-interested and a social nature" (p. 4).
Merton (1937) borrowing from Durkheim, suggests that anomie and
deviant behavior result from a disjunction between cultural goals and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
24
socially approved means to their attainment. Limited means make it
difficult to achieve the dominant goals of society, including material
and financial rewards. Ultimately, this will lead to a loss of self-
esteem and an attempt to achieve goals through illegitimate means
(e.g., delinquent behavior). Success frustrated by blocked opportuni-
ties also leads to a sense of injustice, which can serve as a motiva-
tion and rationalization for engaging in deviant activities. (See, for
example the work of Cloward & Ohlin; Cohen, 1955;, 1960; Merton, 1937;
Miller, 1958).
Opportunity is a key concept in this set of theories (disorgani-
zation theories). It is most apparent in the differential opportunity
theory of Cloward and Ohlin (1960) and, to a lesser extent, in the
status deprivation theory of Cohen (1955). Both theories are built
upon Merton's more general theory of crime.
Cohen (1955), quoted in Howell (1977), proposes that:
The motivation to engage in delinquent behavior is generated
out of the experiences of lower class kids in the middle class
dominated institution of the school. School authorities de-
fine success and award status according to middle class stan-
dards which are basic to the curriculum, methods of teaching,
and selection of personnel. This process of selection and
awards creates unequal opportunity in the competition for the
status rewards that the school has to offer in exchange for
adjustment through conformity. The delinquent youth, accord-
ing to Cohen, will reject the middle class criteria and con-
form to the values of a subgroup of delinquent peers. (pp.
16-17)
Cloward and Ohlin (1960) extended and revised the general social
structural disorganization theory and attempted to integrate it with
cultural disorganization theory in their differential opportunity
structure theory of juvenile delinquency. They suggested two impor-
tant additions to social disorganization theory. First is the notion
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
25
that in addition to differential access to legitimate means to sue-
cess goals, there is also a differential access to illegitimate means.
The second aspect makes the claim that different types of delinquent
groups are generated in different types of community cultural con-
texts. According to Howell (1977):
They propose that there are two opportunity structures (legi-
timate and illegitimate) and that youngsters who cannot
achieve economic success in the legitimate opportunity struc-
ture may find that it is not universally achievable nor easily
achieved in the illegitimate opportunity structure. Accordingly,
the delinquent solution to the frustration created by blocked
access to legitimate opportunity is not automatic, thus, the
ability to utilize illegitimate means depends on the organiza-
tion of the community and the type of illegitimate opportunity
structure determines the model type of delinquent behavior
that is available to youngsters in a community. (p. 17).
According to Ohlin and Cloward (1973), the behaviors of the young
are encompassed within a system of social controls that originates in
both legitimate and illegitimate sectors of the community. On one
hand, conventional adults (middle class authorities) control the dis-
tribution of legitimate rewards for conformity to the conventional
order. On the other hand, criminal adults control the distribution
of illegitimate rewards for conformity to the criminal culture in the
community, and individuals use their motivation and rational thinking
to conform to or deviate from the rules.
Cultural Disorganization Theory
This perspective conceptualizes delinquent behavior as an out-
growth of conformity to a distinctive set of cultural beliefs, values,
or definitions. The concept of the individual which underlies cul-
tural disorganization theory is homo sanctus, that is, the individual
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
26
has a social nature which borders on the holy. An individual desires
to conform because of his(her almost hypermoral nature.
Cultural disorganization theory proposes that juvenile delin-
quency is a result of the desire to conform to cultural values which
are in conflict with those of the dominant order (Shaw & McKay, 1942;
Sutherland & Cressey, 1970). Hirschi (1969) suggests that in some
cultural disorganization theories "the criminal ends up just a little
more moral than the law-abiding cit~~en because his actions are based
on consideration of social solidarity rather than personal achievement"
(p. 12). According to this perspective, delinquent behavior is caused
by proper socialization within a "deviant" social group or culture.
Shaw and McKay (1942) propose that culture conflict explains
the distribution of delinquency by area and cultural transmission
explains the persistence of delinquency rates over time. A youngster
growing up in a neighborhood characterized by its conflicting conven-
tional and criminal value systems (disorganized culture) where social
controls are ill-defined will adapt to one of the systems of social
control. In areas with high delinquency rates, the criminal controls
are stronger than conventional controls. Unfortunately, Shaw and Mc-
Kay did not specify the individual learning processes involved in
cultural transition.
Social Learning Theories
According to Akers, (1973) "The basic premise of the social learn-
ing approach is that both conforming and deviant behavior are learned in
the same way; the substance and direction of the learning are different,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
27
but the general process is the same for both conforming and deviant
behavior" (p. 29). Social behavior, according to the social learning
theory, is thus controlled through punishment and reward, hence, the
emphasis on the rationality element more than that of social solidar-
ity.
Sutherland's (1947) theory of differential association was prob-
ably the first leading statement in this area. Hirschi (1969)
labeled or considered his theory, "a cultural deviance theory" in
which both original formulation and empirical examination tend to
emphasize association with delinquent others as the primary causal
factor in delinquency. The subsequent reformulation of Sutherland's
theory by Jeffery (1965), Burgess and Akers (1966), and Adams (1973)
have all emphasized that the process of learning criminal behavior
involves all the mechanisms that are involved in any other learning
(Sutherland & Cressey, 1970, p. 76), (cited in Minor, 1975, p. 46).
Nettler (1974) has suggested that at least three major
principles are involved in human learning: "classical conditioning,
operant conditioning, and modeling" (p. 222). The classical condition
can be traced to the experiments of Pavlov (1927/1960). Eysenck (1964)
and Scott (1971) have suggested that according to this principle that
"conscience is a conditioned reflex which thwarts crimes. As a deter-
rent, therefore, we must consider that the autonomic reaction, the
conditioned conscience of the criminal has it all the way over the
forces of law and order" (Eysenck, pp. 110-111). Eysenck's formulation
has been criticized for its narrow focus on classical conditioning and
for ignoring the importance of operant conditioning, schedules of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
28
reinforcement, and modeling (Hamilton, 1965).
Proponents of operant conditioning, on the other hand, have
linked a response with a subsequent stimulus. Nettler (1974) argues
that "what happens when we act determines how we act" (p. 224). In
fact, operant principles relate to voluntary behavior, compared to
the relatively limited range of reflexive behavior involved in re-
spondent conditioning. Moreover, operant learning theory applies
to a wide range of social behaviors as Akers (1973) and Skinner
(1953) pointed out.
Bandura and Walters, (1963), and Bandura, (1969) have suggested
that an individual may also exhibit learning behaviors through imita-
tion or modeling, or, in other words, by observing the punishment or
reinforcement, which would follow the behavior of another person.
Most recent literature about social learning has attempted to combine
modeling and operant principles. (See Akers, 1973; Bandura, 1969;
Kunkel & Nagaswa, 1973, cited in Minor, 1975, p. 47).
Social learning has gained a slight acceptance among sociolo-
gists. Traub and Little (1985) have cited dozens of criticisms of
Sutherland's (1947) theory of differential association claiming that:
It omits consideration of free will, is based on a psychology
assuming rational deliberation, ignores the role of the
victim, does not explain the origin of crime, does not define
terms such as systematic and excess, does not take biological
factors into account, is of little or no value to practical
men, is not comprehensive enough because it is not interdis-
ciplinary, is not allied closely enough with more general
sociological theory and research, is too comprehensive because
it applies to non-criminals, and assumes that all persons have
equal access to criminal and anti-criminal behavior pattern.
(p. 22)
Burgess and Akers (1966a) have cited two major weaknesses of
---~------
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
29
learning theory in general: reductionism and tautology. Homans
(1961) has responded to these criticisms by attacking structural
explanations of human behavior as not being explanations at all, that
propositions about aggregates have little generality. Moreover,
Homans attacked Durkheim's paradigm of social facts by arguing that
social facts could be related in causal terms only through interven-
ing psychological processes.
It is important to observe the general reluctance of sociolo-
gists to accept the principles of social learning in general and
behaviorism in particular for psychological explanations of human
behavior. However, differential association theory and subsequent
formulations and modifications have enriched our understanding of
human behavior. Another theory, exchanse theory, has generated a
considerable interest among sociologists (Minor, 1975) and will be
discussed in the next section.
Exchange Theory
Exchange theories have a diverse intellectual heritage, drawing
from sources in economics (see Turner, 1974), psychology, (Emerson,
1969), anthropology (see Frazer, 1919; Levi-Straus, 1949/1969;
Malinowski, 1922; and Mauss, 1925/1954), and sociology. The authors
of the best known exchange theory are Homans (1961) and Blau (1964).
The Hobbesian question of order is central to these theories, and
exchange theory is a control theory in the sense that it either empha-
sizes the rationality of the exchange processes (Homans, 1961) or the
resultant social solidarity (Blau, 1964; Minor, 1975).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30
According to the basic assumption of exchange theory, people
seek rewarding statuses, relationships, and experiences and try to
avoid costs, pain, and punishment. Consequently, people choose those
from whom they expect the most profit, rewards, or satisfaction and
avoid those relationships that are not profitable, rewarding, or
satisfying. Homans (1961) acknowledged his intellectual debt to
behavioral psychology and classical economics. However, Homans is
responsible for originating exchange theory, and represents a per-
spective consistent with that of behavioral psychologists who believe
that behavior can be explained in terms of rewards and punishments
(i.e., rationality).
Homans (1961) presented five propositions which he considered
sufficient for the explanation of human behavior:
1. If in the past the occurrence of a particular
stimulus situation has been the occasion on which a
man's activity has been rewarded, then the more
similar the present stimulus-situation is to the
past one, the more likely he is to emit the
activity, or some similar activity now.
2. The more often within a given period of time a man's
activity rewards the activity of another, the more
often the other will emit the activity.
3. The more valuable to a man a unit of the activity
another gives him, the more often he will emit
activity rewarded by the activity of the other.
4. The more often a man has in the recent past received
a rewarding activity from another, the less valuable
any further unit of that activity becomes to him.
5. The more to a man's disadvantage the rule of
distributive justice fails of realization, the more
likely he is to display the emotional behavior we
call anger. (pp. 53-55, 75)
As a result of these five propositions, Homans was able to
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
31
explain a variety of social behavior, including small group inter-
action, and status and conformity to norms. Homan's theory has been
severely criticized by Ekeh (1974), and Turner (1974). The major
four criticisms deal with the following areas: (1) reductionism, (2)
tautology, (3) logical adequacy, and (4) paradigm compatibility. It
is not the purpose of this research to review and defend these criti-
cisms in spite of this theory's weaknesses. The purpose here is to
share Homan's contributions and to acknowledge his theory as the
dominant paradigm of social exchange theories (Minor, 1975), not to
defend his work against the critics.
While Homans is the leading advocate of behavioral psychology,
Peter Blau (1964), another exchange theorist, has acknowledged his
debt to Mead or to symbolic interactionism (the term is coined by
Blumer). Because the exchange is more subjective and interpretative
and that the exchange occurs on the symbolic level, Blau sought to
go beyond small interactional networks to the analysis of larger and
more complex social systems. Turner (1974) has identified this
attempt as follows:
1. The more profit a person expects from another in
e~itting a particular activity, the more likely he
is to emit that activity.
2. The more a person has exchanged rewards with
another, the more likely are reciprocal obligations
to emerge and guide subsequent exchanges among three
persons.
3. The more reciprocal obligations of an exchange
relationship are violated, the more are deprived
parties disposed to sanction negatively those
violating the norm of reciprocity.
4. The more expected rewards have been forthcoming from
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
32
the emission of a particular activity, the less
valuable the activity, and the less likely its
emission.
5. The more exchange relations have been established,
the more likely they are to be governed by norms of
fair exchange.
6. The less norms of fairness are realized in an
exchange, the more are deprived parties disposed to
sanction negatively those violating the norms.
7. The more stabilized and balanced some exchange
relationships among social units, the more likely
other exchange relations are to become imbalanced
and unstable. (pp. 267-268)
Blau has incorporated elements from a variety of sources, such
as functionalism, interactionism, conflict, and other exchange theo-
ries as Turner (1974) points out. Blau's theory came under attack
because of eclecticism, vagueness, and non-systematic presentation
which have apparently hindered both theoretical refinement and empir-
ical research.
For both theorists (Blau and Homans) , what is important is that
each party receives in the exchange something perceived as equivalent
to that which is given: for Homans, it is distributive justice; for
Blau, it is fair exchange.
The Social Solidarity Model
Containment Theories
While the above theories have stressed the importance of the
rational component of social control, containment theories (e.g.,
Nye, 1958; Reckless, 1961; Reckless & Dinitz, 1967; Reiss, 1951)
and bonding theorists (Hirschi, 1969), as well as role relationship
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
33
theory (Friday & Hage, 1976; Toby, 1957) have stressed the solidar-
ity component of social control. Control theorists have drawn upon
the writings of 18th and 19th century social philosophers who viewed
human beings as basically antisocial. Thus, deviance is part of the
natural order in society, and norm violations are basically attractive
and exciting to most people. Consequently, most people are naturally
motivated to deviate, and our task is to explain their conformity.
Reiss (1951) has stated that "delinquency may be defined as the
behavior consequent to the failure of personal and social controls to
produce behavior in conformity with the norms of the social system to
which legal penalties are attached" (p. 196). Thus, Reiss presented
a tautological definition of delinquency, based on the failure of
integrating individuals to the society. In other words, deviance is a
result of society's failure.
The best known containment theory was presented in a classic pa-
per in 1961 by Reckless, and later by Reckless and Dinitz (1967).
Reckless (1961) sought to explain delinquency in terms of both ex-
ternal and internal factors. He described these factors as follows:
Inner containment consists mainly of self-components such as
self-control, good self-concept, ego strength, well-developed
superego, high frustration tolerance, high resistance to
diversions, high sense of responsibility, goal orientation,
ability to find substitute satisfactions, tension-reducing
rationalization, and so on. These are the inner-regulators.
On the other hand "outer containment" represents the struc-
tural buffer in the person's immediate social world which is
able to hold him within bounds. It consists of such items as
a presentation of a consistent moral front to the person,
institutional reinforcement of his norms, goals, and expecta-
tions, the existence of a reasonable set of social expecta-
tions, effective supervision and discipline (social controls),
provision for reasonable scope of activity (including limits
and responsibilities) as well as for alternatives and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34
safety-valves, opportunity for acceptance, identification,
and belongingness. (pp. 44-45)
As a result of this scheme of theorizing, Reckless and Dinitz
(1967) sought to verify their premises by conducting a series of
studies, aiming to demonstrate that self-concept (a concept missing
in Reiss' theory) played a significant role in preventing delin-
quency. Tangri and Schwartz (1967) have cited three major deficien-
cies in Reckless' theory: (1) methodological, (2) measurement problem
of self-concept, and (3) inadequate theoretical linkage between self-
concept and delinquency. In spite of these deficiencies, Reckless'
containment theory has provided an alternative interpretation of
deviance to the deterministic image, or class-biased interpretation
of Cohen, (1955), Miller, (1958), and Cloward and Ohlin (1960).
Bonding Theories
According to this group of theories, conformity is the conse-
quence of the social bond, and crime occurs only when the bonds to
society are weak or broken (Hirschi, 1969; Nye, 1958; Toby, 1957).
Crime or delinquency also occurs with loss of role relationships
(Friday & Hage, 1976; Hage & Marwell, 1968; Marwell & Hage, 1970);
neutralized behavior (Sykes & Matza, 1957); episodically released
bonds (Matza, 1964); or being overcome by situational inducements
(Briar & Piliavin, 1965). The description of the relationship between
an individual and society has been defined using many terms (e.g., bond,
role relationship, attachment, neutralization, etc.). In spite of these
different terms, the above mentioned theorists have sought to explain
the linkage interaction between individual and society.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35
Nye (1958) has suggested that parent-adolescent relationships
are especially significant for social control and the prevention of
delinquency. He proposed a multi-causal theory which focuses upon
inadequate social control in accounting for delinquency among youth.
He stated that "when internal and external controls are weak and
alternate routes to goal achievement are restricted, delinquent beha-
vior can be anticipated" (p. 4). Toby (1957) has described teenagers
as differing in terms of their "stage in conformity" or, in other
words, what a person risks losing by being detected in deviant beha-
vior. Toby suggested that all of us are tempted by what we resist
to the extent that we feel we have much to lose by deviant behavior.
Hage and Marwell (1968), Marwell and Hage (1970), and Friday and
Hage (1976) have pointed out the importance of understanding the
patterns of role relationships of adolescents and how these role
relationships affect a youth's integration into society. Instead of
the term "bond," they used the term "role" which seems more dynamic.
However, Friday and Hage (1976) argued that their "approach is quite
different from that of Hirschi (1969), who was also inspired by Durk-
heim but put most of the emphasis on attitudes, such as attachment,
rather than behavior, as represented by role relationship variables,
although a role relationship perspective is implied in Hirschi's study
of juvenile delinquency" (p. 349).
Friday and Hage (1976) have cited five major patterns of role
relationships. These are:
(1) kin relationships--including the extended family, (2)
community or neighbors, (3) school, (4) work, and (5) peers
not otherwise defined by the four other categories.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
36
Subsequently, if an individual has intimate role relationships
in all five roles, they are much less likely to engage in
youth crime, or as the intimacy declines (parallel to bond)
both within certain areas and across all of them, the youth is
less integrated into society and more likely to be involved in
various kinds of crime. (p. 350)
According to Sykes and Matza (1957), much delinquency is based
on what is essentially an unrecognized extension of defenses to
crimes, in the form of justification or rationalization for deviance
that is seen as valid by the delinquent but not by the legal system.
For Sykes and Matza, delinquency is the individual's ability to
rationalize or justify the moral order expressed by the law. These
justifications either precede or follow deviant behavior to protect
the individual from self-blame and the blame of others after the act.
Sykes and Matza have labeled these justifications "techniques of
neutralization" which they divide into five major types: (1) the
denial of responsibility, (2) the denial of injury, (3) the denial of
the victim, (4) the condemnation of the condemners, and (5) the appeal
to higher loyalties (Sykes & Matza, 1957, pp. 664-670). See litera-
ture review on Hirschi (1969) later in this chapter.
Matza (1964) severely attacked the positivistic paradigm in
understanding delinquency. For him, the delinquent is an individual,
much like his peers, who swings or drifts between conformity and
deviance. Thus, he rejected the deterministic explanation of deviant
behavior by stating that:
The periodic breaking of the moral order bind to law ar~s~ng
from neutralization and resulting in drift does not assure
the commission of a delinquent act. Drift makes delinquency
possible or permissible by temporarily removing the restraints
that ordinarily control members of society, but of itself
supplies no irreversible commitment or compulsion that would
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
37
suffice to thrust the person into the act--I wish to suggest
that the missing element which provides the thrust or impetus
by which the delinquent act is realized is will. (p. 181)
Of the preceding versions of control theory, Hirschi's (1969)
is the most thoroughly developed. Hirschi's theory and its empirical
testing will be discussed next in more detail.
Drawing upon the writings of Durkheim, Nye, Toby, and others,
Hirschi specified four elements of the bond to society: (l) the
effective attachment to conventional others, (2) a rational commit-
ment to conventional activities, (3) involvement in conventional
activities, and (4) belief in the personal legitimacy of the law.
In Hirschi's theory, attachment is defined as sensitivity to the
opinions of others. Hirschi (1969) stated, "if a person does not
care about the wishes and expectations of other people--then he
is to that extent not bound by the norms. He is free to deviate"
(p. 18). Respondents in Hirschi's study were asked many questions
to explore the relationship between attachment to parents and
delinquency involvement: (a) time spent with parents; (b) supervi-
sian by parents; (c) intimate communication between parents and child;
(d) affectional identification by the juvenile with his/her parents;
and (e) emotional support by the parents (Hirschi, 1969, pp. 88-93).
However, he discounted time as relatively unimportant and was slightly
displeased with the emotional support items in his study as being too
vague. Supervision was important as well as affectional identification
by the juvenile with his/her parents. The following question was asked
to examine identification with parents and delinquent behavior:
"Would you like to be the kind of person your father is?" He found
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
38
64% of those who answered in "every way" compared to 41% who answer-
ed "not at all" to the question, scored low on the delinquency index.
Hirschi (1969) also found that "among his males the relationship be-
tween affection for mother and self-reported delinquency is stronger
than the relationship between affection for father and self-reported
delinquency" (p. 92).
Hirschi (1969) argues that attachment to the school is an arti-
fact of successful bonding to the conventional order; and he postu-
lates that attachment to school is similarly related to delinquency.
Here he argues that "the boy who sees himself as capable of doing
well in school will find school tolerable regardless of his ability
as measured in a more objective fashion" (p. 117). In order to test
this hypothesis, the subjects were asked to rate their academic abil-
ity in relation to other students in the school. He found that 13%
of those who rated themselves "among the best" and 35% of those who
rated themselves "below average" scored high on the delinquency
index.
In order to assess the bond to school, Hirschi asked the re-
spondents to respond to the following questions: "In general, do you
like or dislike school?" (possible responses were "like," "neither
like nor dislike," and "dislike"). According to his findings, 68% of
those who reported "liking school," reported no delinquent behavior,
compared to 33% of those who reported "disliking school."
In connection with the school, and in order to test the bond
(attachment) to conventional figures, Hirschi asked the respondents
to answer the following question: "Do you care what teachers think
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
39
of you?" (possible responses were "a lot," "some," and "not much").
According to his findings, 66% who reported "caring a lot" reported
low delinquent involvement, and 35% who reported "caring not much"
reported low delinquent involvement. Therefore, the findings which
focused on attachment or the bond with the teacher seem significant
in deterring delinquency.
Hirschi (1969) stated that "both differential association and
control theory predict that the delinquent behavior of one's own
friends is strongly related to one's own delinquency behavior"
(p. 98). This hypothesis confirmed or supported his expectations
because 73% of his subjects who reported having "no close friends
picked up by the police" scored low on reported delinquency behavior,
and 25% who reported having "four or more friends picked up by the
police" scored low on reported delinquency behavior.
The fact that the significant others of any individual are par-
ents or friends is relevant. If an individual is strongly attached
to his/her peers, that individual is also more likely to be strongly
attached to his/her parents. Hirschi's results have supported his
position; as attachment to mother increases, so does attachment to
peers. Hirschi postulated that the greater the peer attachment, the
lower the reported delinquency involvement. Sixty-four percent of
those who reported identifying "in most ways" with their friends
reported low delinquent involvement, and 47% of those who reported
identifying least closely and "not at all" with their friends re-
ported low delinquent involvement.
According to Hirschi's theory, both aspirations and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
40
expectations of achieving higher education or a high-status occupa-
tion wererelated to nondelinquency. These hypotheses were inter-
preted as supporting the contentions of Toby (1957) and Briar and
Piliavin (1965) that those with a higher stake in conformity will
be more likely to resist delinquent temptations, which contradict
the strain theorists (Cloward & Ohlin, 1960; Cohen, 1955; Merton,
1937).
Hirschi argued that involvement in adult activities including
smoking, drinking and dating are related to delinquent involvement.
Among those who reported having begun smoking before age 13, 25%
scored low on reported delinquent involvement; 64% who reported not
smoking scored low on reported delinquent involvement. Also, Hirschi
argued that drinking is more strongly related to delinquency than
smoking and that dating is also strongly related to delinquency.
On the other hand, Hirschi speculated that involvement in adult
activities is indicative of commitment to the conventional student
status. Hirschi's argument (1969) is that "evidence of involvement
in school is traditionally measured by time spent in and concern for
homework" (p. 191). In general, there is a monotonic increase in
the proportion of those scoring medium or high on reported delinquent
involvement as the number of hours per day spent on homework decrea-
ses from one and one-half hours (34%) to one hour (48%), one-half
hour (52%), and to less than one-half hour (64%). The second measure
to test the commitment to academic activities is the importance that
the respondent personally attaches to achieving good grades. Among
those who reported viewing grades as being very important to
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41
themselves personally, 64% reported low delinquent involvement, and
among those who reported viewing grades as unimportant, 21% reported
low delinquent involvement. Thus, those who have a stake in school
performance are less likely to engage in delinquent activities.
Engrossment in conventional activities should absorb time avail-
able for delinquent activities. However, as mentioned above, of sev-
eral measures of involvement, only time spent on homework had a con-
sistent and reasonably strong inverse relationship with delinquency
(see commitment to school). Other measures of involvement (working
for pay, dating, feeling that "there is nothing to do," and time
spent watching television, reading comic books, talking with friends,
and riding around in cars) were related to delinquency only weakly,
spuriously, or in the wrong direction. Hirschi concluded that "we
must consider, then, what the child is doing, and assiduously avoid
the idea that doing 'something'--anything--is better than, that is,
inhibitive of, the commission of delinquent acts" (Hirschi, 1969,
pp. 190-191). The problem with such measures of involvement is that
they also may be conceptualized as measures of attachment or commit-
ment.
The final element of the social bonding theory is belief. Hir-
schi argued that belief in the legitimacy of the moral order inhibits
delinquency, while the absence of such belief encourages it. "Belief
in the moral validity of the law is consistently related to the mea-
sures of attachment and commitment discussed earlier" (Hirschi, 1969,
p. 203).
Except for the appeal to higher loyalties (which was not tested)
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
42
and the denial of the victim, Hirschi's findings supported his
theory. He found that each of the techniques of neutralization
(Sykes & Matza, 1957) was related to delinquency. He asked his
subjects to respond to the statement, "It is all right to get around
the law if you can get away with it." Controlling for various mea-
sures of attachment and commitment suggested that the relationship
between the various techniques of neutralization and deviant behavior
were genuine. Seventy percent of those who strongly agreed with that
statement scored medium or high on reported delinquent behavior.
Respect for the police was another measure of belief. Twenty-
nine percent of those who strongly agreed that they have a lot of
respect for the police, and 66% of those who strongly disagreed
scored medium or high on reported delinquent behavior. Another
technique of neutralization is denial of responsibility. An item
designed to measure this is the statement, "Most criminals should
not be blamed for the things they have done." A second item to
measure denial of responsibility is "1 cannot seem to stay out of
trouble no matter how hard :;: try." Fifty-one percent of those who
reported strong agreement with the first item scored medium on re-
ported delinquent involvement, and 39% of those who reported strong
disagreement with the second item of denial of responsibility report-
ed medium or high delinquent involvement. With respect to denial of
injury, Hirschi (1969, p. 208) asked for responses to the statement,
"Most things people call delinquenc~· don't really hurt anyone." The
results of this statement are as follows: Those most strongly agree-
ing with the statement reported most delinquent involvement (72%);
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
43
and those who strongly disagreed with the statement reported least
delinquent involvement (31%).
Denial of the victim was measured by the following item: "The
man who leaves his keys in the car is as much to blame for its theft
as the man who steals it." Results for this item (mentioned earlier)
did not support Hirschi's theory: the percentages reported were 44%
and 37% respectively. And the final item of technique of neutraliza-
tion reported by Hirschi is condemning the condemners. Hirschi test-
ed this by stating "policemen try to give all kids an even break."
The results showed a moderate relationship between agreement with
the item and reported delinquent behavior: 35% and 58% respectively.
In a quasi-replicated study of Hirschi's bonding theory, Hinde-
lang (1973) conducted research on groups of male and female students
in grades six through twelve of one school in a rural area in upstate
New York. The county in which the school studied is located has no
town with a population greater than 4,400 and has a population dens-
ity of 39 inhabitants per square mile. This is contrasted with Con-
tra Costa, California which is a city with a population of 8,500 and a
density of 756 inhabitants per square mile (Hirschi's study). Hinde-
lang had 978 students respond to a self-report questionnaire. Nine
hundred and forty one produced a usable questionnaire.
According to Hindelang's findings, attachment to parents and
reported delinquent involvement were related. Sixty-one percent of
the rural males who wanted to be like their parents "in every way"
and 36% of those who answered "not at all" scored low on the delin-
quency index. With respect to affection for mother and self-reported
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
44
delinquency, Hindelang's findings indicated that affection for
mother is a stronger deterrent than affection for father in predict-
ing delinquent behavior. Seventy-nine percent of those who answered
that they wish to be like their mothers "in every way" but only 36%
of those who answered "not at all" scored low in self-reported delin-
quency. Moreover, Hindelang did not find any differences with re-
spect to parental attachment--whether to mother or father. Thus,
among Hindelang's rural students, attachment to parents was related
to reported delinquent behavior as Hirschi expected.
As mentioned earlier, Hirschi assessed the bond to the school
with the question, "In general, do you like school or dislike school?"
Hindelang's subjects' responses were: 63% of the rural males who
reported liking school and 45% of the rural males who reported dis-
liking school reported low delinquent involvement. At the same time,
67% of the rural females who reported liking school and 41% of the
rural females who reported disliking school reported low delinquent
involvement. Also, Hindelang assessed the bond between teachers and
students as part of the school setting by asking the question, "Do
you care what teachers think of you?" Responses could vary from "a
lot" to "not much." Among Hindelang's subjects, 70% of the rural
males and 67% of the rural females who reported "caring a lot",
reported low on delinquent involvement. Thus, Hindelang concluded
that the bond with the teacher is closely related to self-reported
delinquent involvement.
Hirschi predicts that the delinquent behavior of one's own
friends is strongly related to one's own delinquent behavior.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
45
Hirschi and Hindelang's findings supported this position. Eighty
percent of the rural males who reported having no close friends
picked up by the police scored low on self-reported delinquent
involvement, while 26% of the rural males who reported having four
or more friends picked up by the police scored low on delinquent
involvement. Figures for rural females were weaker: 75% and 31%
respectively. Moreover, Hirschi suggested that the greater the peer
attachment, the lower the reported delinquent involvement.
The results of Hindelang's study did not support Hirschi's
position. Of those who reported identifying most closely with their
friends, 49% reported low delinquent involvement while 67% of those
identifying least closely with their friends reported low delinquent
involvement. Hirschi's figures were 64% and 47% respectively.
Hindelang (1973) speculated "that among rural subjects che charact-
eristics of those to whom one is attached may be necessary before a
consistent relationship will emerge" (p. 479).
Hirschi stated that aspirations to achieve conventional goals
constrain delinquency, unlike the assumptions of strain theory.
Thus, Hirschi assessed involvement in adult activities by adolescents,
predicting that activities such as smoking, dating, and drinking could
lead to illegal behavior. With respect to Hindelang's findings, 28%
of the rural males and 30% of the rural females who reported having
begun smoking before age 13 scored low on reported delinquent in-
volvement; and for those who reported not smoking, 68% of the rural
males and 71% of the rural females scored low on reported delinquent
involvement.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
46
Combining the three adult activities for rural males, only 16%
of those who did not report any of these activities scored medium or
high on reported delinquent involvement while 73% of those who smok-
ed, drank, and dated reported medium or high delinquent involvement.
Thus, Hindelang questioned this aspect of Hirschi's theory.
Furthermore, Hirschi examined the commitment to academic activi-
ties and delinquent involvement by testing the importance that the
subject attaches to good grades. Both Hirschi and Hindelang's find-
ings confirmed this position. Sixty-six percent of Hindelang's rural
males who reported viewing grades as being very important and 65% of
rural females who reported the same also reported low delinquent in-
volvement. For those who reported grades as completely unimportant,
21% of the rural males, and 39% of the rural females reported low de-
linquent involvement. Thus, those who have a stake in school perfor-
mance for which delinquent behavior may endanger that status are less
inclined to engage in delinquent activities, as Hirschi predicted.
Hirschi measured involvement in school activities by time spent
on homework, as well as participation in nonacademic school activi-
ties which he predicted are inversely related to delinquent involve-
ment. Hindelang pointed out that there is a monotonic increase in
the proportion of those who scored medium or high on reported delin-
quent involvement as the number of hours per day spent on homework
decreased from one and one-half hours to one hour, to one-half hour,
and to less than one-half hour. Hindelang's (1973) findings are 39%,
40%, 55%, 53% for rural males; and 35%, 45%, 45%, 67% for rural
females--very similar to Hirschi's findings (34%, 48%, 52%, 64%).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
47
In another study, Linden and Hackler (1973) have developed a
delinquency model based on differential association theory, control
theory and affective ties theory that predict delinquent involvement.
They obtained data on 200 boys, ranging in age from 13-15 years, who
resided in four low-cost housing projects in Seattle, Washington.
They divided them into experimental and control groups for the pro-
gram, but they used both groups.
They measured attachment or ties to parents with 23 questions
dealing with closeness within the family. An average of these 23
responses was used as an indicator of the closeness of the boy and
his parents. On the other hand, ties to peers was measured by ask-
ing respondents how they felt about deviant and conventional peers,
respectively. As Hirschi predicted on the basis of control theory,
48.5% of those boys having weak or nonexistent ties with both con-
ventional peers and parents were delinquent uhile only 16.8% of
those who reported moderate or strong ties with both parents and
peers were delinquent.
Moreover, according to their findings, 42.9% of those who re-
ported weak or nonexistent ties to any of the three groups (delin-
quent peers, nondelinquent peers, and parents) reported themselves
as delinquent. And of those boys with weak ties to conventional
associates but with moderate or strong ties to deviant peers, 58.3%
were delinquent as self-reported.
Linden and Fillmore (1981) undertook a parallel study to
Hirschi's based on a sample of all tenth-grade students (n = 1152)
attending the two senior high schools serving the highest
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48
socioeconomic area of the city, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, (n =
1152). Their primary aim was to demonstrate that studies of delin-
quency done in one modern society could be generalized to other
modern societies and their secondary goal was to more adequately
combine elements of control and differential association theories of
delinquency.
According to the two Canadian scholars, differential associa-
tion has failed to explain why some teenagers have delinquent friends
and others do not. Also, differential association suffered from re-
search evidence that teenagers often begin their delinquent activi-
ties before they begin substantial association with other delin-
quents. On the other hand, Hirschi found that having delinquent
friends had no impact on some teenagers (considering other bonds or
roles in Friday and Hage's theory). Linden and Fillmore sought to
repair these shortcomings of differential association by combining
it with control theory. According to their model, teenagers with
low stakes in conformity have little to lose by association with
other delinquents, and that such association will further amplify
their delinquency as they learn new criminal techniques and are
reinforced for new acts of deviance.
Linden and Fillmore compared the delinquency of Richmond, On-
tario, Canada and Edmonton, Alberta, Canada teenagers based on self-
reported data. They found more serious offenses reported by Rich-
mond's sample than in Edmonton (e.g., auto thefts 10.8 to 5.9 in
Edmonton). But generally, the data are very similar.
Stakes in conformity are measured on the basis of attachment
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
49
to parents and liking school. They found that in both Edmonton and
Richmond these items were negatively correlated with being delinquent
and with having delinquent friends, and in both nations, those with
delinquent friends were more apt to be delinquents. Finally, their
reformulated model fit those data well. In both Canada and the
United States low stakes in conformity seemed to lead to the forma-
tion of attachments to delinquent peers. The correlation for Rich-
mond males was .30 and females .24, but for the Canadian teenagers
the correlations were .11 and .20 respectively.
Jensen (1972) reanalyzed Hirschi's (1969) data, focusing on the
relationship among parents, peers, and delinquency, but used more
extensive measures of availability of delinquent patterns. He tried
to go beyond Hirschi to a more explicit test of differential asso-
ciation theory. Two major hypotheses were investigated: (1) does
parental control affect delinquency directly as Hirschi predicted or
indirectly by influencing the probability of learning delinquent
definitions as differential association suggested? and (2) do delin-
quent peers foster delinquency directly as suggested by the group
process and situationally induced motive theories (Briar & Piliavin,
1965; Short & Strodtbeck, 1965), or indirectly by exposing a boy to
delinquent behavior patterns as differential association theorists
have argued.
In order to measure the availability of delinquent patterns,
Jensen (1972) used official delinquency rates in schools, perceptions
of trouble in the neighborhood and number of delinquent friends. Fur-
thermore, he used four items to represent Sutherland's (1947)
------------------------------------------------
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
so
definitions favorable to violation of the law.
According to his findings, with respect to the first hypothesis,
when the differential association variables are held constant, the
parental control variables (father's supervision and father's support)
still depressed delinquency. Jensen (1972) concluded that "control
theory is supported over differential association" (p. 574). With
reference to the second hypothesis, he found that "the number of
delinquent friends affected delinquency independently of the effect
of delinquent definitions. Again, he concluded that group process
and situational motives theories are empirically superior to differ-
ential association theory" (p. 574).
However, Jensen's findings should be approached carefully with
the realization that he didn't intend to test both theories expli-
citly. Moreover, Matsueda (1982) pointed out that Jensen's study
left several questions unanswered:
First, does measurement error in the indicators of the ratio
of delinquent and antidelinquent definitions seriously at-
tenuate their effects on delinquency? Second, given that
Sutherland (1947) invented the differential association
principle in part to account for certain variations in crime
rates--variations according to age, social class, broken
homes, and neighborhoods--can it in fact do so? And third,
is the substantive picture distorted because Jensen's three
tables fail to capture more complex relationships among
relevant variables? (p. 491).
Caplan and Leblanc (1985) replicated Hirschi's study on a sample
of 1,472 male adolescents attending public high schools on the Island
of Montreal, Canada. According to their findings with respect to
attachment to parents, a negative relationship between affectional
identification with father and delinquency (gamma: -.24), and affec-
tional identification with mother and delinquency (gamma: -.12), was
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
51
very similar. They also found that adolescents with positive atti-
tudes toward school demonstrated the greatest variation in their
delinquent involvement, suggesting that a positive attitude toward
school had the greatest impact on delinquent involvement (gamma =
.26, in Hirschi's study, it was -.41) (p. 126).
With respect to attachment to teachers in the Canadian study,
whether an individual expressed concern about teachers' opinions of
him/her or did not, those opinions had little effect on hisfher
involvement in delinquent acts. Twenty-one percent of those who
cared "a lot" reported involvement in two or more acts, while for
those subjects who reported "not much" concern, 28% admitted two or
more delinquent acts. In sum, the Canadian findings with respect to
the level of concern for teachers' opinions did not affect the adol-
escent's involvement in delinquent acts (gamma= -.04).
Caplan and Leblanc examined the distribution of delinquent
friends in their study, and found certain differences across samples.
In the American studies (Hindelang, 1973; Hirschi, 1969), 48.1% of
Hirschi's sample had delinquent friends, and 63% of Hindelang's sam-
ple also had delinquent friends. This was in contrast with the Cana-
dian study in which only 27.1% had delinquent friends. Moreover,
less than 25% of all three samples listed one or two delinquent
friends.
With respect to involvement in conventional activities, Caplan
and Leblanc found a negative relationship between time spent doing
homework and involvement in delinquent activities. They found 62%
of the adolescents who reported spending less than one half hour a
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
52
day on their homework admitted one or more delinquent acts, compared
to 64% of Hirschi's sample who reported the same level of delinquent
involvement. The second aspect of involvement is participation in
adult activities in which they found that 42% of those adolescents
not involved in smoking, drinking and dating were not involved in
delinquent behavior, while 55% of those who dated and drank com-
mitted one or more delinquent acts.
Finally, the belief component explains the highest proportion
of the variance with the variable of moral duty to obey the law con-
sistently being the most powerful belief item with all four independ-
ent variables. In sum, by examining the overall explanatory power
of the theory for four items in the deviant behavior scale: (1) fre-
quency of alcohol and marijuana use, (2) frequency of use of stronger
drugs, (3) minor delinquent behavior (e.g., vandalism), and (4) ser-
ious delinquent behaviors, moderate support for the theory was
reported.
Conger (1976) utilized data collected from two surveys. The
first was done in Seattle, Washington at the Seattle Atlantic Street
Center in 1964. A total of 374 seventh graders in two schools were
surveyed; 221 blacks, 104 whites, 38 orientals, and 11 of mixed
racial background. The second survey was done in 1965 and the sample
was comprised of 1,588 white males out of the original sample of 5,545
(the same data used by Hirschi, 1969).
Conger's (1976) study attempted to go beyond the social control
perspective. Specifically, he tried to show how social interaction
processes can affect delinquent behavior. The joint influences of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53
parents and peers and how they relate to delinquent behavior was
explored. However, as Conger pointed out, his study didn't intend
to test the two models. As a result, his findings should be
approached very carefully.
According to Conger's (1976) findings, attachment and delin-
quency supported the social learning theory's assertion that no real
prediction can be made unless the type of peer is taken into account.
In other words, attachment to conventional peers tends to decrease
delinquency involvement and attachment to delinquent peers increases
delinquency involvement. The same principle is applicable for
attachment to parents.
Conger (1976) found that "the actual parental behavior, not the
simple existence of juvenile communication or identification with
parents, is the most important part of the bond between parent and
child which protects against delinquency involvement" (p. 36). How-
ever, Conger didn't use the five elements of the bond between parent
and child to test attachment to parents and delinquency involvement;
only communication and identification were used. However, Hirschi
(1969) addressed the shortcomings of his study by stating that "con-
trol theory is supported with two exceptions. First, involvement in
conventional activities was not as important as the theory predicts
in delinquency prevention. Second, the influence of delinquent peers
has an importance in the commission of delinquent acts not predict-
able from the current formulation of control theory" (pp. 230-231).
The importance of Conger's study stems from addressing peer attach-
ment and delinquency involvement.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
54
Hepburn (1977) combined the causal structure of social control
theory (Hirschi, 1969) and Sutherland's (1947) theory of differential
association by using correlational data of 139 males, aged 14-17 in
a medium-sized industrial city. He used four variables to test the
two models: (1) lack of family support, (2) delinquent definitions,
(3) delinquent associates, and (4) delinquent behavior.
According to Hepburn's (1977) findings, Sutherland's (1947) cau-
sal structure depends upon delinquent associates to mediate between
poor family support and delinquent definitions. Empirical support
was not found for such a position. On the other hand, the findings
supported Hirschi's (1969) view that delinquent definitions, whether
using behavioral constraint or willingness to engage in delinquency,
precede having delinquent associates in terms of importance. More-
over, Hepburn praised the affective model presented by Linden and
Hackler (1973) mentioned above, which provides greater clarity for
reformulating Hirschi's causal model of delinquency.
In an attempt to explain the differential rates of delinquency
between rural and urban areas, Lyerly and Skipper (1981) have used
the social control theory of Hirschi, not only to investigate the
differences, but also to measure the extent of delinquency involve-
ment and degree of commitment to five institutional orders:
(1) family, (2) church, (3) school, (4) peers, and (5) formal author-
ity. The combination of these institutional orders and of the indi-
vidual's perception of his/her relationship to them comprised the
social bond which insulates the youth from delinquency.
Lyerly and Skipper's data were drawn from anonymous self-reported
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
55
reported questionnaires administered by the principle researchers to
urban and rural teenagers who were being officially detained (insti-
tutionalized) and held in two separate juvenile detention centers in
Southwestern Virginia. The first center serves the rural counties of
Giles, Pulaski, and Montgomery. The second facility is located in
Roanoke and serves both the city and the county. One hundred white
male juveniles participated in the study; blacks and females were
excluded because of their small numbers.
According to their findings, among the sample of rural and urban
offenders, a strong inverse relationship was found between the compo-
site commitment scores and devianc behavior. Only commitment to fam-
ily did not prove to be statistically significant. Lyerly and Skip-
per explained these findings by indicating that only 33% of the total
sample lived with parents. Commitment to school and legal authority
was most strongly associated with delinquent behavior, and rural of-
fenders were more strongly committed to conformity than urban offend-
ers. No difference was reported between rural and urban youngsters
with respect to legal authority, but rural boys reported less serious
and less frequent involvement in delinquent activity than did urban
boys. Justification for these findings included the idea "that the
urban sample may b~ more typical of the majority of urban youngsters
who live in the metropolitan sections of the nation" (Lyerly & Skip-
per, 1981, p. 398). In sum, the two researchers concluded that both
theory and data suggest that control theory remains a promising one
to explain delinquent behavior in different settings.
Krohn and Massey (1980) replicated Hirschi's (1969) study on a
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
56
sample of 3,065 male and female adolescents in grades 7 through 12
in six communities in three midwestern states to examine the overall
and relative effects of social bonding theory on four separate mea-
sures of deviance.
Attachment was measured by three scales: (1) maternal attach-
ment, (2) paternal attachment, and (3) peer attachment. The ques-
tions comprising these scales were similar to those used by Hirschi
and represent the components of supervision, praise, discouragement,
closeness, and satisfaction. The findings concerning attachment to
friends, and attachment to mother and father are small. The commit-
ment element was measured by the following activities: (a) school
work, (b) athletics, (c) musical groups, (d) pep groups, (e) other
school activities, and (f) community clubs.
Responses ranged from "very important" to "not important at all"
and when combined generated a range of scores from high commitment
(28) to low commitment (7). Furthermore, the G.P.A. (Grade Point
Average) as an indicator of commitment was included. Other measures
of commitment, such as educational aspirations and the importance of
a good career were similar to Hirschi's. However, Krohn and Massey
(1980) included the element of involvement within the element of com-
mitment, arguing that both involvement and Hirschi's notion of at-
tachment to school should be one concept.
With respect to their findings concerning commitment, with the
exception of the minor substance use scale, the commitment component
explained a higher proportion of the variance than did attachment and
belief. And among the most powerful indicators were the commitment
------------------------------------------------------
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
57
index and G.P.A.
The final element of the social bond is belief and it was mea-
sured by three items related to the degree of agreement or dis-
agreement respondents have with parental norms, legal norms, and
belief in the value of education. From the findings, for minor
substance use, the belief component explains the highest proportion
of the variance with the variable of moral duty to obey the law
consistently being the most powerful belief item with all dependent
variables (frequency of alcohol and marijuana use, use of stronger
drugs, minor delinquent behavior, e.g., running away), and serious
delinquent behavior (e.g., theft of things worth $2 to $50).
In sum, for alcohol and marijuana use, the theory accounts for
29% of the variance, whereas for more serious forms of substance use,
the theory accounts for 17% of the variance. Finally, for hard drug
use, 16% of the variance was explained and for serious delinquency,
18% of the variance was explained by Krohn and Massey's (1980) study.
In contrast with Hirschi's study, the findings of the Krohn and
Massey study moderately supported the theory for all four deviant
behavior scales. It should be considered that attachment was the
weakest predictor while the new conceptualization of commitment was
the most powerful predictor of deviance, as well as the element of
belief. However, Krohn and Massey also demonstrated that the ele-
ments of the bond were more predictive of minor offenses than serious
offenses; and moreover, the commitment and belief elements were more
strongly related to female's deviant behavior than male's deviant
behavior.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
58
Using the same criterion as Krohn and Massey (1980) to measure
social bonding theory, Krohn, Lanza-Kaduce, and Akers (1984) combin-
ed it with social learning perspective. They argued that social
bonding theory and social learning theory can be differentiated in
terms of the former's emphasis on the weakening of conventional bonds
and the latter's emphasis on the importance of the process of acquir-
ing and maintaining deviant definitions and behaviors.
Krohn's et al. (1984) data w~re collected as part of a larger
study of adolescent drug use and drinking: 3,065 male and female
students in 22 schools in seven midwestern communities representing
four different settings (rural farm community, rural non-farm com-
munity, suburb, and city) participated in this study.
Krohn's et al. objective was to examine the differences across
the rural-urban continuum on the variable suggested by both theories
on rates of deviant behavior, and to see the significant interaction
between social context and the variables in both theories. They as-
sumed that social learning variables will explain deviant behavior
equally across community contexts, and that any differences in rates
of delinquency will merely reflect different levels of reinforcement,
association with deviant others, and definitions favorable to devi-
ance found in the respective communities. On the other hand, the
social bonding variable might account for any delinquency differences
found across communities.
According to Krohn's et al. (1984) reported findings, with re-
spect to the social learning perspectives, the combination of the
following variables: (a) peer-association scale, (b) respondent's
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
59
own definitions, and (c) the reinforcement balance of good versus
bad consequences of use--explained 52% of the variance for marijuana
use and 68% of alcohol use. Also, the interactions between the
definitions of both farm and city variables were significant in the
analysis for the four communities. The explained variance for the
farm and city was increased, due to the contribution of the defini-
tions variable. Consequently, "the assumption that there would be
no significant interaction in terms of the social learning analysis
was only partially true" (Krohn et al. 1984, p. 362).
With respect to the bonding variable, Krohn et al. regressed
alcohol and mariJuana on bonding variables in each of the communi-
ties with separate regression runs, and found that for alcohol use,
the social bonding variables accounted for the most variance in the
farm community and the least in the nonfarm community. Similar anal-
yses of marijuana use were run, and they found the bonding variables
were slightly less effective in the farm community than in the non-
farm and suburban communities. In contrast with the social learning
theory, they found a higher explained variance for marijuana use than
for alcohol use in the farm community. In sum, "almost all of the
bonding variables showed a significant relationship to the dependent
variable in some, but not all cases. Community context and social
learning variables didn't interact with context, and finally, the
farm and suburban areas were similar, and the nonfarm and urban areas
were more alike than either of the other two contexts" (1984, p. 361).
Virtually most replications of Hirschi's (1969) theory have used
cross-sectional data. According to Agnew (1985), "this is a serious
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
60
problem, ~9cause, as many researchers have noted, there is good rea-
son to believe that delinquency has a causal impact on social bonds"
(p. 49). Agnew points out that delinquency may lead to family and
school problems, thereby lowering attachment. Delinquency may also
jeopardize the adolescent's current and future involvement in con-
ventional activities, thereby lowering commitment. Finally, delin-
quency may reduce the adolescent's belief in conventional values
through a hardening process of the type described by Minor (1984).
It is possible that a large part of the "correlation between delin-
quency and the social bonds is due to the causal impact of delin-
quency" (p. 48). Agnew (1985) concluded that cross-sectional
research has exaggerated the importance of the theory's capability
to explain delinquency. The alternative as suggested by him, is a
longitudinal study.
Agnew (1985) used data from the Youth in Transition Survey, a
national longitudinal survey of adolescent boys conducted by the
Institute of Social Research at the University of Michigan. The
first wave of data collection started in the fall of 1966 when the
subjects were beginning the lOth grade, using a multistage sampling
procedure. An unbiased sample of 2,213 tenth grade boys was selected
in public high schools throughout the nation. The second wave of
data collection started in the spring of 1968, near the end of the
lOth grade for the selected subjects. The total sample was 1,886
boys, which was 85.2% of the Time 1 Sample.
Agnew (1985) measured delinquency by two self-report scales
adopted from Gold (1966). The first was the 26-item "Total
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
61
Delinquency" scale used by Wiatrowski et al. (1981). This scale
included a variety of delinquent acts including interpersonal
aggression, theft, and vandalism. Agnew (1985) wanted to verify
the findings of Krohn and Massey (1980) which indicated that control
theory best explains minor delinquency. To facilitate this attempt
he used another scale. The second scale was called "Seriousness of
Delinquency." In this scale, 20 items were used which dealt with
minor and serious offenses: (a) theft, (b) fighting, (c) arson, and
(d) robbery. According to his findings, total variance explained by
the total delinquency scale was 28.5% and 14.7% of the variance was
explained by the seriousness of delinquency scale. The results of
the total delinquency scale were similar to Wiatrowski's et al.
(1981). Both studies were based on the same sample. However,
Wiatrowski et al. (1981) were able to explain slightly more of the
variance through the use of the Total Delinquency Scale. Agnew
(1985) attributed that to the inclusion of the SES variable. At any
rate, the findings supported Krohn and Massey's conclusions (1980),
that control theory is most effective in explaining minor delin-
quency.
Agnew's (1985) regression results indicated that only three of
the control variables have a significant effect on Time 2 Total De-
linquency, with one effect significant at only the .05 level.
Grades and beliefs have a negative effect on delinquency and dating
has a positive effect. By examining the amount of variance explain-
ed by the control theory variables over and above Time 1 delinquency,
Agnew found that these variables account for 1.5% of the variance in
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
62
Time 2 seriousness of delinquency and 1. 8% of the va.~iance in Time
2 total delinquency. Thus, longitudinal regressions have a very
small impact on Time 2 delinquency.
Further testing and different interpretations were reported to
explain the low amount of variance explained. Agnew (1985) reported
the "panel analysis was subjected to a number of sources of bias, and
certain of these biases may account for the small effect of control
theory" (p. 54). However, Agnew concluded after presenting his three
different explanations that they are not mutually exclusive and the
findings should be approached very carefully. Longitudinal regres-
sion results were very damaging, yielding support for his argument
that the importance of the theory was exaggerated, and its ability
to explain minor delinquency as Krohn and Massey (1980) concluded is
supported. Also, Agnew indicated that social control theory is un-
important in predicting delinquency among middle to older adolescents,
but may be more important in predicting delinquency among younger
adults. Moreover, the regressions indicated that the control vari-
ables were able to explain only from l-2% of the variance in future
delinquency. Finally, Agnew (1985) suggested that "the small effect
of the control variable was attributed to such things as the causal
impact of delinquency on social control, the high stability of delin-
quency among the adolescents in the sample, and the declining impor-
tance of Hirschi's social control variable among older adolescents"
(p. 54).
A longitudinal study was conducted by Wiatrowski et al. (1981),
using the same data that Agnew (1985) used. Wiatrowski et al. (1981)
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
63
suggested that Hirschi's (1969) study "raised three crucial issues:
(1) the extent to which Hirschi's four elements represent empirically
distinct components of socialization is unclear, (2) why are only
four elements of the bond identified when additional elements of the
bond could be considered, and (3) Hirschi fails to incorporate con-
structs such as family socioeconomic level, ability, and significant
others influenced that research has determined to be important in the
development for their aspiration" (p. 526).
In order to overcome these three weaknesses, Wiatrowski et al.
(1981) constructed measures of each of Hirschi's four elements and
estimated how much each element contributes over and above the others
to the explanation of delinquent behavior. Additionally, Wiatrowski
et al. (1981) examined the structure of the social bond very closely
by using factor analysis to discover and define new elements. Two
multivariate analyses were used, incorporating measures of social
class, ability, and grades as well as elements of the social bond.
The objectives of this study were twofold: (l) to extend and repli-
cate Hirschi's study, and (2) to develop and evaluate a revised
model.
According to Wiatrowski's et al. (1981) findings, "parental at-
tachment and school attachment have a strong negative relation with
delinquent, whereas for grades the coefficient was moderately nega-
tive, as would be predicted by control theory. Dating was strongly
related to delinquency as would be predicted by control theory" (p.
536). With respect to commitment variable, it did not exhibit the
strong negative effects predicted by Hirschi's control theory. The
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
64
strength of the involvement relationship was significant and in the
right direction. With respect to the belief variable, a moderate and
significant negative path coefficient was found. Moreover, "in the
context of statistical controls for ability, social class, and grades
in school, the bond elements which emerge as important explanatory
variables are attachment to parents, dating, attachment to school,
belief, and involvement" (p. 537).
When Wiatrowski et al. (1981) examined how the four elements of
the bond operate simultaneously, the zero-order correlations indicated
that larger correlations with Hirschi's four bond elements do exist,
with one exception, "the element of commitment to college and a high
status career" (p. 538). By examining how the four elements of the
bond operate simultaneously and with controls for ability and school
grades several components were found more important than others, such
as school, which meant that "the bond not only found in the family,
as Hirschi argues, but in part in an educational context" (p. 539).
Wiatrowski et al. (1981) found that social class and ability affect
both parental attachment and a set of school-related components of
the social bond which in turn affect belief (Hirschi found no rela-
tionship between social class and delinquency).
Wiatrowski et al. (1981) pointed out several limitations:
First, the causal ordering implied in the research is
questionable, in the sense that their measurement of the
delinquent behavior asked for reports on behavior over the
last three years (Hirschi asked about behavior over the last
academic year). A second potential limitation is the use of
a single, global measure of delinquency. This measure con-
tains some items pertaining to delinquent behavior in school,
and it is possible that apparent influence of attachment to
school and other school-linked variables on delinquency may
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
65
be due to the inclusion of these items. (p. 536)
Finally, the Wiatrowski et al. (1981) revised model was able to
explain 32.5% of the variables in delinquency, unlike Krohn and Massey
(1980) and Agnew (1985) who suggested that control theory is able to
explain only about 15% of the variance in moderately serious forms of
delinquency.
Another longitudinal study was conducted by Paternoster, Saltz-
man, Waldo, and Chiricos (1983) on a sample of three hundred college
students selected randomly from a list of freshmen enrolled at a ma-
jor state university who were interviewed by trained student inter-
viewers between January and June of 1975 (time 1) and one year later
(time 2).
Paternoster et al. (1983), combined deterrence, social bonding
and social learning theories to explain deviant behavior. With re-
spect to deterrence, respondents were asked to estimate their own
chances of getting arrested for each of five offenses, with response
options ranging on a five-point continuum from "very likely" to "very
unlikely." Social bonding elements were measured as follows: Com-
mitment was measured by the G.P.A. in the university as an indicator
of past investment. For future commitment, respondents were asked a
series of questions about whether they thought their involvement in
each of five illegal behaviors would reduce their chances of achiev-
ing: (a) good grades, (b) a college degree, (c) a high paying job,
(d) a secure job, (e) a position of leadership in school, (f) a good
marriage partner, (g) material well-being, and (h) peer acceptance.
To measure attachment, two scales were constructed. One measured
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
66
parental attachment, the second measured attachment to peers.
Respondents were asked to indicate how important it was that their
mothers, fathers, best friends, boy/girlfriends approve of the things
they do, how much influence the disapproval of these others would
have on their behavior, and finally, whether they would like to be
the same kind of person that these others are.
The third element is involvement, and Paternoster et al. (1983)
measured it by summing the number of hours per week that the respon-
dents reported that they spent studying, attending classes, or par-
ticipating in athletics, church affairs, and similar conventional
activities. And finally, belief was measured by asking respondents
to indicate if they thought it was always wrong to commit each of
five deviant acts. Response options for the five belief items ranged
on a five-point scale from ustrongly agreeu to ustrongly disagree.u
For the social learning perspective, they measured the degree
to which the respondent risked informal sanctions for five illegal
behaviors by asking them to indicate the reactions that their mothers,
fathers, best friends, boyjgirl friends would have if the respondent
were to commit each of them. Response options ranged from uhighly
disapproven to uhighly approven with a high score indicating disap-
proval should the respondent commit the deviant act.
Paternoster et al. (1983) measured the dependent variable by
five behaviors, including: (1) theft under $10, (2) theft under
$100, (3) property damage, (4) writing a check with insufficient
funds, and (5) marijuana use. They gave a score of 1 if partici-
pants reported committing the act in the past year and a score of 0
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
67
if they didn't.
Paternoster's et al. (1983) model did a fair job and provided
reasonable support for control theory. The full model with Time 2
criminal involvement as the dependent variable explained 34% of the
variance, a proportion consistent with other studies of social
control (Krohn & Massey, 1980; Wiatrowski et al., 1981). With the
exception of grades and attachments to parents, the other endogenous
variables were well explained within the model. Secondly, it should
be observed that the best predictors of time 2 criminal involvement
were, in order of importance: Time 1 criminal involvement (~ =
-.381), beliefs(~= -.111), informal sanctions(~= -.167), grades
(~ = -.140), and sex(~= .078).
They also found that informal sanctions and beliefs were good
predictors of criminal involvement which is consistent with Krohn and
Massey (1980); Wiatrowski et al. (1981); and Tittle and Villemez
(1977). In sum, Paternoster et al. found that prior delinquency had
a moderate effect on most social bonds. However, they did not con-
trol for prior levels of social control in their analy~is, unlike
Krohn and Massey's study.
Poole and Regoli (1979) utilized data collected from 72 respond-
ents ranging in age between 14-17 years of age in order to test the
effect of parental support and delinquent friends on delinquency.
They placed the primary emphasis of delinquency control theory on the
correlation between the adolescent's attachment to his/her parents
and subsequent deviant behavior. Control theory hypothesized that
delinquent behavior is the outcome of a weak or broken association of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
68
the delinquent adolescent with society. Along with this notion, lack
of family support is viewed as a correlation factor to delinquency.
In addition, the study found that subjects having low family support
engaged in more frequent, serious and varied delinquent activities
than did subjects having a higher family support.
Cernkovich (1978) utilized data collected from anonymous self-
report questionnaires administered to a sample of 412 male students,
ranging in age from 14 to 18, in a predominantly white high school
located in a midwestern city. Cernkovich examined variables derived
from structural theory (socioeconomic status, and perception of
limited opportunities) as well as control theory (conventional value
orientation and subterranean value orientation) and their relation-
ship to reported delinquency involvement.
The findings indicated that of the two theories, control theory
was the most robust in terms of explaining the most variance in de-
linquency involvement (12.5%) while the structural model explained
only 8% of the variance. Furthermore, Cernkovich indicated that by
using both models, the amount of explained variance increased to
14.5%. In sum, the control theory was supported empirically over
structural theory in terms of explaining delinquency.
Linden and Currie (1977) applied social control theory to test
the relationship between religiosity and drug use. "Social control
theory maintains that the greater an individual's stake in conform-
ity, the stronger hisjher ties to conventional others and to conven-
tional institutions, the lower the likelihood will be of his/her
committing a delinquent act" (pp. 346-347). In other words, church
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
69
is one of the major social institutions and the theory would predict
that ties to the church would provide adolescents with a stake in
conformity which would reduce the likelihood of their engaging in
delinquent behavior.
Linden and Currie's (1977) data were gathered as part of a
larger study carried out by Currie (1973) and consisted of a city-
wide sample of 750 Calgary, Alberta, Canada youths between the ages
of 15-24. Seven hundred and eight individuals completed a useful
questionnaire. According to their reported findings, and unlike
the Hirschi and Stark study (1969), church attendance was not found
to be related to delinquency involvement. The Linden and Currie
(1977) study indicated that those who attended church most frequently
had the lowest degree of illicit drug use. Thus, the present study
(Linden & Currie) supported social control theory by showing that
ties to conventional institutions (church) are a major factor in
reducing the likelihood of involvement in illicit drug use.
Linden and Currie (1977) indicated the major difference between
their study and Hirschi and Stark's (1969) study concerning the fol-
lowing: (a) dependent variable, (b) dates on both studies, and (c)
changes of the meaning of church attendance which have occurred dur-
ing that period. With respect to the first factor, the dependent
variable in Hirschi and Stark's study was deviant behavior, while
in Linden and Currie's study it was illicit drug use. The second
factor is that during the 1960s the influence of religion in both
the U.S.A. and Canada had declined. In sum, the Canadian study
supported social control theory by concluding that church attendance
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
70
has a direct impact and acts as a constraint against drug use, and
that it provides an individual with a stake in conformity as the
theory predicted.
Eve (1978) utilized a questionnaire on 300 eleventh and twelfth-
graders in a county-operated public high school located on the edge
of a southeastern city of about 100,000 people. Males represented
34.7% of the sample and females constituted 55.2%. Twenty percent
of the respondents were black, and 79.2% were white. Eve aimed to
test the efficacy of strain theories, cultural theories, and control
theories to explain delinquent behavior (drug use and traditional
deviance).
While the three theories have the ability to explain variance
in the dependent variables, social control theory has substantially
greater explanatory power than either of the other two theories. In
both strain and cultural theories, equal amounts of variance were
explained. Eve (1978) indicated that "high levels of social control
were strongly related to lower levels of drug use, but less strongly
related to lower levels of traditional deviance" (p. 124). In sum,
Eve's findings suggested that while all three theories have some
significant explanatory power, social control manifested the great-
est explanatory power, followed by culture and strain theories.
Chuen (1988) replicated social control theory by using self-
reported data collected from 3717 Chinese junior and senior high
school students in northern Taiwan. Both schools are located in a
high delinquent area and Chuen's findings have a profound implication
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
71
for social control theory.
Chuen assessed the bond to the school with the question, "Do you
like to go to school (dislike/mixed/like)?" Fifty-one percent of
those who responded as disliking school reported high in self-
reported delinquency, and 22.5% of those who responded as liking
school scored low on self-reported delinquency. With respect to
educational aspiration, Chuen asked the question, "How much educa-
tion do you hope to receive?" Fifty percent of those \<7ho hoped to
receive only a junior high school education scored high on self-
reported delinquency, while 31.9% of those who aspired to finish
graduate school or over scored low in self-reported delinquency.
With respect to involvement in conventional activities, Chuen asked
the question, "How much time do you spend doing homework?" Forty-
three percent who reported one hour or less and 25.5% of those who
reported three hours or more scored high on self-reported delin-
quency. Finally, with respect to the belief variable, Chuen asked
the respondents to respond to the statement, "It is all right to
take advantage of the law, if you can be exempt from punishment."
Forty-four percent who agreed and 23.7% who very strongly disagreed
scored high in self-reported delinquency.
Chuen did not report on all of the items he used in his study,
but it is very obvious that his findings are consistent within the
proportions of control theory.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72
Research Hypothesis
Hirschi (1969) did not develop a hypotheses to test his theory.
However, the four components of the social bond (attachment to signi-
ficant others, commitment to conventional activities, involvement in
conventional activities, and belief in the conventional order) have
been explicitly tested by Hirschi and other criminologists, as we
have seen in this chapter.
The following four hypotheses will be tested in this disser-
tation to determine the relevance of Hirschi's ~ontrol theory on
Jordanian juvenile respondents. These are:
1. There are no significant differences between delinquents
and nondelinquents with respect to attachment to significant others.
2. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to commitment to the conventional acti-
vities.
3. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to involvement in the conventional acti-
vities.
4. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to belief in the conventional order.
These hypotheses are simply the components of social bond form-
ulated in a test of significance format. Although Hirschi did not
test hypotheses as mentioned above, his study certainly implies an
examination of this nature.
This is an exploratory study which takes place in another cul-
tural setting, and the essential purpose is to find out if the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
73
original theoretical model is applicable or not in Jordanian socie-
ty. Thus, the reconceptualization of the theory is irrelevant to
the present study. However, the theory's shortcomings will be ad-
dressed and the results of the present study will be compared to
those of Hirschi's work.
Summary
Chapter II presented a brief statement about control theories in
general and an extensive review of the literature, specifically re-
plicated studies of Hirschi's (1969) theory. Finally, the hypothe-
ses of this study were presented.
Control theories were classified into four groups: (1) social
structural disorganization, (2) cultural disorganization, (3) learn-
ing theories, and (4) exchange theories. According to this set of
theories people deviate either to enhance their economic position
(Merton, 1937), social status (Cohen, 1955), or conform to a differ-
ent subculture. Finally, people deviate with expectations of distri-
butive justice (Homans, 1961) or fair exchange (Blau, 1964). The
suggestion was made to place these theories under the rationality
model. In this sense people deviate or conform after rationally
calculating what they stand to lose or gain by their behavior.
The second set of theories are: (1) the containment theory, (2) the
bonding theory, and (3) the role relationship theory. According to
this group of theories, people don't need motivation to deviate, as
deviance is part of the natural order. Our task is to explain why
they don't deviate instead of asking why they do it.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
74
As mentioned earlier, the bonding theory is the purest type in
this cluster of theories. No other theory has received attention
like Hirschi's (1969) theory. The general theoretical framework of
this theory is to answer the question "why don't they do it"
(deviate)? Thus, Hirschi explains lack of deviance by social inte-
gration, or the social solidarity in the society. Whenever the ties
of the individual to his/her society become weak, the chances of de-
viance increase. Moreover, as we have mentioned earlier, Hirschi
(1969) and others thoroughly investigated this relationship by exam-
ining the four major premises of the theory. Thus, this cluster of
theories was placed under the solidarity model in the sense that
solidarity or social integration is a key concept to link all these
theories together.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER III
RESEARCH METHODS
The purpose of this chapter is to present the methodological
aspects of this research in four sections. The first section pro-
vides a description of the setting and the sample. The second sec-
tion explains the methodology involved in the data collection. The
third section offers a description and measurement of variables in
the study. The fourth section focuses on the statistical techniques
utilized in analyzing the collected data.
The Setting: The City of Tafielah
Tafielah is an ancient city located in the southern part of Jor-
dan, inhabited by 16,120 people, according to the 1985 Jordanian Census
Bureau report (Public Census Bureau, 1985). Tafielah's governate has
six major localities (districts), administered by the city, but each
locality has its own municipality or center. The total population of
the governate is 42,820 inhabitants according to the 1987 Census Bureau
report; 16,700 are urban (as defined by the Census Bureau as a
population of 5,000 in 1979), and 26,120 are rural (Public Census
Bureau, 1987).
The six major districts of Tafielah's governate are the follow-
ing: "(1) Bsaira is the largest, and it is inhabited by 10,070
people; (2) El-Hesa ranks second in terms of population and is inha-
bited by 5,540 persons. Most of its population work in the phosphate
75
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
76
fields; (3) Qadisiyyeh is inhabited by 4,700 people; (4) Ain El-
Baidha has a population of 3,085; (5) Emah is inhabited by 1,250
people; (6) Gharandel is inhabited by 1,555 people" (Public Census
Bureau, 1987, p. 50).
During the last two decades, the kingdom has witnessed a rapid
increase of services in terms of social organizations, social cen-
ters, cooperative societies, etc. Tafielah's governate has 295
agricultural cooperative societies (it is mainly a rural city); 72
saving and lending societies; and 383 multiple purpose societies
(such as eight charitable organizations; four motherhood centers,
etc.). Along with these organizations, the Jordanian government has
paid increased attention to the health services as evidenced by the
establishment of nine public health centers, two dental clinics, two
pharmacies, nine post-offices and the government hospital. In sum,
23 governmental services have been established in the city employing
1,156 persons.
From an administrative point of view, six municipalities were
founded to serve the needs of the people residing in villages of less
than 5,000 people. Also one public library in the city was establish-
ed in addition to a library in every school. To summarize, the Taf-
ielains in terms of occupation and profession, are mostly farmers,
governmental employees, and construction workers. Thousands of them
migrated to the capital in the fifties and early sixties and estab-
lished their own section (community) in an attempt to find better
conditions in the city. The above information suggests that the
majority of Tafielains have similar incomes, occupations, and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
77
government services as compared with any city in the country, with
the exception of the more abundant opportunities available in the
capital. Therefore, it can be inferred that the population of
Tafielah have similar levels of education, and share equal rungs on
the socio-economic ladder.
The Sample
As with most public schools serving specific geographic areas in
the country, the student body of Tafielah Governate is a microcosm of
the city and its environs. In the academic year beginning in Septem-
ber, 1989, 13,303 students enrolled in Tafielah's Governate School
and its localities. These students were distributed into 29 male
schools (7,231), 23 female schools (6,072), and 36 mixed schools, but
the student body registered in the Ministry of Education is 13,303
students only (private schools are excluded). These students are
enrolled in elementary schools, preparatory schools, and secondary
schools (Ministry of Planning--Jordan, 1989).
Because Tafielah's Governate is divided into six localities,
each locality or district has a secondary school which serves that
specific locality (except Gharandel's locality which does not have a
secondary school). Tafielah's city has two secondary schools (male
schools). The sample consisted of what are known in Jordan as the
second secondary classes (males, comparable to eleventh grade in the
U.S.A.) of Tafielah's secondary schools, Ain El-Baidha, Aimeh, Bsaria,
Prince Hasan, Qadisiyyeh, and Hesa schools. These seven schools re-
present the city and its localities. It should be mentioned here
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
78
that the classes varied in size from one locality to another. The
sample included 147 pupils in the eleventh grade in the seven
schools (The Annual Book, 1989).
Methodology
Questionnaire
As mentioned earlier, the research instrument was derived from
Hirschi's (1969) study. However, Hirschi's original instrument
was determined to be too long, and therefore, too time consuming for
the purposes of the investigation. All 107 items included in this
instrument came directly or indirectly from Hirschi's instrument,
with the exception of the graffiti item, which was borrowed from El-
liott and Ageton (1980).
The criteria of selecting the items were based on how well these
items fit the Jordanian society. Thus, items related to race, sex,
and school activities such as human relations clubs, art and dance
clubs, etc., were excluded. Moreover, items related to leisure
activities such as drive-in restaurants, and riding around in a car
were also excluded.
Only items related to family, peers, values, and academic work
were included in this study. Some alterations were needed to use
this instrument, such as the different grading system used in Jordan.
The grades of A, B, and C were exchanged for 90s, 80s and 70s,
respectively, to fit the Jordanian society.
The 107 items used in this research were screened very care-
fully, and each element of the social bond was measured by several
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
79
items.
The researcher, fluent in Arabic, translated the instrument into
very simple and understandable Arabic. Then the English copy was
given to two Jordanian doc.torate students in order to translate it to
Arabic. Having three copies of the questionnaire in Arabic provided
the researcher with an indication if there was any genuine inconsist-
ency. The subjects responded to a questionnaire in very simple and
understandable Arabic, and the students answered individually and
anonymously by marking (X) or by writing the answer. The researcher
and the other two students reviewed the questionnaire item by item.
Moreover, a pretest of the instrument was conducted on a small sample
which matched the characteristics of the targeted population.
Pretest
The research design of this study called for the use of a pre-
test for the purposes of time estimation and difficulties in under-
standing the questionnaire and to ascertain what, if any, refinements
should be made with respect to the research instrument. Thus, the
pretest was viewed as a means of testing materials, methods, and
instruments before embarking on the data collection phase of this
study.
The population for the pretest consisted of 10 eleventh graders
in a nearby city, matching the characteristics of the original sample.
Information was gathered by self-administered questionnaire (107
questions). With respect to the time factor, 45 minutes were needed
to finish the questionnaire. The pretest sample didn't experience any
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
80
problem in answering the questions. The pretest was encouraging and
viewed as a reliable criteria reference.
The Questionnaire Administration
The director of the educational department in the Tafielah
Governate was contacted by telephone, requesting his permission and
assistance. A copy of the questionnaire was sent to his office, with
a letter apprising him that he would be contacted by telephone again.
The director contacted each principal in the seven schools, and no
objection was raised by them. However, each principal was contacted
individually in order to set a time table to conduct the survey.
On January 15, 1990, the data collection process began with the
assistance of three trained individuals while in the presence of some
of the school personnel. Each person involved in data collection read
a letter to the students which included instructions as well as re-
minding them that their information and cooperation in the study was
very important for their community's future and also for the success
of the research. Moreover, it was made clear that their information
was confidential and nobody would know who participated in the sur-
vey.
A self-reported questionnaire was given utilizing items selected
from a test instrument previously used in Hirschi's (1969) study.
Each student in the eleventh grade in each school in the Governate
was asked to voluntarily respond to the questionnaire (containing 107
questions, see Appendix A). Respondents were asked to answer the
items on the questionnaire in an honest way in order to measure their
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
81
attitudes, values, and beliefs. There were no right or wrong answers.
The questionnaire which contained 107 questions included items
about values, attitudes, beliefs, or examined the pupil's ties to his
community, parents, schools, and peers.
Given the time constraints and financial burden placed on the
investigators, females were excluded from this study. Based on the
outcome of this study, however, the investigator will include them in
future works. Moreover, females were excluded from this study for
the following three reasons:
1. Official data have shown that young Jordanian males are more
likely to commit criminal acts than young Jordanian.females, 95% and
5% respectively.
2. Males and females have different socialization growth rates.
He expect stronger parental attachment for females because research
has shown that parents are more responsive to female children which
is consistent with the control theory premises (Lewis, 1972), and
parents are less likely to use physical punishment techniques un fe-
male children (Duncan & Duncan, 1978).
3. It has been mentioned earlier that in Jordan only two female
institutions were found to serve the needs of nonconforming females,
which gives us an idea that female delinquency is not as serious a
problem.
To assure anonymity and confidentiality, the subjects were asked
to deposit their completed surveys in a box placed in the same room.
Then, the researcher screened them (not in the research place) to see
if there was any problem or an incomplete questionnaire. However,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
82
none of these difficulties or problems was found.
Measurement Problems and Description of Variables
Caplovitz (1983) asserts that when deciding which indicators to
select to measure a concept, the researcher is confronted with the
problem of assessing the adequacy of the indicator. How good a job
does it do in measuring the concept?
Elaborate statistical procedures have evolved for deciding
which cluster of indicators hang together to measure a con-
cept, but the amateur and even the more advanced researcher
are well advised to avoid factor analysis and rely instead on
simple procedures for constructing indices. However, we
expect the items to be related but we do not want to find too
strong a correlation between the indicators. (p. 223).
One dependent variable was used to test the hypothesis with four
major independent variables. However, many variables were measured
by a single item such as father's occupation, family status, father's
education, age, mother's education, occupation, number of sisters,
brothers, etc.
Of initial concern was the measurement of delinquent behavior,
as was the case in Hirschi's (1969) original work. Hirschi did not
develop a scale to measure delinquent behavior, but rather borrowed
from two different scales: Nye and Short's (1957) seven item de1in-
quency scale, and Dentler and Monroe's (1961) five item theft scale.
The Jordanian study borrowed five items from Hirschi's scale. Those
items were: theft of 3 JD, theft of 3-30 JD, theft of over 30 JD,
battery, and vandalism. One item (graffiti) was borrowed from Elliott
and Ageton (1980). Thus, the Jordanian study demanded a more thor-
ough analysis of an individual's criminal activities.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
83
Some consideration was given to applying the Sellin and Wolfgang
(1964) seriousness weighing schema to the reported deviant activities.
This idea was rejected because the current literature had questioned
the validity of applying a weighing scale to self-reported delinquency.
Of major concern here is the fallacy of a universal consensus with re-
gard to how serious or not serious any offense actually is (Bennett
& Lynch, 1990; Okada, 1987).
The Sellin and Wolfgang (1964) scale requires knowledge of degree
of danger, either actual or perceived, specific dollar amount of dam-
age, and degree of severity of offense that simply are not available
in this study. However, the lack of this information need not be
viewed as a shortcoming of the current research.
Miethe (1982) argues that perceptions of crime seriousness are
simply not universally recognized. "In reality, research that addres-
ses perceptions of crime seriousness, generally derive their findings
by some methodological artifact or by some underlying investigator-
generated normative structure, and therefore are not the product of
empirical evidence" (quoted in Okada, 1987, p. 66).
One of the major criticisms of Hirschi's (1969) scale is the is-
sue of trivial offenses which is also of major concern in this study.
Generally, most research in the area of criminology relies either on
criminal justice statistics or information collected from school boys.
In both cases, the researcher will confront a problem: in the former
instance, the issue of random sampling will be of concern, and for
the latter, the issue of trivial offenses will emerge. Okada (1987)
reported that middle-class boys consistently reported offenses that
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
84
were either status offenses or minor offenses. These included:
"lied about age, cheating on tests, skipping classes, drunkenness,
consuming alcohol, evading payments, and stole from family" (p. 66).
The issue of trivial offenses, however, is not a problem in the
present research. After consulting the frequencies of 13 items in
this study, serious offenses were reported more than trivial offenses:
battery (67.4%) was first, vandalism was second (14.1%), theft up to
three JD was third (13.5%), theft of 3-30 JD was fourth (9.9%), graf-
fiti was fifth (6.8%), and theft of over 30 JD was sixth (6.2%).
Surprisingly, when asked if they ever thought of themselves as
being a delinquent, 52.4% of the respondents responded no, 24.1% did
not know what the word "delinquent" meant, and only 23.5% thought of
themselves as delinquent. This difference suggests that even though
these boys actively participated in delinquent behavior, the majority
(76.5%) did not consider themselves delinquent.
Measurement of Delinquent Behavior
Actual delinquent behavior was measured by responses to the
abbreviated version of Hirschi's (1969) scale adapted for this dis-
sertation. The 13 offenses were screened and six items were adapted
as mentioned earlier. The selection of the items was based on empir-
ical tests (reliability). Responses were as follows: (a) never,
(b) one to three times, (c) four to six times, and (d) seven times or
more.
A dichotomous dependent variable of not delinquent or delinquent
was created for analytic purposes. The six items (battery, vandalism,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
85
graffiti, and three types of theft) included were recognized as being
punishable by the law. Therefore, in constructing specific delin-
quent categories a somewhat arbitrary, but methodologically adequate
method was utilized. The six items were empirically tested before
the final decision was made. ALPHA is .6870 and standardized ALPHA
item is .7254, (see Appendix B), and none of the items was found to
be very highly or weakly correlated. Therefore, the researcher con-
cluded that a valid index was used to measure delinquent behavior
in Jordanian society.
The four independent variables which are described in this
section are: (1) attachment to significant others (parents, schools,
and peers); (2) commitment to the conventional order {education and
occupation); (3) involvement in conventional activities (education);
and (4) belief in the conventional order. These variables are the
major independent variables explored for the purpose of this re-
search.
Attachment to.significant others. This variable was subcategor-
ized into three parts: {1) attachment to parents, (2) attachment to
school, and (3) attachment to peers. Attachment to parents was di-
vided into two parts: (1) attachment to mother, and (2) attachment
to father. Attachment to mother is operationally measured by the
follow-ing items: {a) Does your mother know where you are when you
are away from home? (b) Does your mother know who you are with when
you are away from home? These two items were correlated (.3982).
The responses for these two questions were: Usually, sometimes,
never and these responses were coded 3, 2, 1 and zero for nonresponse
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
86
respectively. This index was called Mother's Index I.
Several items on the questionnaire were chosen to measure the
intimacy of communication between mother and child. The items were:
(a) Does your mother ever ask about what you are doing in school?
(b) Do you share your thoughts and feelings with your mother? Again
the responses were: Usually, sometimes, never and were coded high,
medium, and low respectively. This index was called Mother's Index
II. These two items were correlated .1504.
Another index to measure intimacy of communication between
mother and child was constructed, distinguished from Index II by
the fact that the flow of communication was from the mother to the
child. The items were: (a) When you don't know why your mother
makes a rule, will she explain the reason? (b) Does your mother make
rules that seem unfair to you? The same responses were used for this
index which was called Mother's Index III. The correlation for these
two items is .3279.
Identification with mother was measured by the following ques-
tion: "Would you like to be the kind of person your mother is?"
Responses were in every way, in most ways, in some ways, in just a
few ways, not at all. These responses were recorded as follows:
high = in every way and in most ways; medium = in just a few ways,
and low= not at all (3-1). ALPHA for mother's attachment index
is .5029 and standardized item ALPHA is .4535.
All these previous items were tested for reliability and corre-
lation in order to justify combining them together (see Appendix B).
Particularily when a high correlation was found, items were deleted
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
87
from the analysis.
Attachment to father was measured by the following items:
(a) Does your father know where you are when you are away from home?
(b) Does your father know who you are with when you are away from
home? The responses for these two questions were: usually, some-
times, never and coded 3, 2, 1 and zero for nonresponse, respective-
ly. This index was called Father's Index I. These two items were
correlated .243.
In order to measure the intimacy of communication between the
father and the child, the following items were used: (a) Does your
father ever ask about what you are doing in school? (b) Do you share
your thoughts and feelings with your father? The responses were
treated the same as in the mother's case. This index was called
Father's Index II and the items were correlated (.2792).
In order to measure intimacy of communication between the child
and the father (father to child) the items were: (a) When you don't
know why your father makes a rule, will he explain the reason?
(b) Does your father make rules that seem unfair to you? The same
responses were used for this index which was called Father's Index
III. Correlation of these two items is .4832.
Finally, identification with father was measured by the follow-
ing item "would you like to be the kind of person your father is?"
The responses were: in every way, in most ways, in some ways, in
just a few ways, not at all. These reposes were recorded as follows:
high in every way and in most ways; medium= in just a few ways,
and low= not at all (3, 2, 1).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
88
Thus, attachment to parents was measured by 14 items. All these
items were coded the same for statistical reasons. Again, all these
were empirically tested before using them. For father's index, ALPHA
is .47 and standardized item ALPHA is .43.
Attachment to peers was measured by the following five items:
1. "How much do you think most teachers like the group of friends
you are with?" The responses ranged from "very much" to "I don't know."
The seven responses were recoded as follows: High (3) = "very much,"
medium (2) = "fairly well," and low (1) = "not much, not at all, I
have no friends and I don't know."
2. "Would you like to be the kind of person your best friends
are?" Again responses were recoded as follows: High (3) = "in most
ways," medium (2) = "in a few ways," and low (1) = "not at all and I
have no best friends."
3. "How much do you think most students like the group of friends
you go with?" The responses were treated as in item number 1.
4. "Do you respect your best friends' opinions about the impor-
tant things in life?" The five responses were recoded from high
(3) ="completely," medium (2) =''pretty much," and low (1) ="a lit-
tle, not at all," and "I have no best friends."
5. "Have any of your close friends ever been picked up by the
police?" Responses ranged from high (3) ="four or. more," medium=
(2) "1-3" and low (1) = "nobody."
These five items were combined to form an index of attachment to
peers, after being empirically tested (ALPHA= .4580, standardized item
ALPHA= .4191).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
89
Attachment to school. The following seven items were used to
measure this variable:
1. "What kind of grades do you think you are capable of get-
ting?" The possible responses to this question were seven ranked from
high to low. These responses were recoded as follows: high (3)
"80-100," medium (2) = "69-79," and low (1) = "49 and below."
2. "In general, do you like or dislike school?" The responses
to this question were "I like it," "like/dislike it about equally,"
and "dislike it." These responses were recoded 3, 2, 1 respectively.
3. "Do you care what teachers think of you?" The responses were:
"I care a lot," "I care some," "I don't care much." Again, these
responses were recoded 3, 2, 1 respectively.
4. "Teachers pick on me."
5. "I feel nervous and tense in school."
6. "It is none of the school's business if a student wants to
smoke outside of the classroom."
The responses to items 4, 5, and 6 ranged from "strongly agree"
to "strongly disagree." If a student strongly agreed, one point
was given. On the other hand, if he strongly disagreed, three points
were given.
7. "How do you rate yourself in school ability compared with
other students?" Six responses were used to answer this question.
These responses were recoded as follows: High = "among the best" and
"above the average," medium= "about average," and low= "below aver-
age," "among the worst" and "I don't know" (3, 2, 1 respectively). From
highest to lowest (ALPHA= .51 and standardized ALPHA item= .53).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
90
Commitment to conventional activities was measured by the fol-
lowing items:
1. "How much schooling would you like to get?"
2. "How much schooling do you actually expect to get?"
Three responses were used to answer these two questions. These
responses were coded as follows: High school or less = 1, Junior
College 2, and College graduation= 3.
3. "What kind of grades to you think you are capable of get-
ting?" This question was coded as mentioned earlier.
4. "How important is getting good grades to you personally?"
Four responses were used from high to low as mentioned earlier.
5. "How important do you think grades are for getting the kind
of job you want when you finish high school?" Five responses were
used and they were recoded as follows: High= "very important,"
medium= "somewhat important," and low= "unimportant," "! have no
idea," and "1 don't plan to work when I finish high school."
6. "As you see it now, do you plan to graduate from high
school?" Three responses were used to answer this question. These
responses were recoded as follows: high (3) ="yes," medium (2)
"yes, but leave for a while and come back," and low ( 1) = "no."
7. "The only reason to have a job is money?" Five responses
were used to answer this question. These responses were recoded as
follows: high (3) = "disagree" and "strongly disagree," medium
(2) ="undecided," and low (l) "agree" and "strongly agree" (3,
2, 1, respectively). (ALPHA= .3820, standardized ALPHA item=
.3647).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
91
Involvement in conventional activities was measured by the
subjects' responses to eight questions:
1. "How much time do you spend doing homework?" Values range
from 3 to 1, highest to lowest. The responses were: 3 indicates
1-1/2 hours or more, 2 indicates 1 hour, and 1 indicates a half hour
or less.
2. "How often do you work in the garden with your mother?"
3. "How often do you work in the garden with your father?"
Three responses were used to answer these two questions. These
responses were coded as follows: high (3) = "usually," medium (2) =
"sometimes," and low (1) ="never" (3, 2, 1, respectively).
4. "Kinds of grades you are capable of getting." Values range
from 3 to 1, highest to lowest. The response of 3 indicates 80-100,
2 indicates 60-79, and 1 indicates 59 and below.
5. "Do you finish your homework?"
6. "Do your teachers check your homework?" Values range from
3 to 1 (highest to lowest). The response of 3 indicates "always," 2
indicates "seldom," and 1 indicates "never" and "we are not given any
homework."
7. "Have you ever been suspended from school?" Values range
from 3 to 1, with 1 indicating "often," 2 indicating "a few times,"
and 3 indicating "once or twice," and "never."
8. "On the average, how many times do you go to the Mosque?"
Responses were: (a) "daily," (b) "every Friday," (c) "once a month,"
(d) "only on occasions," (e) "I don't go at all." Possible values
range from 3 to 1. A value of 3 was assigned to responses (a), (b)
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
92
and (c). Response d was given a value of 2, and the last response
(e) was given a 1 (highest to lowest). ALPHA= .3820, and stand-
ardized ALPHA item= .3696.
Belief in the conventional order was measured by subject re-
sponse to the questions:
1. "I have a lot of respect for the Tafielah police."
2. "It is alright to get around the law if you can get away
with it."
3. "Most criminals really should not be blamed for the things
they have done."
4. "Most people in government are not really interested in the
problems of families like mine."
5. "Suckers deserve to be taken advantage of."
6. "I can't seem to stay out of trouble no matter how hard I
try."
7. "The man who leaves the keys in his car is as much to blame
for its theft as the man who steals it."
8. "Policemen try to give all kids an even break."
Items 1 and 8 were recoded as follows: "strongly agree" and
"agree" = 3 points; undecided 2 points, and "disagree" and "strong-
ly disagree" = 1 point; items 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7 were recoded as
follows: "Strongly agree" and "agree" = 1 point, "undecided" = 2
points, and "disagree" and "strongly disagree" = 3 points. Reli-
ability for these items was .26 and (standardized ALPHA item= .21).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
93
Data Analysis Procedures
In order to test the research hypothesis and answer other relat-
ed questions, a variety of summary statistics were computed for des-
criptive purposes. Analysis of ordinal data was made with the assis-
tance of appropriate statistics for each level. Cross-tabulation was
computed and all analysis was done at the .05 critical value. T-test
was calculated for test of differences for ordinal data, and the dif-
ference between means of the two groups was used to test the null
hypothesis. Correlation also was used to find out about the direction
and strength of the relationship between the dependent and the inde-
pendent variables.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER IV
FINDINGS
The purpose of the present chapter is threefold: First, to pre-
sent a descriptive profile of the 147 boys that comprise the sample
for this investigation, how they are distributed in terms of schools,
age, parent's occupation and education, delinquent activities, broken
homes, family status, religiosity, etc.; second, to compare the pre-
sent study's results with Hirschi's (1969) study, and third, to test
the four hypotheses formulated to ascertain the degree to which they
are supportive of Hirschi's four bonding factors: attachment to
significant others, commitment to the conventional activities,
involvement in the conventional activities, and, finally, belief in
the conventional order.
Profile of the Sample
Examination of the descriptive data revealed that the subjects
were unevenly divided among the seven schools. The distribution of
the subjects was as follows:
Table 1
The Sample Distribution According to School's Name
School Name Frequency %
'
Tafielah Secondary School 35 23.8
Emah Secondary School 12 8.2
94
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
95
Table 1--Continued
School Name Frequency %
Besara Secondary School 29 19.7
Al-Gaddesyah Secondary School 13 8.8
Ein-Elbedeh Secondary School 7 4.8
El-Hesa Secondary School 31 21.1
Prince Hasan Secondary School 18 12.2
Total 147 100.0
Respondents ranged in age from 15 to 18 with a mean age of
16.9.
Table 2 shows the age distribution of the population of the study.
Table 2
The Respondents' Distribution According to Their Age
Age Frequency %
15 2 1.4
16 33 22.4
17 83 56.5
18 29 19.7
Total 147 100.0
--------------------------------------------------------
According to this table, 78.9% of the subjects were 16 and 17 years
of age at the time of the study, 19.7% of the students were 18 and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
96
only 1.4% (2 students) were 15 years old at that time.
The parents of this population were reported to be in the fol-
lowing occupations: 78.9% of their fathers were unskilled and semi-
skilled laborers (peasants, construction workers), 16.3% were skilled
laborers (machine operators, merchants, governmental employees), 2.7%
were professionals (physicians, lawyers). With respect to their
mothers' occupations, 94.6% were unskilled laborers (housewives) and
only 4.1% were professional (physicians, school teachers).
The parents of this population were generally uneducated,
especially the mothers, of whom 91.2% had no formal education. Only
3.4% finished four year colleges and 5.4% had some high school.
In looking at the fathers' educational level, 37.4% had no formal
education, 46.3% had some high school education, 8.8% finished high
school, 6.1% had four years of college, and 1.4% had two years (i.e.,
graduated from junior colleges). According to the traditional belief
in Jordanian society, the men have the responsibility of supporting
their families, and thus it is no surprise that fathers had a better
education, especially in a small conservative town.
In describing the delinquent activities of the subjects, 23.8%
never committed any delinquent act, while 76.2% had committed one or
more delinquent acts. The dependent variable was dichotomized into
delinquent or nondelinquent for statistical analysis. When subjects
asked if they ever thought of themselves as delinquents, 52.4% report-
ed they did not, 24.5% didn't know what the word meant, and only 22.1%
thought of themselves as delinquents (mentioned earlier). Regarding
contact with police, 91.2% had never been picked up by the police and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
97
only 8.8% had had contact with police.
That crime is a group phenomenon is a common observation in the
literature. When subjects were asked about their friends' behavior
with respect to police contact, 63.2% reported that they never had
any friends picked up by the police, 34.0% reported having 1-3
friends who had contact with the police, and only 2.8% had four or
more friends picked up by the police.
With regard to their natural parents, 98% of the sample reported
living with their natural fathers, only 2.0% reported living with
their stepfathers, 91.8% reported living with their natural mothers,
4.8% reported living with their stepmothers. Only 3.4% reported
their natural mothers were not living with them. Therefore, it can
be assumed that a stable traditional nuclear family exists for this
population.
With respect to siblings, 95.9% reported having more than one
brother, 96.0% reported having more than one sister, 39.5% reported
having no older brothers and sisters, while 60.5% reported having
more than one brother and sister.
One of the early assumptions of this research was that the re-
sidents of Tafielah Governate and its localities, by and large,
shared an equivalent economic status. When asked the question,
"comparing your family with other families in your neighborhood, how
would you relate yours in terms of living style?" It was a real sur-
prise that the data did not support this assumption, in that 59.2%
of the respondents answered that their families were "better off" and
only 20.4% of the respondents indicated that they were the same,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
98
15.0% of the respondents answered "less well off," "worse than
others," and "I don't know." However, 8 subjects (5.4%) did not
answer this question.
In examining religiosity in the population, when asked the ques-
tion "on an average, how many times do you go to the Mosque?",
32.7% reported going daily, 59.2% every Friday, 7.5% once a month,
and less than 1% (.6%) reported not going at all. Therefore, it
could be assumed that the subjects are regular Mosque goers.
In describing their interest in school, well over half (57.8%
of the sample) said they liked school, an additional (36.1%) answered
that they like it and dislike it equally, and only 6.1% disliked
school. In further addressing this group's interest in academia,
72.1% were planning to attend a four year college, while 2.0% wanted
to finish high school only.
Comparison to Hirschi's Causes of Delinquency
In this section the writer will compare the responses of the
Jordanian subjects with Hirschi's respondents in the United States.
Analyses were conducted to see whether or not Hirschi's theory pro-
vided a reasonable explanation of juvenile delinquency in Jordan.
Some variables were selected from Hirschi's work that represent the
elements of the social bond and they were compared to the dependant
variable (nondelinquent/delinquent) that was constructed for this
dissertation.
Hirschi (1969) suggested that attachment to parents is very
important in deterring delinquency. He argues that "when parental
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
99
attachment is strong, parental values are more readily accepted"
(p. 83). To assess the bond of parental attachment, the subjects
were asked: "Would you like to be the kind of person your father
is?" The possible responses to this question were: "In every way,"
"in most ways," "in some ways," "in just a few ways," and "not at
all."
For purposes of comparison, the five responses above were col-
lapsed into the following categories: High= "in every way" and "in
most ways"; Medium= "in some ways"; Low= "in just a few ways" and "not
at all." The reader should note that the data in the following table
represent the percent of the comparison that involves the number of
subjects in each study whose responses place them in the high cate-
gory and who reported no delinquency or some delinquency involvement,
followed by the number (again shown in percentages) of subjects whose
responses place them in the medium category and who reported no
delinquency or some delinquency involvement. A similar comparison
will be made for those who fall in the low category.
Table 3
Self-Reported Delinquency by Identification With Father:
"Would you Like to be the Kind of Person Your Father is?"
Reported Acts High Medium Low
Nondelinquents HM 65%'l\" HM 55%* HM 41%*
IM 31% IM 25% IM 21%
Delinquents HM 35%* HM 45%* HM 59%*
IM 69% IM 75% IM 79%
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
100
Table 3--Continued
Reported Acts High Medium Low
Total HM* (525) (387) (311) 1223
IM ( 26) ( 44) ( 77) 147
HM ~Hirschi's male subjects
IM = Irfaifeh's male subjects
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 20
(1969, p. 92).
According to Table 3, of Hirschi's 1223 subjects, 525 were in the
high category. Of the 525, 340 (65%) respondents were placed in the
high category and reported no delinquency involvement. However, 184
(35%) were also found in the high category and they did report one or
more delinquency involvements.
In the present study, of the 147 subjects, 26 were in the high
category. Of the 26, 8 (31%) of the respondents were placed in the high
category but with no delinquency involvement, while 18 (69%) were in
the high category and each reported one or more delinquent acts. This
comparison shows that of the subjects with a high attachment to their
fathers, a much higher percentage of those in this (Irfaifeh's) study
reported being involved in delinquency (69% vs. 35%).
Further comparison was made in the medium and low categories in
much the same fashion as noted in the above example. For example, of
Hirschi's 1223 subjects, 387 were in the medium category. Of the 387,
213 (55%) respondents were placed in the medium category and reported
no delinquency involvement. However, 174 (45%) were also found in the
medium category and they did report one or more delinquent acts. Of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
101
Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 44 were in the medium category. Of the 44,
11 (25%) respondents were in the medium category but reported no delin-
quency involvement, while 33 (75%) in the medium category and each
reported one or more delinquent acts. This comparison shows that of
the subjects with a medium attachment to their fathers, a much higher
percentage of those in Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in
delinquency (75% vs. 45%).
For the low category, of Hirschi's 1223 subjects, 311 were in
the low category. Of the 311, 139 (41%) respondents were in the low
category and they reported no delinquency involvement. However, 172
(59%) were also found in the low category and they did report one or
more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147, 16 (21%) respondents were in
the low category and reported no delinquency involvement. However,
61 (79%) were also found in the low category and they did report one
or more delinquent acts. This comparison shows that of the subjects
with a low attachment to their fathers, a much higher percentage of
those in Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency (79%
vs. 59%).
In summary, with respect to Irfaifeh's subjects, 18 (69%) of the
respondents who identified themselves as being highly attached to
their fathers reported one or more delinquent acts and 77 (79%) of
the respondents who did identify themselves wanting to be like their
father in a few ways or not at all in the low category did report
delinquent acts. Hirschi's percentages were 184 (35%) and 173 (59%)
respectively. Therefore, the Jordanian data were consistent theo-
retically with Hirschi's theory.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
102
Identification with mother was measured by the same question
and the same response was used. Interestingly enough, none of the
Jordanian subjects identified himself in every way or in most ways
with his mother. The whole sample was placed either in the medium
or the low category. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 65 were in the
medium category. Of the 65, 18 (28%) respondents in the medium cate-
gory did not report any delinquency involvement. However, 47 (72%)
respondents in the medium category did report one or more delinquent
acts.
With respect to the low category, of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 17
(20%) respondents did not report any delinquency involvement, while
65 (80%) did report one or more delinquent acts. Comparing father's
to mother's percentages, it was noticed that identification with
father and self-reported delinquency is slightly stronger in deterr-
ing delinquency than identification with mother, contrary to Hinde-
lang's (1973) findings "that affection for mother and self-reported
delinquency is stronger than the relation between affection for
father and self-reported delinquency" (p. 476). Hirschi (1969) did
discuss this matter by stating that "the relation between identifi-
cation with the mother and delinquency is somewhat stronger than
the relation between identification with the father and delinquency"
(p. 92). However, Hirschi did not provide any data to sustain his
statements. In sum, the Jordanian data with respect to identifica-
tion with mother and delinquency did not support Hirschi's theory.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
103
Attachment to School
Hirschi (1969) stated that attachment to school is related to
delinquency, i.e., "those who do poorly in school reduce their in-
terests in school and hence, are free to the extent of their reduced
attachment to, commitment to, and involvement in school-related
activities, to commit delinquent acts" (pp. 122-124). A comparison
between Hirschi's subjects and this dissertation's subjects was made
to assess the bond to the school (see Table 4).
Table 4
Self Reported Delinquency by Perceived Academic Ability:
"How Do You Rate Yourself in School Ability Compared With
Other Students in Your School?"
Self-Reported Acts High Medium Low
Nondelinquents HM 59%* HM 57%* HM 36%*
IM 51% IM 33% IM 6%
Delinquents HM 41%* HM 43%* HM 64%*
IM 49% IM 67% IM 94%
Total HM* (514) (619) (94) 1227
Total IM (103) ( 27) (17) 147
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 31,
(1969, p. 118).
For purposes of comparison, the four possible responses to the
above question were collapsed into three categories: High "among
the best" and "above the average"; Medium= "average"; and Low= "below
average." Of Hirschi's 1227 subjects, 514 were in the high category.
Of the 514, 303 (59%) respondents in the high category did not report
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
104
any delinquency involvement, while 211 (41%) respondents in the
same category did report one or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's
147 subjects, 103 were in the high category. Of the 103, 53 (51%)
respondents in the high category did not report any delinquency
involvement. However, 50 (49%) respondents in the same category
did report delinquency involvement. This comparison means that of
the subjects with a high attachment to their schools, a much higher
percentage of those in Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in
delinquency (49% vs. 41%).
For the medium category, of Hirschi's 1223 subjects, 619 were in
the medium category. Of the 619, 353 (57%) respondents were placed
in the medium category and they did not report any delinquency acts.
However, 266 (43%) respondents in the same category did report one or
more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 27 were in the
medium category. Of the 27, 9 (33%) in the medium category did not
report any delinquency involvement, while 18 (67%) did report one or
more delinquent acts. As with the high category, Table 4 shows that
of the subjects with a medium attachment to their school, a much
higher percentage of those in Irfaifeh's study reported being in-
volved in delinquency (67% vs. 43%).
Finally, with respect to the low category, of Hirschi's 1227
subjects, 94 were in the low category. Of the 94, 34 (36%) respond-
ents who were placed in the low category did not report any delin-
quency involvement, while 60 (64%) respondents in the same category
did report one or more delinquency acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects,
17 were in the low category. Of the 17, one (6%) subject was in this
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
105
category and did not report any delinquent acts. However, 16 (94%)
respondents who were in the low category did report one or more
delinquent acts. This comparison shows that of the subjects with a
low attachment to their schools, a much higher percentage of those in
Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency (94% vs. 64%).
In sum, according to Irfaifeh's findings, the results are consistent
with the theory, 49% of the respondents who were highly attached to
their school reported delinquent acts, while 94% of the respondents
who were weakly attached to their schools reported delinquency involve-
ment.
Another item to assess the bond to school was examined with
the question: "In general, do you like or dislike school?" (Like/
neither like nor dislike it/dislike it). These responses were
categorized as: High= "like it"; Medium= "neither like nor dislike
school"; and Low= "dislike it." Table 5 illustrates the comparison
of the two studies.
Table 5
Self-Reported Delinquent Acts by Attitudes Toward School:
"In General, do you Like or Dislike School?"
Self-Reported Acts High Medium Low
Nondelinquents HM 68%* HM 48%* HM 33%*
IM 58% IM 25% IM 11%
Delinquents HM 32%* HM 52%* HM 67%*
IM 42% IM 75% IM 89%
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
106
Table 5--Continued
Self-Reported Acts High Medium Low
Total HM* (580) (648) (72)
Total IM ( 85) ( 53) ( 9)
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 33,
(1969, p. 121).
Of Hirschi's 1300 subjects, 580 were in the high category. Of the
580, 394 (68%) of respondents who reported liking school did not report
any delinquent acts, while 186 (32%) respondents from the same category
did report delinquent acts. For Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 85 were in
the high category. Of the 85, 49 (58%) respondents who were in the
same category (high attachment to school) did not report any delinquent
acts. However, 36 (42%) respondents who were in the same category did
report delinquent acts. This comparison shows that of the subjects
with a high attachment to their schools, a much higher percentage of
those in Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency
(42% vs. 32%).
With regard to the medium category, of Hirschi's 1300 subjects,
648 were in the medium category. Of the 648, 311 (48%) respondents
did not report any delinquent acts. However, 337 (52%) respondents
in the same category did report delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147
subjects, 53 were in the medium category. Of the 53, 13 (25%)
respondents in the medium category did not report any delinquent acts,
while 40 (75%) respondents from the same category did report delin-
quent acts. The comparison shows that of those moderately attached
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
107
to their school, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects
reported being involved in delinquent acts (75% vs. 52%).
Finally, with respect to those placed in the low category, of
Hirschi's 1300 subjects, 72 were placed in the low category. Of the
72, 24 (33%) respondents did not report any delinquent acts. However,
48 (67%) respondents did report one or more delinquent acts. Of
Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 9 students were in the low category. Of the
9, one student (11%) did not report any delinquent act, while 8 (89%)
respondents did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison
shows that for those subjects who had low attachment to their school,
a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being in-
volved in delinquency (89% vs. 67%). To sum up this table, Irfaifeh's
findings were consistent with Hirschi's theory, that the higher the
attachment to a conventional figure (school) the less likely the
delinquency, and the lower the attachment, the more likely the
delinquency.
Further testing was conducted to assess the bond to the con-
ventional figure (school). Respondents were asked the following
question: "Do you care what teachers think of you?" (A lot/somewhat/
not much), or high, medium and low. Table 6 illustrates the compar-
ison of the two studies.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
108
Table 6
Self-Reported Delinquent Acts by Concern for Teacher's Opinions:
"Do You Care What Teachers Think of You?"
Self-Reported Acts High Medium Low
Nondelinquents HM 66%* HM 53%* HM 36%*
IM 85% IM 43% IM 34%
Delinquents HM 34%* HM 47%* HM 64%*
IM 15% IM 57% IM 66%
Total HM* (588) (503) (209) 1300
Total IM ( 21) ( 53) ( 73) 147
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 34,
(1969, p. 123).
Hirschi (1969) stated that "the less a boy cares about what
teachers think of him, the more likely he is to have committed
delinquent acts" (p. 123). Of Hirschi's 1300 subjects, 588 were in
the high category. Of the 588, 388 (66%) respondents who were placed
in the high category did not report any delinquent acts. However, 200
(34%) respondents in the same category did report one or more delin-
quent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 21 were in the high category.
Of the 21, 18 (85%) respondents who were in the high category did not
report any delinquent acts, while 3 (15%) respondents from the same
category did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison shows
that for those who were highly attached (care a lot) about their
teacher's opinions, a much higher percentage of Hirschi's subjects
were involved in delinquency (34% vs. 15%).
For the second category (medium), of Hirschi's 1300 subjects,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
109
503 were in the medium category. Of the 503, 267 (53%) respondents
who were placed in this category did not report any delinquent acts.
However, 236 (47%) respondents from the same categocy did report one
or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 53 were in the
medium category. Of the 53, 23 (43%) respondents in the medium cate-
gory did not report any delinquent acts, while 30 (57%) respondents
from the same category did report one or more delinquent acts. The
comparison shows that of the subjects with medium atta.chment to their
teachers, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported
being involved in delinquency (57% vs. 47%).
Finally, regarding the subjects in the low category: of Hir-
schi's 1300 subjects, 209 were in the low category; of the 209, 75
(36%) respondents who were placed in the low category did not report
any delinquent acts; however, 134 (64%) respondents did report one or
more delinquent acts. For Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 73 were in the low
category. Of the 73, 25 (34%) respondents who were placed in the low
category did not report any delinquent acts, while 48 (66%) respond-
ents from the same category did report one or more delinquent acts.
The comparison shows that of the subjects with a low attachment to
their teachers, almost the same percentage in both studies reported
being involved in delinquency (66% vs. 64%). In sum, Irfaifeh's
findings supported Hirschi's theory, that the higher the attachment
to the conventional figure (teacher), the less likely the delinquency
involvement.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
110
Attachment to Peers
Hirschi stated that delinquent behavior of one's own friends is
strongly related to one's own delinquent behavior. Respondents were
asked the following question: "Have any of your close friends ever
been picked up by the police?" The six possible responses were:
"No," "one," "two," "three," "four or more," and "I don't know."
These were collapsed into three categories: None, 1-3 Friends, and
4 or more Friends. The "I don't know" response was excluded from
the comparison. Table 7 illustrates the comparison.
Table 7
Self-Reported Delinquency by Friends, Contact With the Police:
"Have any of Your Close Friends Ever Been
Picked up by the Police?"
None 1-3 Friends 4-More Friends
Nondelinquents HM 73%* HM 45%* HM 25%*
IM 0% IM 8% IM 33%
Delinquents HM 27%* HM 55%* HM 75%*
IM 100% IM 92% IM 67%
Total HM* (520) (323) (208)
Total IM ( 2) ( 52) ( 93)
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 24
(1969, p. 99).
Of Hirschi'G 1051 subjects, 52 were 520 were in the high category.
Of the 520, 380 (73%) respondents who did not have any close friends
picked up by the police did not report any delinquent acts. However,
140 (27%) of subjects who were in the same category did report one
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
111
or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, no one reported
delinquent acts among those who had no close friends picked up by
the police, while only two students (100%) who did not have any
friends picked up by the police reported one or more delinquent acts.
The comparison of the two studies shows that of the subjects with no
friends picked up by the police, a much higher percentage of those in
Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency (100% vs. 27%).
With respect to the second category (1-3 friends), of Hirschi's
1051 subjects, 323 were in the medium category. Of the 323, 145
(45%) respondents who had one to three friends who had had contact
with the police did not report any delinquent acts. However 178 (55%)
respondents who had had one to three friends picked up by the police
reported one or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 52
were in the medium category. Of the 52, 4 (8%) of respondents who had
had one to three friends picked up by the police did report one or more
delinquent acts, while 48 (92%) respondents who had had one to three
friends picked up by the police reported one or more delinquent acts.
The comparison shows that of the subjects who had one to three friends
picked up by the police, a much higher percentage of those in Irfaifeh's
study reported being involved in delinquency (92% vs. 55%).
Finally, for those who had four or more friends picked up by the
police, of Hirschi's 1051 subjects, 208 were in the third category.
Of the 208, 52 (25%) respondents who were in this category did not
report any delinquent acts. However, 156 (75%) respondents in this
category (four or more friends) did report one or more delinquent
acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 83 were in the third category. Of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
112
the 83, 31 (33%) of respondents who were placed in this category did
not report any delinquent acts, while 52 (67%) respondents did report
one or more delinquent acts. In sum, the findings of Irfaifeh's study
did not support Hirschi's theory on this item.
Hirschi went further to suggest that the greater the attachment
to friends, the lower the reported delinquent involvement. Table 8
illustrates the comparison for the two studies.
Table 8
Self-Reported Delinquency by Identification With Best Friends:
"Would you like to be the Kind of Person
Your Best Friends Are?"
Self-Reported Delinquency In Most Ways In Few Ways Not At All
Nondelinquents HM 64%* HM 54%* HM 47%*
IM 14% IM 27% IM 25%
Delinquents HM 36%* HM 46%* HM 53%*
IM 86% IM 73% IM 75%
Total HM* (353) (748) (160)
Total IM ( 28) ( 34) ( 85)
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 44
(1969, p. 146).
Of Hirschi's 1261 subjects, 353 were in the first category. Of
the 353, 226 (64%) respondents who reported "in most ways" in their
attachment to their best friends did not report any delinquent acts.
However 127 (36%) respondents, who were placed in this category did
report one or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 28
were in the first category. Of the 23, 4 (14%) respondents who
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
113
reported "in most ways" with respect to their identification with
their best friends did not report any delinquent acts, while 24
(86%) respondents in the same category did report one or more delin-
quent acts. The comparison shows that of the subjects who identified
with their best friends in most ways, a much higher percentage of
those in Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency (86%
vs. 36%).
With respect to those in the second category, "in a few ways," of
Hirschi's 1261 subjects, 748 were in the second category. Of the 748,
404 (54%) respondents did not report any delinquent acts. However,
345 (46%) of respondents in this category did report one or more
delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 34 were in the second
category. Of the 34, 10 (27%) respondents who identified with their
best friends in a few ways did not report any delinquent acts, while
24 (73%) respondents in the same category did report one or more
delinquent acts. Again, the comparison of the two studies with re-
spect to this category shows that of the subjects in Irfaifeh's study,
a much higher percentage reported being involved in delinquency (73%
vs. 46%).
For those who did not identify themselves at all with their best
friends, of Hirschi's 1261 subjects, 160 were in the third category.
Of the 160, 75 (47%) respondents did not report any delinquent acts,
while 85 (53%) respondents in this category did report one or more
delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 85 were in the third
category. Of the 85, 21 (25%) of the respondents did not report any
delinquent acts. However, 64 (75%) of respondents who did not
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
114
identify themselves at all with their friends did report one or mvre
delinquent acts. Again, the comparison of the two studies with re-
spect to this issue shows a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's
subjects who reported being involved in delinquency (75% vs. 53%).
In sum, according to Table 8, Irfaifeh's findings did not support
Hirschi's theory on this item.
Commitment to Conventional Activities
Hirschi (1969) suggested that commitment to conventional activi-
ties such as doing well in school, achieving good grades, etc.,
constrain delinquency. On the other hand, involvement in adult
activities such as smoking, dating, drinking is related to delin-
quent behavior.
Involvement in adult activities was measured by many items such
as: "Do you smoke cigarettes?" There were four possible responses
to this question: (1) "Yes, began before 13"; (2) "Yes, began 13-
15"; (3) "Yes, began after 15 years old"; and (4) "No." For purposes
of comparison the four responses were collapsed into two responses:
"yes" =Responses 1, 2, and 3; and "no" =Response 4. Table 9 illus-
trates the comparison between the two studies.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
115
Table 9
Self-Reported Delinquency by Smoking:
"Do You Smoke?"
Self-Reported Acts Yes No
Nondelinquents HM 30%* HM 65%*
IM 26% IM 10%
Delinquents HM 70%* HM 35%*
IM 74% IM 90%
Total HM* (300) (952) 1251
Total IM (126) ( 21) 147
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his
Table 55 (1969, p. 167).
Of Hirschi's 1252 subjects, 300 were in the "yes" category. Of
the 300, 89 (30%) respondents who were placed in the "yes" category did
not report any delinquent acts. However, 211 (70%) respondents from
the same category did report one or more delinquent acts. For
Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 126 were in the "yes" category. Of the 126, 33
(26%) from the "yes" category did not report any delinquent acts, while
93 (74%) of respondents who were in the same category did report one or
more delinquent acts. The comparison shows that of those subjects who
reported smoking, the percentages of the two studies were very close
(70% for Hirschi's study and 74% for Irfaifeh's study) with regard to
reported delinquent acts.
With respect to the second category, non-smokers, of Hirschi's
1252 subjects, 952 were in the "no" category. Of the 952, 619 (65%)
respondents who did not smoke reported no delinquency involvement,
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
116
while 333 (35%) respondents from the same category did report one or
more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 21 were in the "no"
category. Of the 21, 2 (10%) respondents of the non-smokers did not
report any delinquent acts. However, 19 (90%) of respondents in this
category did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison
shows that of those subjects who did not smoke, a much higher per-
centage of Irfaifeh's study reported being involved in delinquency.
Thus, Hirschi's theory with respect to this type of adult activity
(smoking) was not supported.
With respect to alcohol consumption and dating as forms of adult
activities, of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 2 (40%) respondents who never
consumed alcohol did not report any delinquent acts, while 3 (60%)
respondents from the same category did report one or more delinquent
acts.
With respect to the second category, ("yes"), 33 (24%) respondents
who had had alcohol in the last academic year did not report any
delinquent acts. However, 109 (76%) respondents from the same cate-
gory did report one or more delinquent acts. Therefore, drinking was
related to delinquency involvement. Hirschi did not give figures
about drinking, but he suggested that drinking was more strongly re-
lated to delinquency than smoking, and also dating was strongly re-
lated to delinquency as well. However, as we have seen in Table 7,
smoking among Irfaifeh's subjects was not related to delinquency
involvement in contrast to Hirschi's theory, but drinking was found to
be strongly related.
The final item of adult activities to be tested here was dating.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
117
The possible responses were "yes" or "no." Of Irfaifeh's 147 sub-
jects, 7 were in the "no" category. Of the 7, one (14%) respondent
who never dated in the last academic year did not report any delin-
quent acts. However 6 (86%) respondents who were placed in the same
category of "no" did report one or more delinquent acts. With respect
to the second category of "yes", of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 140 were
in the "yes" category. Of the 140, 34 (24%) respondents did not re-
port any delinquent acts, while 106 (76%) respondents from the same
category did report one or more delinquent acts. Therefore, dating
was not found to be strongly related to delinquency in the sense that
86% of those who never dated reported being involved in delinquency
vs. 76% of respondents who were dating and reported being involved in
delinquency. Thus, with respect to dating, Hirschi's theory was not
supported. Hirschi did not give figures about dating and delinquency
in his study.
Commitment to conventional activities such as the importance of
getting good grades and time devoted to do homework were also related
to delinquency as Hirschi suggested. In order to measure commitment
to conventional activities, the respondents were asked the following
question: "How important is getting good grades to you personally?"
There were four possible responses to this question: (1) "very
important," (2) "somewhat important," (3) fairly important," and (4)
"completely unimportant." These four responses \'lere collapsed into
three categories for comparison purposes. High= (1); Medium= (2)
& (3); and Low= (4). Table 10 illustrates the comparison of the two
studies.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
118
Table 10
Self-Reported Delinquency by Perceived Importance of Good Grades:
"How Important is Getting Good Grades to You Personally?"
Self-Reported Acts High Medium Low
Nondelinquents HM 64%* HM 50%* HM 21%*
IM 0% IM ?% IM 32%
Delinquents HM 36%* HM 50%* HM 79%*
IM 100% IM 91% IM 68%
Total HM* (674) (585) (38) 1297
Total IM ( 4) ( 45) (98) 147
*The results of Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 94
(1969, p. 224).
According to Table 10, of Hirschi's 1297 subjects, 674 were in
the high category. Of the 674, 432 (64%) respondents who were placed
in the high category did not report any delinquent acts. However,
243 (36%) respondents from the same category did report one or more
delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 subjects, 4 were in the high
category. Of the 4, no one was found in the high category and
therefore, no delinquency involvement was reported, while 4 (100%)
respondents who were placed in the high category did report one or
more delinquent acts. With respect to the medium category, of
Hirschi's 1297 subjects, 585 were in the medium category. Of the
585, 293 (50%) respondents did not report any delinquent acts, while
the other half did report one or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's
147 subjects, 45 were in the medium category. Of the 45, 4 (9%)
respondents did not report any delinquent acts, however, 41 (91%)
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
119
respondents who were found in the same category, medium, did report
one or more delinquent acts. Finally, in the low category of Hirschi's
1297 subjects, 38 were in the low category. Of the 38, 8 (21%) re-
spondents did not report any delinquent acts, while 30 (79%) respond-
ents did report one or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 147 sub-
jects, 98 were in the low category. Of the 98, 32 (32%) respondents
of those in the low category did not report any delinquent acts. How-
ever, 66 (68%) respondents from the same category did report one or
more delinquent acts. The comparison shows that of those who were
weakly committed to achieving good grades, a higher percentage of
Hirschi's subjects reported being involved in delinquency. However,
Irfaifeh's findings were not consistent with Hirschi's theory with
respect to this item.
Involvement in Conventional Activities
Time devoted to doing homework as a measure of both commitment
and involvement in conventional activities was measured by the number
of hours per day of involvement from one and one-half hours or more,
one hour, one-half hour, and less than one-half hour. Table 11
illustrates the comparison of the two studies.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
120
Table 11
Percent Committing One or More Delinquent Acts
by Time Devoted to Homework
1-1/2 1 1/2 Less Than
Time Reported Hours Hour Hour 1/2 Hour
Delinquents HM 34%* HM 48%* HM 52%* HM 64%*
IM 66% IM 71% IM 80% IM 80%
Total HM* (593) (361) (199) (117) 1270
Total IM ( 6) ( 5) ( 41) ( 60) 112
*The figures for Hirschi's males were derived from his Table 71
(1969, p. 191).
According to Table 11, of Hirschi's 1270 subjects who reported
being involved in delinquency 593 (34%) respondents did report one
or more delinquent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 112 subjects who reported
being involved in delinquency from the first category, one and one-
half hours or more, 6 (66%) respondents did report one or more delin-
quent acts. The comparison shows that of the subjects who devoted
one and one-half hours or more a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's
subjects reported being involved in delinquency (66% vs. 34%).
With respect to the second category, those who devoted one hour
to do their homework, of Hirschi's 1270 subjects who reported being
involved in delinquency, 361 (48%) respondents devoted one hour only.
Of Irfaifeh's 112 subjects, 5 (71%) respondents of those from the same
category did report one or more delinquent acts. Again, comparing
the two studies, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects re-
ported being involved in delinquency (71% vs. 48%).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
121
For the third category containing those subjects who spent one-
half hour and reported being involved in delinquency, of Hirschi's
1270 subjects, 199 (52%) respondents did report one or more delin-
quent acts. Of Irfaifeh's 112 subjects who reported being involved
in delinquency, 41 (80%) respondents from the same category did report
one or more delinquent acts. Again, comparing the two studies with
respect to this category, one-half hour, a much higher percentage of
Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved in delinquency (80% vs.
52%). Finally, regarding those subjects who devoted less than one-
half hour to doing homework, and who reported being involved in de-
linquency, of Hirschi's 1270 subjects, 60 (64%) respondents did report
delinquent acts. With respect to Irfaifeh's subjects 60 (80%)
respondents being placed in this category did report one or more
delinquent acts. The comparison shows that in all four categories, a
much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved
in delinquency. Moreover, no big differences were found among the four
categories with respect to time devoted to homework and delinquency.
Thus, Hirschi's theory was not supported by Irfaifeh's study on this
item.
Besides testing the relationship between academic involvement
and reported delinquency behavior, another non-academic item of
school-related activities was tested in Irfaifeh's sample only. The
following question was used to assess the bond between non-academic
school activities and delinquency behavior.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
122
Table 12
Self-Reported Delinquency by Involvement in School-Related Activities:
"Are You Active in School Activities?"
Self-Reported Very Somewhat Not Active
Delinquency Active Active At All I Don't Know
Nondelinquents 6 (15%) 23 (29%) 4 (27%) 2 (13%)
Delinquents 33 (85%) 50 (71%) 11 (73%) 11 (73%)
Total 39 73 15 13
According to Table 12, 39 were in the very active category. Of
the 39, 6 (15%) respondents who reported being very active in school-
related activities did not report any delinquent behavior. However,
33 (85%) respondents from the same category did report one or more
delinquent acts. Comparing the two groups (85% vs. 15%) of nondelin-
quents vs. delinquents the data was not supportive of Hirschi's
theory. With respect to the second category the somewhat active
group, 73 were in the somewhat active category. Of the 73, 23 (29%)
respondents did not report any delinquent behavior, however, 50 (71%)
respondents did report one or more delinquent acts. With respect to
the third group of not active at all, 15 were in the not active at
all category. Of the 15, 4 (27%) respondents did not report any
delinquent acts, compared to 11 (73%) of respondents who did report
one or more delinquent acts. Finally, of the "I don't know" category,
13 were in that category. Of the 13, 2 (13%) respondents did not re-
port any delinquent acts, while 11 (73%) of respondents did report one
or more delinquent acts. The overall comparison among the four
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
123
categories did not support Hirschi's theory, as illustrated in the
table where 85% of respondents who reported being very active com-
mitted one or more delinquent acts versus 73% of respondents who
reported being not active at all repcrted one or more delinquency
acts. However, seven students did not respond to this question.
Belief
Hirschi (1969) claimed that "delinquency behavior does not re-
sult from beliefs which require delinquency but instead that delin-
quency is made possible by the absence of beliefs that forbid delin-
quency" (p. 198). Hirschi used many items to assess the relationship
between belief in the conventional order and involvement in delin-
quent activities. These items were: respect for police, attitudes
toward the law, belief in individual responsibility, helplessness to
avoid trouble, denial of injury, denial of the victim, and finally,
condemnation of the condemner.
The possible responses for these items were the following:
"strongly agree," "agree," "undecided," "disagree," and "strongly
disagree." For comparison purposes the five responses were collapsed
into three responses: (1) "agree and "strongly agree," (2) "undecided,"
and (3) "disagree" and "strongly disagree." Table 13 illustrates the
comparison of the two studies.
According to Table 13, of Hirschi's subjects who agreed with the
statement "I have a lot of respect for the police," 769 (35%)
respondents did report one or more delinquent acts. \~ile in Irfaifeh's
study, 25 (83%) respondents did report one or more delinquent acts.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
124
Table 13
Percent Committing One or More Delinquent Acts
By Hirschi's Belief Items
Belief Agree N** Undecided N** Disagree N**
1. I have a lot of respect HM 35%* 769 54%* 325 63%* 187
for the Richmond police. IM 83% 25 88% 22 71% 65
2 0 It is alright to get
around the law if you HM 69%* 142 54%* 219 73%* 919
can get away with it. IM 84% 16 82% 18 74% 78
3 Most criminals shouldn't
0
be blamed for the things HM 51%* 154 48%* 177 41%* 952
they have done. IM 74% 63 92% 11 76% 38
4. I can't seem to stay out
of trouble no matter how HM 66%* 150 49%* 176 38%* 842
hard I try. IM 76% 78 78% 32 50% 2
5. Most things people call
delinquency don't really HM 59%* 310 49%* 432 36%* 540
hurt anyone. IM 73% 19 81% 22 75% 71
6. The man who leaves his
keys in his car is as
much to blame for its
theft as the man who HM 43%* 852 49%* 131 43%* 313
steals it. IM 78% 78 60% 14 83% 20
7 0 Policemen try to give HM 50%* 720 56%* 255 79%* 311
all kids an even break. IM 100% 57 91% 36 68% 19
*The results for Hirschi's males were derived from his Tables 78, 80, 82, 83, 84,
85, 86 (1969, pp. 201-211).
**Those who committed delinquent acts.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
125
The comparison shows that a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's
subjects reported being involved in delinquency (83% vs. 35%). For
those in the second category of undecided, of Hirschi's subjects, 325
(54%) respondents did report one or more delinquent acts compared to
22 (88%) respondents in Irfaifeh's study. Again, the comparison shows
a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects who reported being
involved in delinquency (88% vs. 54%).
Finally, of Hirschi's subjects, 187 (63%) respondents who either
disagree or disagree strongly committed one or more delinquent acts.
Of Irfaifeh's subjects, 22 (71%) respondents in the same category did
report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison shows a much
higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved in
delinquency. In sum, according to Irfaifeh's findings, Hirschi's
theory was not supported in the sense that we expected those in the
first category to have less delinquent acts reported versus the last
category. With respect to the second item, attitudes toward the iaw,
of Hirschi's subjects, 142 (69%) respondents who agreed with the
statement did report one or more delinquent acts, compared with
Irfaifeh's subjects 16 (84%) respondents from the same category did
report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison shows that of those
who agreed with the statement "it is all right to get around the law if
you can get away with it," a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's
subjects reported being involved in delinquency (84% vs. 69%). With
respect to those subjects in the undecided category, of Hirschi's
subjects, 219 (54%) respondents reported one or more delinquent acts,
compared to 18 (82%) respondents in Irfaifeh's study. Again, the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
126
comparison shows a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects who
reported being involved in delinquency, (82% vs. 54%).
With respect to the final category, of Hirschi's subjects who
disagreed with the above statement, 919 (73%) respondents reported
one or more delinquent acts compared to Irfaifeh's subjects, 78 (74%)
respondents did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison
shows that Hirschi's theory was not supported by his study nor by
the present one.
The third item to assess the relationship between belief and
involvement in delinquency was: "Most criminals should not be blam-
ed for the things they have done." Of Hirschi's subjects, 154 (51%)
respondents who agreed with the statement did report one or more
delinquent acts. The comparison shows of those subjects who agreed
with the above statement, a much higher percentage of lrfaifeh's study
reported being involved in delinquency (74% vs. 51%).
With respect to the second category of undecided, 177 (48%)
respondents in Hirschi's study reported one or more delinquent acts.
In lrfaifeh's study, 11 (92%) of respondents in the same category did
report one or more delinquent acts. Again, the comparison shows a much
higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved in
delinquency (92% vs. 48%).
The third category, those subjects who disagreed with the above
statement, 952 (41%) respondents of Hirschi's subjects reported one
or more delinquent acts versus 38 (76%) respondents in Irfaifeh's study.
The comparison shows that for those subjects who disagreed with the
statement, a much higher percentage of lrfaifeh's subjects reported
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
127
being involved in delinquency. In sum, with respect to the third
item, attitudes toward the law, Hirschi's theory was not supported
by Irfaifeh's study.
The fourth item, "I can't seem to stay out of trouble no matter
how hard I try," of Hirschi's subjects 150 (66%) who agreed with the
above statement reported one or more delinquent acts versus 78 (76%)
respondents in Irfaifeh's study who agreed with the statement, andre-
ported one or more delinquent acts. The comparison of the two studies
shows that of those subjects who agreed with the above statement, a
much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved
in delinquency (76% vs. 66%).
With respect to the second category, undecided, Hirschi's 176
(49%) respondents who were undecided reported one or more delinquent
acts. For Irfaifeh's subjects, 32 (78%) respondents in the same
category did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison shows
that of those subjects who were undecided about the statement, a much
higher per~entage of Irfaifeh's subjects did report one or more
delinquent acts (78% vs. 48%).
The final category, those subjects who disagreed with the state-
ments, of Hirschi's subjects, 842 (38%) respondents did report one or
more delinquent acts. In Irfaifeh's study 2 (SO%) respondents from the
same category did report one or more delinquent acts. The comparison
shows of those subjects who disagreed with the fourth statement, a
much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved
in delinquency. However, Irfaifeh's findings did support Hirschi's
study with respect to the fourth item.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
128
The fifth item to assess the relationship between the bond and
involvement in delinquency was, most criminals shouldn't really be
blamed for the things they have done. Of Hirschi's subjects, 310
(59%) respondents who agreed with the statement did report one or
more delinquent acts. With respect to Irfaifeh's findings, 19 (73%)
respondents who were in this category reported being involved in de-
linquency. The comparison shows that of those subjects who agreed
with the statement, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's study did
report one or more delinquent acts (73% vs. 59%).
For the undecided category, 432 (49%) respondents in Hirschi's
study reported one or more delinquent acts, compared to 22 (81%)
respondents in Irfaifeh's study. Therefore, a much higher percentage
of Irfaifeh's subjects who were undecided about the statement reported
being involved in delinquent behavior (81% vs. 49%).
Finally, with respect to the subjects who disagreed with the
above statements, 540 (36%) respondents of Hirschi's study did report
one or more delinquent acts. Compared to Irfaifeh's subjects, 71 (75%)
respondents who were placed in the same category did report one or
more delinquent acts. The comparison shows that of those subjects
who disagreed with the above statements, a much higher percentage of
Irfaifeh's subjects reported being involved in delinquency (75% vs.
36%). Thus, with respect to this item, no support was found for
Hirschi's theory in Irfaifeh's study.
The sixth item, the man who leaves his keys in his car is as
much to blame for its theft as the man who steals it, of Hirschi's
respondents 852 (43%) who agreed with the statement did report one or
------------------------------ --
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
129
more delinquent acts. For Irfaifeh's subjects, 78 (78%) of respond-
ents who did agree with the statement reported one or more delinquent
acts. The comparison shows that of those subjects who agreed with
the above statements, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects
reported being involved in delinquency (78% vs. 43%).
For the undecided category, 131 (49%) respondents in Hirschi's
study reported one or more delinquent acts. Compared to Irfaifeh's
findings, 14 (60%) respondents did report one or more delinquent
acts. Again, the comparison shows that of those subjects who were
undecided, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported
being involved in delinquency.
Finally, those subjects who disagreed with the above statement,
313 (43%) respondents in Hirschi's study did report one or more de-
linquent acts, compared to 20 (83%) of respondents in Irfaifeh's
study. The comparison shows that of those subjects who disagreed
with the above statement, a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's
subjects reported being involved in delinquency. In sum, with re-
spect to this item, Hirschi's theory was supported in the sense that
78% of respondents who agreed with the statement did report one or
more delinquent acts versus 83% of respondents who disagreed with
the statement and did report one or more delinquent acts.
Finally, the last item to assess the relationship between the
bond and involvement in delinquency was that policemen try to give
all kids an even break. Of Hirschi's subjects, 720 (SO%) of re-
spondents who agreed with the statement did report one or more de-
linquent acts. In Irfaifeh's study, 57 (100%) respondents who agreed
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
130
with the statement reported one or more delinquent acts. Therefore,
a much higher percentage of Irfaifeh's subjects reported being in-
volved in delinquency.
With respect to the second category of undecided, 255 (56%)
respondents in Hirschi's study did report one or more delinquent acts
versus 36 (91%) of respondents in Irfaifeh's study. Again, the per-
centage of those subjects in Irfaifeh's study was much higher than
Hirschi's study.
The last category consisting of those subjects who disagree with
the statement, 311 (79%) respondents of Hirschi's subjects did report
one or more delinquent acts. For Irfaifeh's study, 19 (68%) respondents
who were placed in the same category reported one or more delinquent
acts. The comparison shows that of those who disagreed with the
above statement, a much higher percentage of Hirschi's subjects re-
ported being involved in delinquency. However, Hirschi's theory was
not supported with respect to this item.
In sum, two items (4, 6) were found to be supportive of Hir-
schi's theory, while the rest of the items did not support the theory
at all.
Summary
The aim of this section was to determine the degree to which the
univariate relationship found and reported in Hirschi's study hold
for the Jordanian society. It was clear from the overall correspon-
dence between Hirschi's results and the results in this research,
that there was an inconsistency between the two studies with some
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
131
minor exceptions.
Attachment to father was supported and consistent with Hirschi's
findings and even further, attachment to father was found to be
stronger and more effective in deterring delinquent behavior than
attachment to mother. In fact, attachment to mother was not consis-
tent at all. Attachment to school as measured by "how do you rate
yourself in school ability compared with other students in your
school" and "in general do you like or dislike school" were related
to delinquent involvement.
Attachment to peers as measured by friends' contact with the po-
lice was not supported and attachment to peers measured by identifi-
cation with friends was inconsistent with the theory. Therefore,
attachment to significant others, with three exceptions "attachment to
fathers, teachers, and schools" were found inconsistent to Hirschi's
findings.
Commitment to conventional activities as measured by the impor-
tance of getting good grades was inconsistent with Hirschi's theory.
Concerning commitment to adult activities--smoking, drinking and dating,
the research results with respect to smoking and dating were not
consistent with Hirschi's theory, while drinking was found related to
delinquent involvement.
With respect to involvement in conventional activities and
delinquency behavior as measured by time devoted to homework, the
research findings were inconsistent and did not support Hirschi's
theory. Regarding involvement in non-academic, school-related
activities, the research findings were also inconsistent with Hirschi's
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
132
theory.
Looking at the element belief, the following aspects were found
to be inconsistent with Hirschi's results: (a) respect for police,
(b) attitudes toward the denial of responsibility, (c) denial of in-
jury, and (d) equitability of police treatment. Helplessness to avoid
trouble, and denial of injury, were the only two items were consistent
with the theory.
Further analysis was conducted to test the four hypotheses of
this research. The results are reported in the next section.
Test of the Research Hypotheses
In this section test of the research hypotheses were conducted
using the t-test. The statistical analyses were computed in testing
the nondirectional hypotheses using .05 critical value for committing
a type 1 error (alpha). However, before we proceed to talk about the
research hypotheses, the writer believes that the reader should be
acquainted with the indices used to test the hypotheses.
Each independent variable was measured by several items, as men-
tioned in the third chapter. Thus, attachment to mother was measured
by a seven-item index, and "an index is a variable which is a compo-
site of other variables that are assumed to reflect some underlying
construct" (Bohrnstedt & Kroke, 1982, p. 359). In this case the
attachment to mother index is the sum of each individual's responses
to seven items. The seven items deal with intimacy of communication
between mother and child, identifications with mother, and supervi-
sor. Each item in this index has a score which ranges from 1 to 3.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
133
Thus, each individual total scores ranges from 7-21. Attachment to
father was treated in the same fashion.
With respect to attachment to peers, a five-item index was
constructed as also mentioned in the third chapter. The five items
deal with identification with best friends, teachers, attitudes toward
one's friends, respect best friends, and finally friends' contact with
police. Each item has a score which ranges from 1 to 3. The total
scores of each individual score ranges from 5-15.
Attachment to school was measured by a seven-item index. The
seven items deal with attitudes toward school, teachers, self-
scholastic ability, self-rating in school ability, school-generated
emotional tension. Each item has three responses which range from 1 to
3. Consequently, each individual total scores ranges from 7-21.
Commitment to conventional activities was measured by a seven-
item index. Each item has these responses: high to low. The seven
items deal with educational aspiration, educational expectation, and
GPA. The range of each individual's total scores ranges from 7-21.
Involvement in conventional activities was measured by an eight-
item index. Each item has three responses (high to low). The eight
items deal with time devoted to homework, religiosity, housework, and
attitudes toward school. The index score ranges from 8 to 24 for each
individual's total scores.
Finally, with respect to belief, an eight-item index was used to
measure this variable. The eight items deal with attitudes toward
police, law, and individual's responsibility; each item has three
responses from high (agree) to low (disagree). Thus, the total
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
134
individual's scores range from 8 to 24.
The following hypotheses were tested:
1. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to significant others.
2. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to commitment to conventional activities.
3. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to involvement in the conventional order.
4. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to belief in the conventional order.
With respect to the first hypothesis, attachment to significant
others, four subhypotheses were created: attachment to mother,
father, school and peers. The reader should notice in the following
pages that the writer will start an overall test of the whole index,
then an individual test will be conducted for each item in the four
indices.
Hypothesis 1
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to significant others.
The data presented in Table 14 did support the relationship
stated in Hl. According to the data in Table 14, the mean scores of
nondelinquents on the attachment's index was slightly higher than the
total scores of delinquents, 57.6 vs. 56.7, respectively.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
135
Table 14
Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and Nondelinquents
on Attachment to Significant Others Index
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Mean SD N Mean SD N df t p
57.6 6.1 35 56.7 5.3 109 142 .84 .4
The mean differences between the two groups (delinquents and non-
delinquents) of self-reported behavior on the attachment index was not
significant at alpha level .05 (t = .8; p = .4). Therefore, the null
hypothesis was accepted, and the alternative hypothesis had to be
rejected.
Individual Tests
According to Table 15, the total scores of the nondelinquents
and delinquents on all attachment indices (mother, father, school,
and peers) with the exception of attachment to father index was high-
er than the mean scores of delinquents on each of the attachment
indices.
Table 15
T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on Attachment Indices
Attachment
To Signifi- Nondelinquents Delinquents
cant Others N Mean SD N Mean SD df t p
Father 35 13.9 3.07 112 14.4 3.07 145 - .89 .37
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
136
Table 15--Continued
Attachment
To Signifi- Nondelinquents Delinquents
cant Others N Mean SD N Mean SD df t: p
Mother 35 14.6 2.90 112 14.3 2.5 145 .63 .5
School 35 16.5 2.40 112 15.9 2.5 145 1.26 .2
Peers 35 12.5 2.11 112 12.1 1.9 145 1.14 .2
No significant differences were found at .05 level, between the two
groups. The mean score of nondelinquents on the attachment to father
index was 13.9, less than the mean score of nondelinquents on the
attachment to mother index. The mean score of delinquents on the
attachment to father index was slightly higher than the mean score of
delinquents on the attachment to mother index, 14.42 and 14.25,
respectively.
Regarding attachment to school index, nondelinquents had higher
mean score (16.5) than the delinquents (15.9), which indicates that
attachment to school might have had some effect on their behavior.
However, no significant differences were found between the two groups.
Finally, nondelinquents had a higher mean score (12.5) than the delin-
quents (11.11) on the attachment to peers index. Again, although the
differences between the two groups were not significant, even so,
attachment to peers for the nondelinquents was higher according to their
total scores on the index. In sum, no significant differences were
found between the two groups on the four attachment indices.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
137
Hypothesis la
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to mother in each item in
the mother's attachment index.
The data presented in the previous table (15) indicated no signifi-
cant differences between the two groups with respect to attachment
to mother index. Now we turn to each item on the attachment to mother
index in order to see if the mean score of the two groups are the
same or different.
As mentioned earlier, a seven-item index was constructed to mea-
sure attachment to mother, and each item ranged from low (1) to high
(3). The data presented in Table 16 indicated that the mean score
of nondelinquents was less than the mean score of delinquents on
items 1, 2, 5, 7, while they had the same mean score on the rest of
the items 3, 4, 6. The actual alpha for items 1, 2, 5 and 7 was .3,
.5, .4 and .3 respectively. Thus, attachment to mother had no sig-
nificant effect on the self-reported behavior of the two groups.
Consequently, the null hypothesis was accepted, and the alternative
hypothesis had to be rejected.
Hypothesis lb
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to father in each item
in the father attachment index.
It was revealed in Table 15 that no significant differences were
found between delinquents and nondelinquents mean score on the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
us
Table 16
T- test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and De 1 inquents
on Mother's Attachment Index (Item by Item)
Item #
Regarding Nondelin'quents Delinquents
Mother N Mean so N Mean so df p
1. 'Jnf-.ir
rules. 35 1.3 .63 112 1.5 .67 145 -1.10 ·. 3
2. Explanation
of rules. 35 1.7 .9 112 1.8 .B 145 - .66 .7
3. Knows where
you are. 35 2.4 .9 112 2.4 .7 145 - .32 .8
4. Know who you
are with. 35 2.3 .8 112 2.3 .8 145 .90 .4
5. Concern
about
schoolwork. 35 2.3 .B 112 2.5 .7 145 .20 .8
6. Share thoughts
and feelings
with mother. 35 1.8 .8 112 1.8 .8 145 .98 .3
7. Identifi-
cation with
mother. 35 2.5 .5 112 1.6 .5 145 1.11, .2
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
139
attachment to father index. However, there was a practical impor-
tance of the reported differences between the two groups. In other
words, there was a weak effect on the behavior of those who were
nondelinquents. Their mean scores were 13.9 and 14.4, respectively.
Table 17 shows what happened when each item was tested to find
out if there was any significant differences between the two groups
regarding the father attachment index.
According to the data presented in the above table, attachment
to father as measured by the seven items had no significant effect on
the reported delinquency or nondelinquency of the two groups. However,
three of the items (2, 5, 6) had the same mean score for both delin-
quents and nondelinquents (1.8, 2.4, 1.7). The mean scores, however,
for the nondelinquents on items 3 and 4 were slightly larger than the
scores of delinquent group (2.4 vs. 2.2; 2.3 vs. 2.2)., but of no
significant effect. Finally, the mean scores of the delinquents were
slightly larger than the nondelinquents on items 1 and 7 (1.6 vs. 1.5;
2.4 vs. 2.2, respectively).
In sum, all items were supportive of the null hypothesis of no
significant differences between the mean score of the two groups.
In other words, attachment to father had no effect on the self-
reported behavior of the two groups.
Hypothesis lc
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to school on each item
in the school attachment index.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
140
Table 17
T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
on FRther's Attachment Index (Item by Item)
Item #
Regarding Nondelinquents Delinquents
Father N Mean SD N Mean SD df t p
l. Unfair
rules. 35 1.5 . 74 112 1.5 . 70 145 .14 .9
2. Explanation
of rules. 35 1.8 .85 112 1.8 .77 145 .28 .8
3. Knows where
you are. 35 2.4 .77 112 2.2 .74 145 1.16 .24
4. Know who you
are with. 35 2.3 .76 112 2.2 . 79 145 1. 07 .3
5. Concern
about
schoolwork. 35 2.4 .77 112 2.4 .73 145 - .40 .7
6. Share thoughts
and feelings
with father. 35 1.7 . 70 112 1.7 .76 145 .01 .9
7. Identifi
-cation with
father. 35 2.2 .80 112 2.4 .75 145 -1.05 .3
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
141
The data presented in Table 15 indicated no significant differ-
ences between delinquents and nondelinquents mean score on the total
attachment to school index, 15.9 and 16.5, respectively. However,
each item in the seven-item index was tested in order to find out if
out if any of these items was significant.
According to Table 18, two items (1 and 2) were found to be
significant, .03 and .05, respectively. Thus, attitudes toward
school and perceived scholastic ability were effective with respect
to the self-reported behavior of nondelinquents and delinquents.
The mean score of nondelinquents were larger than the mean scores
of delinquents (2.7 vs. 2.5, 2.8 vs. 2.6 respectively). However,
attachment to school as measured by the other five items did not
indicate any significant differences between the two groups. Thus,
attachment to school as measured by grades, concern for teachers'
opinions, perception of teachers' treatment, and scopes of school
authority did not indicate any significant effect with respect to the
self-reported behaviors of the two groups.
In sum, the null hypothesis was accepted partially in the sense
that only two out of seven items supported the hypothesis, while five
of them did not give any significant differences. The reader might
be confused here about the reported result of this subhypothesis,
due to the fact that either we reject or accept the null hypothesis.
However, the purpose of testing these subhypotheses was to find out
the significance of each item. In other words, which item was more
effective than others on the reported-self behavior of the two groups.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
142
Table 18
T·test Comparison Between Nonde1inquents and Delinquents
on Attachment to School Index (Item by Item)
Nonde linquents Delinquents
Item# N Mean SD N Mean SD df t p
1. Like/dislike
school. 35 2.7 .52 112 2.5 .62 145 2.21 .03
2. Rate your·
self in
school. 35 2.8 '53 112 2.6 .64 145 1. 92 .05
3. Kinds of
grades. 35 2.4 .49 112 2.4 .49 145 .04 .9
4. Care what
teachers
think of
you. 35 2.05 .48 112 2.06 .47 145 . .06 .9
5. None of
school's
business. 35 2.02 .99 112 1.9 .88 145 .64 .5
6. Nervous
in school. 35 2.3 .85 112 2.1 .89 145 .76 .4
7. Teachers
pick on
me. 35 2.3 .82 112 2.4 .81 145 .92 .4
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
143
Hypothesis ld
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to peers on each item in
the school attachment index. According to the data presented in
Table 15, the overall test of attachment to significant others,
no significant differences were found between the two groups,
delinquents and nondelinquents.
As mentioned in the beginning of this chapter, a five-item index
was constructed to measure attachment to peers. According to Table
19, only one item, 3, was significant, while the other four items were
not significant at all. Thus, friends' contact with police was very
effective on the reported self-behavior of the two groups.
As reported in the above table, students' attitudes toward one's
friends, teachers' attitudes toward one's friends, identification and
respect for best friends did not have a significant effect on the
behavior of the subjects, while a number of friends picked up by the
police was significant at any alpha level. Thus, control theory
should reconsider the kinds of friends one has instead of assuming
the more attached the person is the less likely he will be delin-
quent. However, nondelinquents had a larger mean score on items 1,
3, and 4 and lower mean score on item 5, when compared to their
counterparts, and both groups had the same scores on item 2.
In sum, attachment to friends as measured by the five-item index,
was supportive of the null hypothesis partially, or with one excep-
tion, Item 3.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
14~
Table 19
r-eese Comparison Between Nondelinquencs and Delinquents
on Attachment co Peers Index (Item by Item)
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Item # N Mean so N Mean SD df t p
l. Students
like your
friends. 35 2.4 .88 112 2.3 .88 145 078 .4
2 0 Teachers
like your
friends. 35 2.5 .78 112 2.5 .81 145 .08 .9
3 0 Friends
picked up
by police. 35 2.9 032 112 2.5 .53 145 3.66 .000
4. Identifi-
cation with
friends. 35 2.5 070 112 2.4 .81 145 .84 .4
50 Respect best
friend's
opinions. 35 2.3 . 98 112 2.5 .83 145 .25 .4
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
145
Summary of First Hypothesis
The analysis of the data as presented in Tables 14 through 19
consisted of using the t-test to find out if there were any signifi-
cant differences between the delinquents and nondelinquents mean
scores of their reported self-behavior on the overall attachment in-
dex, and the individual items in the index.
The overall test of the first hypothesis was supportive, and
consequently, the null hypothesis was accepted, and the alternative
hypothesis was rejected. However, further testing was conducted on
index items (mother, father, school, and peers) to find out if there
were any significant differences between delinquents and non-
delinquents with respect to each item. The analysis revealed some
slight difference in the mean score of the two groups on most items
in the four indices. However, two items in the attachment to school
index were significant (items 1 and 2) and only item number 3 was
found to be significant on the attachment to peers index.
Hypothesis 2
There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to commitment to conventional activities.
As mentioned in the introduction to this chapter, commitment to
conventional activities was measured by a seven-item index. Accord-
ing to Hirschi's theory, delinquents are not aspirers. He argues that
aspiration to achieve conventional goals constrains delinquent beha-
vior. Thus, delinquents were expected to be significantly different
in their conventional commitment to education and occupation than
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
146
nondelinquents.
The overall test of the commitment index supported Hirschi's
stand that delinquents are significantly different from nondelin-
quents. According to Table 20, alpha was significant at .02.
Table 20
Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and Nondelinquents
With Respect to Commitment to Conventional Activities
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Mean SD N Mean SD N df t p
19.5 2.03 35 18.4 2.8 112 145 2.3 .02
The mean score of nondelinquents was larger than the mean score
of delinquents (19.5 vs. 18.4). Therefore, the null hypothesis was
not accepted and the alternative hypothesis is considered. Now we
turn to examine each item in the commitment index in order to see
which items were significant on the self-reported behavior of the two
groups.
According to the data presented in Table 21, items 2 and 6 were
significant at .002 and .005 level. With respect to item 2, impor-
tance of good grades, the mean score of nondelinquents was larger
than the mean score of delinquents (2.9 vs. 2.6), while for item 6,
school expectation, the mean score of nondelinquents was larger than
the mean score of delinquents (2.8 vs. 2.4). Thus, individuals who
were concerned about their grades and expected to finish from col-
lege scored lower on the delinquency index than those who were not
concerned.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
147
Table 21
T-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
With Respect to Commitment to Conventional Activities
(Item by Item)
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Item # N Mean so N Mean SD df t p
l. Kinds of
grades. 35 2.4 .50 112 2.4 .48 145 .04 .9
2. Importance
of good
grades. 35 2.9 .32 112 2.6 .56 145 3.21 .002
3. Importance
of good
grades. 35 2.6 .69 112 2.4 .83 145 1.45 .2
4. Job-money
is the only
reason. 35 2.9 1.4 112 3.07 1.4 145 .69 .5
5. Plan to
graduate
from high
school. 35 3.00 .16 112 2.9 .24 145 1. 52 .1
6. Schooling--
expect to
get. 35 2.8 .51 112 2.4 .92 145 2.8 .005
7. Schooling--
would like
to get. 35 3.00 .17 112 2.6 .76 145 2.8 .006
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
148
However, the rest of the items did not indicate any significant
differences between the two groups. In fact, for items 1 and 5 both
groups had almost the same mean score, which means that graduation
from high school and the kinds of grades had no significant effect
on the subjects behavior. It is interesting to notice that on item
number 7, although it was not significant, the mean score of non-
delinquents was larger than the mean score of delinquents. In sum,
two items (1 and 6) were found to be effective on the subjects'
behavior, while the rest of the items were not significant.
Summary of Second Hypothesis
The data presented in Tables 20 and 21, indicated that commit-
ment to conventional activities was significant in deterring delin-
quency. The null hypothesis was not accepted, and the alternative
hypothesis had to be accepted. The individual test of the items, as
presented in Table 21 indicated that importance of getting grades and
school expectations were significant, the other five items did not
indicate any significant differences between the two groups.
Hypothesis 3
There are no significant differences between delinquents and non-
delinquents with respect to involvement in conventional activities.
An eight-item index was constructed to measure involvement in
conventional activities. Hirschi (1969) argued that "analysis of
involvement in conventional activities parallels analysis of attitud-
inal commitments to conventional success goals. Such activities are
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
149
presumably in large part consequences of such commitment" (p. 191).
Table 22
Results of t-test Comparison Between Delinquents and Nondelinquents
With Respect to Involvement in Conventional Activities Index
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Mean SD N Mean SD N df t p
18.9 1.8 35 19.1 1.9 112 145 .9 .3
According to the data presented in Table 22, the overall test of
the null hypothesis indicated no significant differences between the
two groups (delinquents and nondelinquents). The mean score of
nondelinquents and delinquents was 18.9 vs. 19.1. Consequently, the
null hypothesis had to be accepted, and the alternative hypothesis was
rejected. We turn now to examine each item in the involvement to
conventional activities index.
The data presented in Table 23 indicated that no significant
differences were found between the two groups with respect to their mean
score. However, nondelinquents had larger mean scores on items 2, 3, 7
and 8, while both groups had the same mean score on items l, 4, 5 and 6.
Thus, involvement in conventional activities as measured by kinds of
grades, finish homework, time devoted to homework, attending religious
services, and working around the house had no significant effect on the
self-reported behavior of ~be two groups.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
150
Table 23
Results of t-test Comparison Bet~een Delinquents and Nondelinquents
With Respect to Involvement in Conventional Activities (Item by Item)
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Item ~> N Mean SD N Mean SD df p
1. Kinds of
grades. 35 2.4 .so 112 2.4 .48 145 .04 .9
2. Finish
homework. 35 2.4 . 54 112 2.3 .54 145 .81 .4
3. Teachers
check your
homework. 35 2.1 .47 112 2.04 .36 145 .92 .4
4. Time
devoted to
homework. 35 2.5 .60 112 2.5 .so 144 .64 .5
5. Suspension
from
school. 35 2.7 .61 112 2. 71 .57 145 .25 .81
6. Going to
religious
services. 35 2.9 .35 112 2.9 .24 145 -1.5 .13
7. Working
with mother
in the
home. 35 1.9 .80 112 2.14 .73 145 -1.77 .07
8. Working
with father
in the
home. 35 1.9 .76 112 2. 09 .74 145 -1.08 .3
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
151
Summary of Third Hypothesis
The data presented in Tables 22 and 23 indicated no significant
differences between delinquents and nondelinquents. Therefore, the
null hypothesis was accepted and the alternative hypothesis had to be
rejected. Furthermore, the individual test of the items did not
indicate any significant differences between the two groups. It was
expected that juveniles, who were invo~ved in conventional activities,
such as getting good grades, doing their homework, mosque-goers, and
helping their families around the house, had less of a chance to engage
in delinquent behavior. Hirschi suggested that involvement in conven-
tional activities is the consequence of commitment to educational
and occupational goals. However, the above findings suggested that
Hirschi's conception of involvement should be reconsidered as some
criminologists have already noted (Krohn & Massey, 1980).
Hypothesis 4
There are no significapt differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to belief in the conventional order.
Hirschi (1969, p. 203) argues that "belief in the moral validity
of the law is consistently related co the measures of attachment and
commitment discussed earlier: the child with little intimate commun-
ication with his parents, the child who does not like school, the
child who is unconcerned about the opinions of teachers, the child
who has little respect for the police, the child who feels little de-
sire for success in conventional terms, is unlikely to feel that the
demands of law are binding on his conduct."
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
152
The overall test of the null hypothesis of no significant dif-
fe~ences between delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to be-
lief in the conventional order was accepted. The mean scores of de-
linquents vs. nondelinquents were 15.4 vs. 15.8, respectively.
Table 24
Results of t-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
With Respect to Belief in the Conventional Order
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Mean SD N Mean SD N df t p
15.8 2.20 35 15.4 2.4 112 145 .94 .3
Table 24 reveals toe differences to be insignificant (p = .3) when
the index mean scores were compared. Consequently, the alternative
hypothesis of significant differences between the two groups was
rejected.
Each item in the belief index was examined in order to see
if there were any significant differences between the mean scores of
delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to their self-reported
behavior.
According to the data in Table 25, none of the belief items were
found to be statistically significant at the .05 alpha level. Also,
none of the mean scores of the two groups, delinquents and
nondelinquents were found to be identical. In items l, 3, 5, 6, 7
and 8 the mean scores of nondelinquents were slightly larger than the
mean scores of delinquents.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
153
Table 25
Results of t-test Comparison Between Nondelinquents and Delinquents
With Respect to Belief in the Conventional Order (Item by Item)
Nondelinquents Delinquents
Item # N Mean SD N Mean SD df t p
1. Victim of
theft is
equally re-
sponsible. 35 1.8 .70 112 1.5 .78 145 .02 . 90
2. Perception of
equitability
of police
treatment. 35 1.5 .61 112 1. 66 .75 145 .84 .40
3. Attitudes
toward
the law. 35 2.7 .62 112 2.6 .73 145 1.17 .24
4. Denial of
responsi·
bility. 35 1.71 .95 112 1.8 .93 144 - .35 .73
5. Respect for
the Tafilah
Police. 35 2.6 .73 112 2.4 .82 145 1. 74 .08
6. Perception of
equitability
of government
treatment. 35 1.9 .83 112 1.8 .83 145 .34 .73
7. Denial of
the victim. 35 2.5 .81 112 2.4 .82 145 .41 .67
8. Helplessness
to avoid
trouble. 35 1. 37 .59 112 2.32 .50 145 .49 .62
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
154
Summary of Fourth Hypothesis
The final null hypothesis of no significant differences between
the mean score of the two groups was accepted. An examination of the
belief element items, one-by-one, showed none to be significant.
Contrary to Hirschi's theory, the hypothesis of no significant
differences was accepted and consequently, the alternative hypothe-
sis of significant differences between the two groups was rejected.
The analysis of the data as presented in Tables 14 through 25 dealt
with the four research hypotheses and four research subhypotheses
proposed in this research. These research hypotheses and subhypotheses
are:
1. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachme~t to significant others (mother,
father, school and peers).
la. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to mother in the total index,
and in each item in the index.
lb. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to father in the total index,
and in each item in the index.
lc. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to school on the total index,
and on each item in the index.
ld. There are no significant differences between delinquents and
nondelinquents with respect to attachment to peers on the total index
and on each item in the index.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
155
2. There are no significant differences between delinquents
and nondelinquents with respect to commitment to the conventional
activities.
3. There are no significant differences between delinquents
and nondelinquents with respect to involvement in the conventional
activities.
4. There are no significant differences between delinquents
and nondelinquents with respect to belief in the conventional order.
Hypotheses la, lb, lc and ld stress that delinquents are not
significantly attached to their significant others (mother, father,
school, and peers) as is true for nondelinquents. Delinquents were
not found to be significantly different from nondelinquents in their
attachment to significant others. The null hypothesis was accepted
and the alternative hypothesis was retained.
Hypothesis 2 stresses that delinquents are not significantly
different from nondelinquents in their commitment to the conven-
tional activities. Delinquents were found to be significantly dif-
ferent from nondelinquents in their commitment to the conventional
activities. The null hypothesis was not accepted, and the alterna-
tive hypothesis had to be accepted.
Hypothesis 3 stresses that delinquents are not significantly
different from nondelinquents in their involvement in conventional
activities. The null hypothesis was accepted, and the alternative
hypothesis was retained.
Hypothesis 4 stresses that delinquents are not significantly
different from nondelinquents in their belief in the conventional
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
156
order. The null hypothesis was accepted, and the alternative hypo-
thesis was retained.
In analyzing the four major hypotheses and the four subhypo-
theses, only commitment to conventional activities was found to be
significant, the rest of the hypotheses and subhypotheses were not
supportive of Hirschi's theoretical model. In the next section, a
comparison between this dissertation's findings and Hirschi's find-
ings will shed some m0re light on Hirschi's theory and its utility
and applicability in another cultural setting, the Jordanian society.
Sun~ary of the Chapter
In Chapter IV, three aspects were addressed: (1) profile of the
sample, (2) a comparison of Hirschi's cause of delinquency and (3)
the testing of the research hypothesis was conducted.
The data revealed that 147 students completed a usable ques-
tionnaire. The subjects were unevenly distributed in the seven
schools. Seventy-eight percent of the students were 16 and 17 years
old. The parents of the students were mainly unskilled laborers in
which 37.4% of the fathers had no formal education and 91.2% of the
mothers had no formal education with the majority of them (94.6%)
housewives.
The subjects in general came from unbroken homes, 98% of the
subjects lived with their natural fathers, and 91.8% reported living
with their natural mothers. Furthermore, 95.9% of the subjects re-
ported having more than one brother and 96.6% reported having more
than one sister. In terms of religiosity, 93% reported going to the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
157
Mosque.
In describing their interest in school, well over half of the
sample reported liking school, and 72.1% planned to finish four
years of college.
From the sample profile we can assume that the subjects of this
study are attached to their parents, committed to education, and
involved in conventional activities, as well as having faith in pur-
suing education. However, over two-thirds of them reported being
involved in various activities.
The overall comparison between the results of this study and
Hirschi's findings revealed that with respect to attachment to
significant others, only attachment to father was consistent with
Hirschi's findings. Commitment to conventional activities was
inconsistent with Hirschi's findings, but commitment or involvement
in adult activities such as drinking and dating were consistent with
the theory. Smoking was another exception.
With respect to the involvement element, both involvement in
conventional and nonconventional activities were found to be
inconsistent with the theory. Finally, concerning the belief items,
five out of the seven items were inconsistent with Hirschi's findings;
only two were consistent.
Testing of the four major hypotheses revealed that only commit-
ment to conventional activities was found significant at .02 level.
At the same time, attachment to significant others, involvement in
conventional activities and belief were not significant at the .OS
alpha level. It clearly shows through the t-test that attachment to
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
158
significant others, involvement in conventional activities, and be-
lief in the conventional order had no effect on the mean scores of
delinquents or nondelinquents in Jordan.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER V
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
This chapter presents a summary of this research, its findings
as they relate to a replication of Hirschi's (1969) co<•trol theory in
the Jordanian society, and the results of the research hypotheses
which were examined. Additionally, the limitations of this research
and recommendations for future research will be discussed.
The purpose of this research was to apply Hirschi's (1969) con-
trol theory in a cultural setting dissimilar to the one found in the
United States. Hirschi's theory was selected f~r replication in Jor-
dan for several different reasons. First, Hirschi's theory is quan-
tifiable which made it possible to construct an instrument to measure
objectively the core bonding elements of social control. Second,
according to Shoemaker (1984), control theory has enjoyed a remarkable
success record, as research adopting this theoretical model has been
able to explain an average of 40% of the delinquency examined. Third,
it was believed that the extended family in Jordan still plays a sig-
nificant role in the socialization process as well as in virtually all
other areas of life. Therefore, attachment to family, peers, and
schools was expected to be significant in deterring delinquent beha-
vior. Consequently, Hirschi's theory was seen as a very appropriate
model for studying deviant behavior among Jordanian adolescents.
Fourth, it was the writer's sincere belief that in traditional,
conservative societies, people sharing common values, attitudes and
159
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
160
beliefs would be reflected in the responses of a sample of Jordanian
youth. Fifth, whether in small towns or villages, the assumption
was made that Jordanian adolescents are strongly attached, committed
to conventional activities, involved in conventional lives, and
share a strong belief in the society and its social order.
Summary
In Chapter I, a brief introduction was given about the country,
its juvenile delinquency problems, the judicial system, criminal law
as applied to juveniles, theoretical formulation, and the rationale
for the study with the statement of the problem.
Chapter II focused on the control theories in general, selected
replications of control theory and other criminological models, and
the hypotheses of the research. Control theories were divided into
two major models. First, the rationality model was presented which
included the following theories: social disorganization theory,
subcultural theories, social learning theories, and exchange theories
(especially Homans, 1961). Second, the social solidarity model
was presented which included the following theories: (a) containment
theory, (b) bonding theory, and (c) role relationship theory.
The literature review provided a remarkable record of success for
Hirschi's theory not only in terms of understanding juvenile delin-
quency in the United States, but also its international reputation.
The theory has been replicated in Canada (Caplan & Leblanc, 1985;
Linden & Currie, 1977; Linden & Fillmore, 1981). Chuen (1988) used
Hirschi's theory in Taiwan and his partial replication was consistent
---- ----------------------------------------
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
161
~,.i th the theory.
Chapter III reported on the setting, the sample, the
construction of the research instrument used in this study, data
collection procedures, measurement problems, description of the
variables, and data analysis procedures. The subjects in this study
were male eleventh graders in seven schools in the Tafielah Governate.
The total sample was composed of 147 male subjects. The subjects
answered a self-reported survey consisting of 107 questions. The
items in the questionnaire dealt with attitudes, beliefs, values,
and experiences of the subjects, among others.
Five different indices were constructed to test the hypotheses
of the research. The dependent variable consisted of a six-item in-
dex, and it was dichotomized into delinquent or nondelinquent sub-
headings. The index of "Attachment to father" was constructed from
seven items (7-21 total possible scores from the lowest to the high-
est); "Attachment to mother" was treated in the same fashion as the
father's index. "Attachment to peers" was constructed from five
items (5-15 total possible scores, from lowest to highest). "Attach-
ment to school" was constructed from seven items (ranging from 7-21
total possible scores from the lowest to the highest). Therefore,
the "Attachment to significant others" item ranged from 26-78 total
possible scores.
The "Commitment to conventional activities" index was measured
by seven items (ranging from 7-21 total possible scores). The
"involvement in conventional activities" index was measured by eight
items (ranging from 8-24 total possible scores). Finally the
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
162
"Belief" index was constructed from eight items (ranging from 8-24
total possible scores). Each index was empirically tested by using
the Cronbach alpha. The level of significance was determined at
the . OS level.
Discussion of Findings
The main questions to be answered by the data are: Is Hirschi's
theoretical model an appropriate one for understanding deviancy,
i.e., delinquency, among adolescents in Jordanian society? Do the
data support the research hypotheses? Are Jordanian adolescents
attached to their significant others, committed to conventional
activities, involved in conventional activities, and share a strong
belief in the society's laws and values?
Regarding the above questions, the findings from the Jordanian
sample appear to suggest that the youth are indeed attached to con-
ventional things, committed to conventional things, involved in con-
ventional things, as well as believe in conventional things. Theo-
retically, one would expect little or no delinquency involvement
among these young Jordanians. This assertion is based on the fact
that this study found that 52.4% of the subjects said they wanted
to be like their fathers in every 111ay or in most ways; 58% liked
their schools; 70.1% rated themselves above average compared to other
students; 57.2% cared a lot about what their teachers think of them,
77.6% claimed to have never cheated on an exam, 57.8% would like to
be like their friends in every way, 55.8% said they wanted to be
like their mothers in some ways; 93% go to religious services; 90.5%
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
163
planned to graduate from high school; 72.1% expected to finish a
four-year college, 82.3% said they would like to graduate from a
two-year college, 71.4% said that they believe in law and order, that
people who break the law should be punished, and almost 60% said that
going to the Juvenile Court would bother them. Although these
descriptive statistics appear to suggest that control theory, as
formulated by Hirschi, is appropriate for understandfng delinquency
and nondelinquency in Jordanian society, they do not provide the
total picture of the theory's appropriateness or adequacy as revealed
in the t-test results.
While the figures are consistent with Hirschi's model of social
control, methods used to test the four major hypotheses and the sub-
sequent set of four subhypotheses cast some doubt on the utility of
Hirschi's theory as the theory of best fit for explaining deviancy
in Jordan.
With one exception, the t-test conducted to test the four major
hypotheses and the subsequent set of four subhypotheses did not sup-
port Hirschi's theory. The exception was commitment to conventional
activities. In this instance, a significant difference was found
between the delinquent and nondelinquent subjects.
The first hypothesis stated that "There are no significant
differences between delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to
attachment to significant others." With regard to the first hypothe-
sis, attachment to significant others, the mean scores of the non-
delinquents were not significantly different from the mean score of
the delinquents, 57.6 vs. 56.7 respectively. Thus, attachment to
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
164
significant others had no significant effect on the reported beha-
vior of the subjects. Based on the descriptive figures discussed
earlier, it was expected that those subjects who reported liking
school, wishing to be like their mothers, fathers, and friends in
every way or in most ways, would not engage themselves in delinquent
activities. However, further test of the individual indices (father,
mother, school, peers) did not support the theory. The mean score
of nondelinquents and delinquents were as follows; 13.9 vs. 14.4;
14.6 vs. 14.3; 16.5 vs. 15.9; and 12.5 vs. 12.1 respectively. While
nondelinquents had slightly higher mean scores than delinquents on
all attachment indices, no significant differences were found, hence,
control theory was not supported. Consequently, the null hypothesis
was accepted.
The individual tests of both attachment to mother and attachment
to father did not support the theory. None of the individual index
items in the two indices was found to be significant. However, two
items were found to be significant in the attachment to school index
while only one item was found to be significant in the attachment to
peers index.
The second hypothesis stated that "There are no significant
differences between delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to
commitment to conventional activities." With regards to this hypo-
thesis, commitment to conventional activities, the mean scores of
nondelinquents and delinquents were 19.5 vs. 18.5, respectively.
The nondelinquents had larger mean scores than their delinquent
counterparts which resulted in a significant group difference.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
165
Consequently, commitment to conventional activities affected the
self-reported behavior oi the two groups. The null hypothesis was
rejected(~= .03). The test of the individual items in the commit-
ment index showed that "importance of good grades" and "schooling one
expects to complete" (aspiration) were the two significant items.
The third hypothesis stated that, "There are no significant
differences between delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to
involvement in conventional activities." Concerning this hypo-
thesis, involvement in conventional activities, the mean score of
nondelinquents and the mean score of delinquents were 18.9 and
19.1, respectively. No significant differences were found. Conse-
quently, the null hypothesis was accepted. Thus, involvement in
conventional activities had no effect on the self-reported behavior
of the subjects. Furthermore, none of the items comprising this
index was significant.
The fourth hypothesis stated that "There are no significant
differences between delinquents and nondelinquents with respect to
belief in the conventional order." The mean scores of nondelinquents
and delinquents were 15.8 and 15.4, respectively. According to the
t-test, no significant difference was found between the two groups.
Therefore, the null hypothesis was accepted. The individual test of
the items in the belief index did not support the theory as none of
the items were found to be statistically significant.
In sum, with one exception, the t-test conducted to test the
four major hypotheses and the subsequent set of the four subhypo-
theses did not support Hirschi's theory. However, the hypothesis
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
166
regarding commitment to conventional activities was significant,
thus supporting one aspect of Hirschi's theory. These and other
findings (see percentages) suggest that, on the one hand, Jordanian
youth are bonded to their society (i.e., parents, schools, peers,
etc.) while, on the other hand, they were also involved in various
delinquent activities (see hypotheses). This pattern of behavior
is contrary to the central thesis of Hirschi's theory.
Limitations of This Study
The findings of this study were limited by the very nature of
the sample. Only 147 eleventh-grade students were asked to partici-
pate in the study. Students in other grades were not part of the
study, therefore, the findings cannot be generalized beyond the
eleventh-grade level. Moreover, it was initially thought that the
sample size of the eleventh-graders would have been much larger given
the population size of the Governate. However, the investigators
later learned the class size for eleventh-graders was very small
throughout the Governate in Jordan, as there were only 147 students in
all of the seven schools selected for this investigation.
Another limitation stemmed from employing a theory devised .n
one society to explain delinquent behavior in a different culture.
As Midgley (1977) correctly pointed out, "unless there is clear-cut
evidence of concrete beliefs and values being either indigenous to a
particular setting or having been diffused into it, then the ready
application of concepts derived from a distant source is fraught with
difficulty" (p. 246). Since Hirschi's theory reflects the culture in
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
167
which it originates, (i.e., the United States in particular and Canada
in general), its relevance to a dissimilar culture is questionable.
Conclusion
Delinquency and criminal behavior have been of concern to
sociologists, and others who attempted to answer the question of why
people commit or don't commit crimes. Theories have been proposed,
accepted and rejected, each in its own time and under its own unique
circumstances. Most of these theories have originated and been tested
in the industrial world which make their applicability to Third World
countries questionable.
The theory of social control, as advanced by Hirschi, suggests
that individuals are prevented from engaging in delinquency by four
social bonds: (1) attachment to significant others; (2) commitment to
conventional activities; (3) involvement in conventional activities; and
(4) belief in the central value system of the society. When these
social bonds are weak, the individual is free to engage in delinquency.
Hirschi's theory does not appear to be particularly appropriate when
explaining juvenile delinquency in Jordan.
In comparing Hirschi's study with this study, the data indicated
that the four social bonds were not effective in deterring delin-
quency; "commitment to conventional activities" was the only exception.
Therefore, based on the data used in this research, as well as know-
ledge of the societal conditions in Jordan, the conclusion is that
Hirschi's theory does not appear to be the most suitable one to explain
deviancy, i.e., delinquency, in the Jordanian society. This assertion
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
168
is offered as a Lentative conclusion because of the limitations of
this study.
Future Study
It seems that most of the theories developed by Americans and
Europeans appear to be applicable not only to these countries in
general, but are even more specific to certain areas within those
countries. Unless theories can withstand testing cross-culturally,
it is doubtful that there is any value in basing governmental poli-
cies or programs on them.
The data presented in this study were sufficient to shed doubt
on the appropriateness of Hirschi's theory for this population.
Researchers wishing to replicate his theory should seek to understand
how bonds (both inner and outer) have been created and strengthened.
For example, why does one village school produce large numbers of
boys who go on to college, while another in the same area of the vil-
lage produces only a few? What are the reasons that one poor family
from a particular village fnnctions well and inspires commitment from
its children, while the family next door does not?
Answers to these and other questions would go a long way toward
a better understanding of how bonds are created, sustained, and
strengthened. Moreover, researchers should reconceptualize Hirschi's
theory with commitment and involvement formulated as one construct.
Furthermore, the items to measure these concepts should be pretested
thoroughly. Random samples must be taken and adequate control groups
must be established to see what factors are important in explaining
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
169
delinquency and crime in Jordan.
Much research is needed in developing broader theories of juve-
nile delinquency, and Jordan is particularly appropriate for this
type of investigation because of its homogeneous population and its
urban development.
In conclusion, this dissertation should serve as a prelude to
more comprehensive and significant research in the cross-cultural
applications of theories of juvenile delinquency. Countries like
Jordan could serve as excellent sites on which to test the various
theoretical paradigms, and, until they can withstand a cross-cultural
analysis, it is questionable if they can be particularly helpful in
expanding our knowledge in the area of juvenile delinquency.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix A
Research Instruments
170
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
171
RESEARCH INSTRUMENTS
This study aims to find ways to make life better for the young
people in the Tafielah Governate and its localities. In order to plan
useful programs we need to know a great deal about your opinions, plans,
experience, and problems.
Nobody will know your answers at the school. At the university we
will count how many students answer questions each way. Your names are
not required for this research to conceal your identity.
Dear Student:
The questionnaire is quite long, so please work as rapidly as
possible. Answer the questions frankly and honestly, even if you think
there are people who disagree with you. Also, all the questions should
be answered on a separate sheet where you can write your answers.
Please if you can't read or understand a question, raise your hand and
the researcher will answer anything you need.
Please answer the questions the way you feel. It is important for
us to know what students think. When you have finished these quc3tions,
raise your hand and the researcher will take care of it.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PLEASE NOTE:
Copyrighted materials in this document have not been filmed
at the request of the author. They are available for consultation,
however, in the author's university library.
These consist of pages:
Appendix A 172-186
Reliability Analysis 187-190
U·M·I
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
191
Correlation Matrix of Each Index
I. Attachment to father Index.
1. Would you like to be like father.
2. Father makes unfair rules.
3. Father explains the rules.
4. Father knows where you are.
5. Father knows who you are with.
6. Father's concern about your school work.
7. Do you share your feelings with father.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1 1.000
2 - .12 1.000
3 -.05 .35 1.000
4 .18 .23 .24 1.000
5 .25 .12 .22 .57 1.000
6 .35 .09 .26 .28 .30 1.000
7 .22 -.01 .12 .25 .28 .26 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
192
II. Attachment to mother Index.
1. Would you like to be like mother.
2. Mother makes unfair rules.
3. Mother explains the rules.
4. Mother knows where you are.
5. Mother knows who you are with.
6. Mother's concern about your school work.
7. Do you share your feelings with mother.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1 1.000
2 .08 1.000
3 .09 .26 1.000
4 -.17 .09 .15 1.000
5 -.13 .04 .23 .44 1.000
6 -.04 .05 .09 .18 .29 1.000
7 -.14 .07 .08 .09 .21 .19 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
193
III. Attachment to peers Index.
1. Students like your friends.
2. Teachers like your friends.
3. Friends picked up by police.
4. Would you like to be like your best friends.
5. Respect best friend's opinion.
1 2 3 4 5
1 1.000
2 .29 1.000
3 .17 .14 1.000
4 .21 .13 .21 1.000
5 -. 07 - .11 - .17 -.34 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
194
IV. Attachment to school Index.
1. Do you like school.
2. Rate yourself in school.
~. Kinds of grades.
4. Teachers care about you.
5. None of school's business.
6. Teachers pick on me.
7. Nervous in school.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1 1.000
2 .13 1.000
3 .03 .29 1.000
4 .11 -.06 -.02 1.000
5 .04 .15 -. 09 -.18 1.000
6 .20 .32 .27 .15 .09 1.000
7 .24 .19 .19 .03 .28 .61 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
195
V. Commitment Index.
1. Kinds of grades.
2. Importance of getting good grades.
3. Importance of grades for job.
4. The only reason to have a job is money.
5. Planning to graduate from high school.
6. Schooling expect to get.
7. Schooling would like to get.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1 1.000
2 .12 1.000
3 .03 .29 1.000
4 .11 -.06 -. 02 1.000
5 .04 .15 .09 - .18 1.000
6 .20 .32 . 27 .15 .08 1.000
7 .24 .18 .19 .03 .27 .62 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
196
VI. Involvement Index.
1. Kinds of grades.
2. Do you finish your homework.
3. Do teachers check your homework.
4. Time devoted to homework.
5. Suspension from school.
6. Mosque.
7. Do you work with your mother in the house.
8. Do you work with your father in the house.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1 1.000
2 .03 1.000
3 -.08 .20 1.000
4 .09 .16 .13 1.000
5 .07 .18 - 0 01 -.04 1.000
6 .17 .12 .18 .04 .07 1.000
7 -.08 .OS .12 .05 .02 - 0 07 1.000
8 -. 01 - 0 01 .13 .05 .02 - .07 .65 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
197
VII. Belief Index.
1. Leaves the keys.
2. Policemeen try to give all kids a break.
3. Alright to get around the law.
4. Criminals should not be blamed.
5. Respect for the police.
6. Government does not care.
7. Suckers deserve to be taken advantage of.
8. Stay out of trouble.
l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1 1.000
2 .21 1.000
3 -.03 -.02 1.000
4 -. 09 -.08 .05 1.000
5 -.11 .02 .16 -.15 1.000
6 -.09 -.05 -. 06 -.04 .15 1.000
7 -.03 .20 .09 -.03 .10 .13 1.000
8 -. 09 .05 -. 09 .04 .02 -. 03 - .12 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
198
VIII. Delinquency Index.
1. Theft of 3 JD.
2. Theft of 3-30 JD
3. Theft of more than 30 JD.
4. Fights.
5. Written on school restroom walls.
6. Damaged a street sign.
1 2 3 4 5 6
1 1.000
2 .68 1.000
3 .73 .65 1.000
4 .22 .08 .06 1.000
5 .26 .24 .26 .37 1.000
6 .22 .15 .13 .15 .39 1.000
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
199
.:.:.I~.J-5 .=.:!I.e. ~·~·
~~~ -,;,jl..2~
}'\'\'\•!' I' 0
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PLEASE NOTE:
Copyrighted materials in this document have not been filmed
at the request of the author. They are available for consultation,
however, in the author's university library.
These consist of pages:
Instruments
200-213
U·M-1
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Abbott, D. J. (1971). Crime and development in an African city.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin,
Madison.
Adams, R. (1973). Differential association and learning principles
revisited. Social Problems, 20, 458-470.
Agnew R. (1985). Social control theory and delinquency: A longi-
tudinal test. Criminology, 23(1), 47-61.
Akers, R. L. (1973). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Al··langawi, A. M. (1987). Unrecorded delinquency in the State of
Kuwait, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Mississippi State
University, Jackson.
Arnold, E. L. (1983). Preventing adolesc~nt alienation: An inter-
personal approach. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Bandura, A., & Walters, H. R. (1963). Social learning and personality
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Beaumont, P., Gerald, B. H., & Wagstaff, M. (1967). The Middle East:
A geographical study. New York: Wiley.
Bennett, R. R., & Lynch, J. P. (1990). Does a difference make a
difference? Comparing cross-national crime indicators.
Criminology, 28(1), 153-181.
Blau, P.M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York:
Wiley.
Bohrnstedt, G. W., & Kroke, D. (1982). Statistics for social data
analysis. Itasca, IL: Peacock.
Briar, S., & Piliavin, L. (1965). Delinquency, situational induce-
ments, and commitment to conformity. Social Problems, 13, 35-45.
Burgess, R. L., & Akers, R. (1966). Are operant principles
tautological? Psychological Record, 16, 305-312.
Caplan, A., & Leblanc, M. (1985). A cross-cultural verification of a
social control theory. International Journal of Comparative and
Applied Criminal Justice, ~(2), 123-138.
214
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
215
Caplovitz, D. (1983). The stages of social research. New York:
Wiley.
Cernkovich, S.A. (1978). Evaluating two models of delinquency
causation: Structural theory and control theory. Criminolo~,
16(3)' 334-352.
Chambers, C.D., & Inciardi, J. (1971). Deviant behavior in the Middle
East: A study of delinquency in Iraq. Criminology, 2(2), 291-315.
Chuen J. S. (1988). Juvenile delinquency in the Republic of China: A
Chinese empirical study of social control theory. International
Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 12(2), 59-71.
Clinard, M. B., & Abbott, D. (1973). Crime in developing countries.
New York: Wiley.
Cloward, R. A., & Ohlin, L. (1960). Delinquency and power in social
life. New York: Wiley.
Cohen, A. K. (1955). Delinquent Boys. New York: Free Press.
Conger, R. (1976). Social control and social learning models of
delinquent behavior: Asynthesis. Criminology, 14(1), 17-40.
Currie, R. F. (1973). Religion and images of man among Calgary youth.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Fordham University, Toronto,
Canada.
Defleur, B. L. (1967). A cross-cultural comparison of juvenile
offenders and offenses: Cordoba, Argentina and the United States.
Social Problems, ~. 483-495.
Dentler, R. A., & Monroe, L. (1961). Social correlates uf early
adolescent theft. American Sociological Review, 26, 733-743.
Downes, D. (1966). The delinquent solution. New York: Free Press.
Duncan, B., & Dunca~, 0. (1978). Sex typing and social ro1es. New
York: Academic Press.
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide. (J. Spaulding & G. Simpson, Trans.).
New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1897).
Ekeh, P. P. (1974). Social exchange theory: The two traditions.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Elliott, D. S., & Ageton, S. S. (1980). Reconsidering race and class
differences in self-reported and official estimates of delinquency.
American Sociological Review, 45(2), 95-110 .
. - ---·- -- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - · - - - - ---
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
216
Ellis, D. P. (1971). The Hobbesian problem of order: A critical
appraisal of the normative solution. American Sociological Review,
36(3), 692-703.
Emerson, R. M. (1969). Operant psychology and exchange theory. InK.
Christiansen (Ed.), Scandinavian studies in criminology (Vol. l,
pp. 117-46). London: Tavistock.
Empey, L. (1978). American delinquency. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
Eve, R. (1978). A study of the efficacy and interactions of several
theories for explaining rebellious<;css among high school students.
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 69, 115-125.
Eysenck, H. (1964). Crime and personality. Boston, MA: Houghton
Mifflin.
Fisher, W. B. (1986). Jordan: Physical and social geography. The
Middle East and North Africa (32nd ed.). Europa: Publications
Limited.
Frazer, S. J. (1919). Folklore in the Old Testament (Vol. II).
London: Macmillan.
Frazier, C. E. (1976). Theoretical approaches to deviance: An
evaluation. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Friday, P. C. (1970). Differential opportunity and a&sociation in
Sweden: A study of youth crime. Unpublished doctoral
dissertat~on, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
Friday, P. C., & Hage, G. (1976). Youth crime in postindustrial
societies: An integrated perspective. Criminology, 14, 347-368.
Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1950). Unraveling juvenile delinquency.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gold, M. (1966). Undetected delinquent behavior. Journal of Research
in Crime and Delinquency, 1. 27-46.
Hage, J., & Marwell, G. (1968). Toward the development of an empirical
based theory of role relationships. Sociometry, 31(6), 200-212.
Hamilton, V. (1965). Book review of H. J. Eysenck, Crime and
Personality, British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,~.
159-60.
Hepburn, J. (1977). Testing alternative models of delinquency
causation. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 67, 450-460.
Hindelang, M. J. (1973). Causes of delinquency: A partial replication
and extension. Social Problems, 20, 471-487.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
217
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Hirschi, T., & Stark, R. (1969). Hellfire and delinquency. Social
Problems, 17, 202-213.
Hornans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New
York: Harcourt, Brace & World.
Howell, J. C. (1977). A comparative analysis of delinquency prevention
theorv: Preventing delinquency (Vol. I of IX). Washington, DC:
U. S. Department of Justice.
Irfaifeh, A. A. (1988, March). Recent trends in juvenile delinquency
in Jordan. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Michigan
Academy of Arts, Sciences and the Humanities, Saginaw, MI.
Jeffery, G. R. (1965). Criminal behavior and learning theory. Journal
of Criminal Law. Criminology, and Public Science, 56, 294-300.
Jensen, G. F. (1972). Parents, peers, and delinquent action: A test
of the differential association perspective. American Journal of
Sociology, 78{3), 562-575.
Johnson, R. E. (1979). Juvenile delinquency and its origins.
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Knox, G. W. (1981). Social disorganization models of deviance. In G.
F. Jensen (Ed.), Sociology of delinquency (pp. 6-7). Beverly
Hills: Sage.
Kornhauser, R. R. (1978). Social sources of delinquency: An appraisal
of analytic models. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Krohn, M.D., Lanza-Kaduce, L., & Akers, R. L. (1984). Community
context and theories of deviant behavior: An examination of social
learning and social bonding theories. The Sociological Quarterly,
25, 353-371.
Krohn, M.D., & Massey, J. (1980). Social control and delinquent
behavior: An examination of the elements of the social bond.
Sociological Quarterly, 21, 529-544.
Kunkel, J. H., & Nagasawa, R. H. (1973). A behavioral model of man:
Propositions and implications. American Sociological Review, 38,
530-543.
Levi-Strauss, C. (1969). The elementary structures of kinship. (H. B.
James, R. S. John & N. Rodney, Trans.). Boston: Beacon.
(Original work published 1949).
Lewis, M. (1972). Parents and children: Sex-role development. School
Review, 80, 229.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
218
Linden, R., & Currie, R. E. (1977). Religion and drug use: A test of
social control theory. Canadian Journal of Criminology and
Corrections, 19, 346-355.
Linden, R., & Fillmore, C. (1981). A comparative study of delinquency
involvement. Canadian Review of Sociolo~y and Anthrop~, 18(3),
342-361.
Linden, E., & Hackler, J. C. (1973). Affective ties and delinquency.
Pacific Sociolo~ical Review, 16, 27-46.
Lipton, W., & Hackler, J. (1973). Affective ties and delinquency.
Pacific Sociolo~ical Review, 16(1), 27-46.
Lyerly, R. R., & Skipper, J. K., Jr. (1981). Differential rates of
rural-urban delinquency: A social control approach. Criminology,
19, 385-399.
Malinowski, G. (1922). Ar~onauts of the Western Pacific. London:
Routeledge & Kegan Paul.
Marwell, G., & Hage, J. (1970). The organization of role
relationships: A systematic description. American Sociolo~ical
Review, 10(35), 884-900.
Matsueda, R. L. (1982). Testing control theory and differential
association: A causal modeling approach. American Sociological
Review, 47(4), 489-504.
Matza, D. (1964). Delinquency and drift. New York: Wiley.
Matza, D. (1969). Becomin~ deviant. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Mauss, M. (1954). The Gift. (I. Gunnison, Trans.). New York: Free
Press. (Original work published 1925).
Merton, R. K. (1937). Social structure and anomie. American
Sociolo~ical Review, 10(3), 672-682.
Midgley, J. (1977). The sociology of crime in South Africa: Studies
in the cross-cultural replication of criminological models.
International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 2. 245-261.
Miethe, T. D. (1982). Public consensus of crime seriousness:
Normative structure or methodological artifact? Criminology,
20(3), 515-526.
Miller, W. B. (1958). Lower class culture as a generating milieu of
gang delinquency. The Journal of Social Issues, 14, 5-19.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
219
Ministry of Interior Affairs, Police Bureau. (1982). The Criminal
Annual Re:[1ort. Amman, Jordan: Amman Department of Public Safety.
Ministry of Interior Affairs, Police Bureau. (1986). The Crimina:l
Annual Rei!ort. Amman, Jordan: Amman Department of Public Safety.
Ministry of Planning--Jordan. (1986). Five year plan for economic
and social development, The Annual Book. Amman, Jordan: The
Institute for Planning.
Ministry of Planning--Jordan. (1989). Five year plan for economic
and social development, The Annual Book. Amman, Jordan: The
Institute for Planning.
Ministry of Work and Social Development. (1978). The Annual Report.
Amman, Jordan: Amman Department of Public Works.
Minor, W. W. (1984). Neutralization as a hardening process: Consi-
derations in the modeling of change. Social Forces, 52, 995-
1019.
Minor, W. W. (1975). Control theory and deterrence of crime: A
theoretical and em:[1irical integration. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Florida State University, Tallahassee.
Morris, N. (1964). Impediments to penal reform. University of Chicago
Law Review, 33, 627-696.
Nettler, G. (1974). Explaining crime. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Nye, P. (1958). Family relationships and delinquent behavior. New
York: Wiley.
Nye, P., & Short, J. F., Jr. (1957). Scaling delinquent behavior.
American Sociological Review, 26, 733-743.
Ohlin, L., & Cloward, R. (1973). The prevention of delinquent
subcultures. In M. S. Weinburg & E. Rubington (Eds.), The.study
of social problems: Five perspectives (2nd ed, pp. 20-41). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Okada, D. W. (1987). Jai!anese American juvenile delinquency: An
examination of control theory in a Japanese American community.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, College
Park.
Paternoster, R., Saltzman, L., Waldo, G., & Chiricos, T. (1983).
Perceived risk and social control: Do sanctions really deter? Law
and Society Review, 17, 457-479.
Pavlov, P. (1960). Conditional Reflexes. (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). New
York: Dover. (Original work published 1927).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
220
Poole, E. D., & Regoli, R. M. (1979). Parental support, delinquent
friends, and delinquency: A test of interaction affect. The
Journal of Criminal Lm~ and Criminality, 1, 189-193.
Public Census Bureau. (1979). The Annual Census Report. Amman,
Jordan: Department of Statistics.
Public Census Bureau. (1985). The Annual Census Report. Amman,
Jordan: Department of Statistics.
Public Census Bureau. (1987). The Annual Census Report. Amman,
Jordan: Department of Statistics.
Radzinowicz, L., & King, J. (1977). The growth of crime. New York:
Basic Books.
Reckless, W. C. (1961). A new theory of delinquency and crime.
Federal Probation, 25, 42-46.
Reckless, W. C., & Dinitz, S. (1967). Pioneering with self-concept
as a vulnerability factor in delinquency. Journal of Criminal
Law, Criminology, and Police Science, 58, 515-523.
Reiss, A. J., Jr. (1951). Delinquency as the failure of personal
and social control. American Sociological Review, 16, 196-207.
Scott, J. F. (1971). Internationalization of norms: A sociological
theory of moral commitment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Sellin, T., & Wolfgang, M. (1964). The measurement of delinquency.
New York: Wiley.
Shaw, C., & McKay, H. (1942). Delinquency in urban areas. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Shoemaker, D. J. (1984). Theories of delinquency. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Short, J. F., Jr., & Strodtbeck, F. (1965). Group process and gang
delinquency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York:
Macmillan.
Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of criminology (4th ed.).
Chicago: Lippincott.
Sutherland, E. H., & Cressey, D. (1970). Criminology. Philadelphia:
Lippincott.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
221
Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of rteutralization: A
theory of delinquency. American Journal of Sociology, 22, 664-
670.
Tangri, S. S., & Schwartz, M. (1967). Delinquency research and the
self-concept variable. Journal of Criminal Law. Criminology and
Police Science, 58, 182-190.
Tittle, C. R., & Villemez, W. (1977). Social class and criminality.
Social Forces, 12(56), 474-502.
Toby, J. (1957). Social disorganization and stake in conformity:
Complementary factors in the predatory behavior of hoodlums.
Journal of Criminal Law Criminology and Police Science, 48, 12-
17.
Touq, M. (1980). Juvenile delinquency in Jordan: Pilot study.
Dirrasat, l, 7-57.
Traub, S. H., & Little, C. B. (1985). Theories of deviance. Itasca,
IL: Peacock, Inc.
Turner, J. H. (1974). The structure of sociological theories.
Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.
Weinberg, K. S. (1964). Juvenile delinquency in Ghana: A comparative
analysis of delinquents and non-delinquents. Journal of Criminal
Law Criminology and Police Science, 55, 471-481.
Wiatrowski, M., Griswold, D. B., & Roberts, M. K. (1981). Social
control theory and delinquency. American Sociological Review, 46,
525-541.
Wolfgang, E. M., & Ferracuti, F. (1967). The subculture of violence.
London: Tavistock.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.