English Phonetic Course Assimilation & Elision: Related Papers
English Phonetic Course Assimilation & Elision: Related Papers
Lesson Plan
1. To raise students’ awareness that assimilation and elision are the typical features of rapid
speech, during which several articulators are involved and are not capable of moving
instantaneously.
2. To explain the rules of assimilation and elision to students before attempting to put them into
practice.
3. Design some activities for focusing on assimilation and elision.
4. Ask the students to listen to the tapes and imitate the connected speech in focus.
5. Have the students listen to the conversations and attempt to gain a natural transition of two
adjacent sounds.
6. Have the students practise the guided conversation. Ask them to pay special attention to
assimilation and elision in connected speech.
7. Comment on the students’ performance by highlighting the achievement of the students and
the efforts they need for the improvement.
8. Ask the students to do more practice after class and get ready for presentation during the next
session.
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
Teaching Notes
1. Assimilation
Assimilation is concerned with one sound becoming phonetically similar to an adjacent sound.
Sounds that belong to one word can cause changes in sounds belonging to other words.
When a word’s pronunciation is affected by sounds in a neighbouring word, we call this process
assimilation.
Much recent phonetic research in this area refers to coarticulation (Hardcastle & Hewlett,
1999: 7), though the latter term strictly refers to the fact that, when pronounced, certain sounds are
uttered together and thus it actually describes the cause of assimilation. In rapid and casual speech,
the assimilated form is more typical of connected speech.
Depending on the type of feature that spreads from one segment to another we can observe
several major types of assimilation such as assimilative processes involving voicing, manner of
articulation, place of articulation (Roach, 2000: 139) and coalescence of place and manner of
articulation (Collins & Mees 2003: 106).
Phoneme1 Phoneme 2
progressive
regressive
Regressive assimilation: the sounds assimilated are influenced by the succeeding sounds
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
Progressive assimilation: the sounds assimilated are affected by the pronunciation of the
preceding sounds. This is often sen in the inflectional endings -s and –ed. If preceded by a voiced
sound, they become voiced; if preceded by a voiceless one, they become voiceless. For example:
Voiceless Voiced
Plural: students /-s/; books /-s/ girls /-z/; pictures /-z/
Possessive: students’ books /-s/ girls’ pictures /-z/
3rd person singular: He writes /-s/; He speaks /-s/ She reads /-z/; She plays /-z/
Past tense and past participle: worked /-t/; laughed /-t/ learned /-d/; played /-d/
This may refer to assimilation involving the feature [+/- voice]. In a certain environment we
can consequently observe the voicing or devoicing of a segment. If we examine the pronunciation
of the voiced labiodental fricative in the sequence give books /gɪv bʊks/, and we compare it with the
sequence give peace /gɪf pi ːs/ we will easily notice that while in the first case /v/ is fully voiced, in
the second it is rather pronounced as some kind of /f/. However, word boundary final voiceless
consonants in English do not show tendencies to assimilate to their voiced counterparts; thus
the pronunciation of nice boy /naɪz bɔɪ/ will sound foreign to English ears (Cruttenden, 2001: 284).
The most common phonemic changes at word boundaries concern changes of place of
articulation, particularly involving de-alveolarization. A well-known case is that of English
word-final alveolar consonants such as /t, d, n/: if a word ending in one of these consonants is
followed by a word whose initial consonant begins with a bilabial, a velar or a dental, the word-final
alveolar consonant is likely to change its place of articulation to match that at the beginning of the
second word. Thus the word ‘that’ /ðat/ may be followed by ‘boy’ /bɔɪ/ and become /ðap/, or it may be
followed by ‘girl’ and become /ðak/, namely:
that boy → /ðap bɔɪ/ that girl → /ðak gɜːl/ (Cruttenden, 2001: 285)
good pen → /gʊb pen/ good concert → /gʊg kɒnsət/ (Cruttenden, 2001: 285)
ten players → /tem pleɪəz/ ten cups → /teŋ kʌps/ (Cruttenden, 2001: 285)
that thing → /ðat ̪ θɪŋ/ get those → /gɛ t ̪ ðəʊz/ (Roach, 2000: 139)
Such changes are based on roughly homorganic mouth articulations. They always occur in the
least obvious part of the syllable coda position. “The reason why initial consonants are so
rarely obviously changed by assimilatory processes is that syllable initial consonants play a
much more important part in identifying a word than do syllable final consonants.” (Brown
1990: 65).
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
Another change of place of articulation involves the change of alveolar fricatives /s, z/ into
postalveolar fricatives. Any sequence of two words where the first ends with /s, z/ and the second
begins with /ʃ/ or /j/, that /s/ becomes /ʃ/, and /z/ becomes /ʒ/. Thus this shoe /ðɪs ʃuː/ becomes /ðɪʃ ʃuː/;
those years /ðəʊz j ɪəz/ becomes /ðəʊʒ j ɪəz/. (Roach, 2000: 140)
Assimilation of place is noticeable in the regressive assimilation of alveolar consonants.
Progressive assimilation is relatively uncommon. It may occur when a plosive is followed by
a syllabic nasal and the nasal undergoes assimilation to the same place of articulation as the
preceding plosive, e.g.
/n/ → /m/ after /p, b/, happen, urban /hæpm, ɜːbm/; and
/n/ → /ŋ/ after /k, g/ in second chance, organ, bacon as /sekŋ tʃɑːns, ɔːgŋ, beɪkŋ/
Regressive assimilation is commonly seen in some prefixes, such as in- (before /t, d, s, n/), im-
(before /p, b, m/); il- (before /l/); ir- (before /r/). Foe example:
in- → indirect, insane,
im- → impolite, imbalance, immoral (不道德的)
il- → illegal
ir- → irregular
Assimilation of manner is typical of the most rapid and casual speech, in which case one
sound changes the manner of its articulation to become similar in manner to a neighbouring sound.
An example can be a rapid pronunciation of “Get some of that soup”, where instead of the expected
/gɛt sʌm əv ðat suːp/ an English speaker says /gɛs sʌm v ðas suːp/, with /s/ replacing /t/ in two words. It
is thus possible to find cases where a final plosive becomes a fricative or nasal (e.g. ‘good night’ /gʊn
naɪt/), but most unlikely that a final fricative or nasal would become a plosive (Roach, 2000: 140).
Assimilation of different types may occur simultaneously: e.g. the plosives /t, d/ merge with
word-initial palatal approximant /j/ in a process of reciprocal assimilation of place and manner, and the
fricatives /s, z/ have similar reciprocal assimilation with /j/. The resulting single articulation is
postalveolar, i.e. about halfway between alveolar and palatal, and so represents a ‘compromise’. ”
(Heselwood 2005) Examples can be found as follows (Cruttenden, 2001: 286):
/z/ + /j/ → /ʒ/ Has your letter come? /hæʒɔː letə kʌm/
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
The coalescence is more complete in the case of /t, d/ + /j/ (especially in question tags, e.g.
won’t you?, could you? etc.); in the case of /s, z/ + /j/, the coalescence into /ʃ, ʒ/ may be marked
by extra length of friction, e.g. miss you /mɪʃʃu/. “Sometimes the coalescence is not complete and
there is a /j/ after the postalveolar consonant, e.g. not yet becomes /nɒtʃ j ɛt/, red yacht becomes /rɛdʒ
j ɒt/; miss York becomes / mɪʃ j ɔːk/, these young people becomes /ði ːʒ j ʌŋ pi ːpl/. Where this happens it is
really a case of assimilation rather than coalescence, although it can be referred to as ‘incomplete
coalescence’.” (Heselwood 2005)
One reason for the assimilatory processes is that several articulators are involved in making a
speech sound, and that they are not capable of moving instantaneously. Much of the earlier writing on
assimilation has suggested that assimilatory changes generally involve a change from one phoneme
to another; for example, that boy is expressed as showing a change from /t/ to /p/. However, this does
not mean that all assimilations involve phonemic change of this sort. An easy process to observe is
the position of the lips. In a vowel such as English /i ː/ (as in see), the lips are spread, as for a smile. In
a vowel such as English /uː/ (as in two), the lips are rounded and pushed forward. Since lip-position
is not a distinctive feature opposing any two phonemes in English, the difference between rounded
and non-rounded /t/ is not phonemic (Cruttenden, 2001: 282). Another explanation for assimilation is
based on the articulatory one. The explanation seems to assume that speakers are basically lazy, and
do as little work as possible – this is sometimes called the “principle of least effort” (Zipf 1949), and
it does seem to explain phonetic features of assimilation in a very simple way.
2. Elision
The term elision describes the disappearance of a sound. For example, in the utterance He
leaves next week speakers would generally elide (leave out) the /t/ in next saying /neks
wi:k/. Again here, the reason is an economy of effort, and in some instances the difficulty
of putting certain consonant sounds together while maintaining a regular rhythm and
speed.
Some rules for elision
2.1 The most common elisions in English are /t/ and /d/, when they appear within a
consonant cluster.
We arrived the next day. (/t/ elided between /ks/ and /d/)
When we reached Paris, we stopped for lunch. (/t/ elided between // and /p/, and
between /p/ and /f/
We bought a lovely carved statuette. (/d/ elided between /v/ and /st/)
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
It’s a question of collective responsibility. (// can disappear)
Are you coming out tonight? (// can disappear)
That’s an interesting idea. (// is not pronounced by many speakers, reducing the
number of syllables in the word)
Have we got any vegetables? (// is not pronounced by most speakers, reducing the
number of syllables in the word)
4. Practice
Work in pairs. First find out what kind of assimilation each phrase or sentence belongs to
and then read them aloud.
7.1 would you / could you / did you / this year / miss you / space ship / question / immediate /
associate / education / newspaper / in ten minutes / handkerchief / breadth / absorption (absorb)
/ fifth / measure / of course / won’t you / student / presume / commercial / leisure / special /
appreciative / virtue / mutual / congratulate / literature / sociology / punctual / Christianity / in
case you need it / in good condition
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn
Could you tell me the way to the post office?
Would you mind if I open the window?
We’ve been waiting for you for ten minutes.
Don’t be disappointed, he’ll soon be here.
I used to play volleyball when I was young.
He returned the books with thanks.
Give me a book.
Coke could hardly believe his ears. Here he was, a stranger in her house and yet she
said she was sorry.
“I’m sorry.” She said again. “I didn’t hear you. Did you knock? I’m deaf, you see.”
Why don’t you see if any of his clothes fit you? You can bring them back tomorrow.
PDF 文件使用 " pdf Fact or y Pr o" 试用版本创建 www. f i nepr i nt . com. cn