Responding To Misbehavior in Young Children: How Authoritative Parents Enhance Reasoning With Firm Control
Responding To Misbehavior in Young Children: How Authoritative Parents Enhance Reasoning With Firm Control
Responding to Misbehavior
in Young Children: How
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
DOI: 10.1037/13948-005
Authoritative Parenting: Synthesizing Nurturance and Discipline for Optimal Child Development, Robert E.
Larzelere, Amanda Sheffield Morris, and Amanda W. Harrist (Editors)
Copyright © 2013 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
89
for the effects of different styles on children’s development” (Parke & Buriel,
2006, p. 437).
In this chapter, we focus on specific processes consistent with authorita-
tive parental responses to misbehavior, especially in 2- to 8-year-old children,
whether clinically referred or not. Misbehavior refers to behavior considered
inappropriate by parents, whether considered intentional or not. The chapter
applies to both mothers and fathers.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
due only to its nurturance and communication components and not to its
power-assertive components? If some power-assertive characteristics are an
inherent part of authoritative parenting (Baumrind et al., 2010), then how is
effective power assertion distinguished from ineffective or counterproductive
power assertion?
To find examples of effective use of power-assertive tactics, we need look
no further than the consequence-focused research that dominates behavior
management research to treat oppositional defiance and conduct disorders in
2- to 8-year-olds. Five of the six most effective clinical treatments for these
disorders teach parents to use power-assertive tactics systematically, includ-
ing clear commands, single warnings, time out, and enforcements for time
out (Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs, 2008). Currently, enforcements for time out
vary from a brief forced isolation (Hembree-Kigin & McNeil, 1995; Roberts
& Powers, 1990), chores or privilege removal (Forgatch & Patterson, 2010),
and a gentle restraint (M. Sanders, personal communication, May 2, 2011).
In contrast to a reasoning-focused perspective, the only kind of disciplinary
reasoning permitted in behavior management’s responses to misbehavior is
a concise description of the disciplinary consequences (e.g., Hembree-Kigin
& McNeil, 1995, pp. 78–79) or, in one variation, brief statements showing
empathy or addressing fairness (M. Sanders, personal communication, May 2,
2011). Parental rationales and negotiations are not considered permissible
responses to misbehavior.
To summarize, some prominent parenting researchers support an author-
itative combination of disciplinary reasoning and power assertion. In con-
trast, some conclusions from child developmental research seem to oppose all
power assertion, whereas the best-supported parent management treatments
for disruptive behavior disorders train parents in some power-assertive tac-
tics while limiting the use of reasoning mostly to clarifying consequences.
How can these contrasting perspectives help clarify the authoritative inte-
gration of disciplinary reasoning and power assertion? Bell’s control system
model suggests a synthesis that accounts for the contrasting evidence while
showing how reasoning, power assertion, and other aspects of discipline can
best work together in authoritative parenting.
Bell’s control system model explains how parents and children regu-
late each other’s behavior (Bell & Chapman, 1986; Bell & Harper, 1977).
The model is derived from general control systems theory and from Bell’s
(1968) classic article incorporating child effects into an understanding of
parental influences on children. Figure 4.1 illustrates how this model applies
to children with competent or oppositional behavior, to the different empha-
ses found in reasoning-focused and consequence-focused research, and for
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
Time
Ongoing
Temperature
Fluctuation
Maintenance Region of
Optimal Temperature
Time
Figure 4.1. Two applications of Bell’s control system model.
dence against power assertion and its role in effective parental management
for oppositional children. We conclude by summarizing other implications
of Bell’s model, first that the relative effectiveness of reasoning and power
assertion varies by child temperament and that the sequence of disciplin-
ary actions provides a basis for reducing the need for parents to use power
assertion.
gencies from previous episodes can provide the background power assertion
that is ideal for maximizing moral internalization according to Hoffman. To
Patterson (1982, p. 111), the most important skill to teach parents of defi-
ant children is how to use punishment effectively, referring to time out or
privilege removal. Effective use of time out to enforce compliance improves
cooperation with parental requests because the child’s defiance no longer
succeeds in getting the parents to drop their demands. Oppositional children
learn to cooperate with a milder verbal discipline tactic to the extent it has
been enforced effectively with time out or similar negative consequences in
prior episodes. Whereas mild verbal discipline is limited to commands and
warnings in behavior management, the same contingencies can also enforce
the kinds of disciplinary explanations emphasized in the reasoning-focused
perspective (Larzelere, Sather, Schneider, Larson, & Pike, 1998). This illus-
trates the potential of integrating processes involving both reasoning and
consequences into a single model consistent with authoritative parenting.
This systematic sequence of disciplinary tactics not only accounts for the
effectiveness of behavior management but also explains why correlations are
biased against all power-assertive tactics.
versus less effective ways of implementing them (Deptula et al., 2010; Hughes
et al., 2006). Similar discriminations need to be made about corrective dis-
ciplinary actions to clarify how authoritative parents use them to respond
effectively to misbehavior.
The associations of corrective actions with negative child outcomes are
due to an inherent selection bias caused by child effects (Larzelere, Kuhn, &
Johnson, 2004). Parents are less likely to use corrective actions when chil-
dren are cooperative, respond well to reasoning, do well in school, do not
experience racial discrimination, and are unlikely to smoke or be sexually
precocious. Quite simply, the correlational evidence underlying these con-
clusions occurs because parents need no corrective actions when children are
doing well and not facing risky environments. Many parent–child associa-
tions in the literature may therefore reflect statistical artifacts due to child
effects rather than true causal effects.
This type of artifact explains why corrective actions shown to be effec-
tive in randomized trials appear harmful according to longitudinal correla-
tions. For example, longitudinal analyses show that psychotherapy and Ritalin
appear to be as harmful as spanking and nonphysical punishment, even after
controlling for preexisting differences on child outcomes (Larzelere, Cox,
et al., 2010; Larzelere, Ferrer, et al., 2010).
Correlations cannot discriminate between effective versus harmful cor-
rective actions if those actions are activated by a control system process like
Bell’s model. Indeed, a perfect corrective action would produce a correlation
of r = .00 with subsequent measures of the problem being corrected because
it would make recipients indistinguishable from those who never had the
problem. Relevant causal evidence about corrective disciplinary actions must
overcome this bias to clarify how disciplinary tactics can best be sequenced in
a manner that is consistent with authoritative parenting.
research needs to compare alternative tactics for the same disciplinary situa-
tion, such as enforcing cooperation with time out. A second principle is that
multiple options help parents find the best alternative for each child and each
disciplinary situation. Roberts and Powers (1990) found that the two best
enforcements each worked when the other one failed to achieve compliance
with time out after several attempts. The availability of multiple alterna-
tives is desirable at any stage of a sequence of tactics to prevent unnecessary
increases in forcefulness. A third principle is illustrated by the fact that the
mildest enforcement for time out worked for some children even though it
was the least effective overall. That is, the child-determined release worked
for children who sat on the time out chair for more than a few seconds.
A fourth principle was illustrated by Roberts’s (1982) research on a
single warning preceding time out. Compliance improved just as rapidly as
in no-warning sequence, but the number of time outs needed to achieve
compliance was reduced by an average of 74%. This step in the sequence of
tactics thus reduced forcefulness (e.g., number of time outs) without compro-
mising effectiveness.
This conditional sequence of disciplinary tactics helps clinically defiant
children learn to cooperate with verbal discipline in the form of commands
and warnings and enables parents to phase out the most forceful tactics in the
sequence, starting with the time-out enforcement and then decreasing the
frequency of time out itself (McNeil & Hembree-Kigin, 2010). Children thus
learn to cooperate with the milder verbal tactics that developmental scholars
have found to be correlated with more appropriate child behavior (Roberts
& Powers, 1990). Thus, consequence-focused and reasoning-focused perspec-
tives share the goal of maintaining appropriate cooperation with mild verbal
discipline tactics. Behavior management research, however, has produced
causal evidence showing how power-assertive tactics can be sequenced to
achieve this goal, even for the most oppositional young children (Eyberg
et al., 2008; Pelham & Fabiano, 2008; Roberts & Powers, 1990).
Disciplinary reasoning cannot influence moral internalization unless
children pay attention to it. Consistent with authoritative parenting, the
effectiveness of disciplinary reasoning depends on it being backed up with
Time-Out (T-O)
Warning
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
this, Kochanska (1997; Kochanska et al., 2007) has shown repeatedly that
observed power assertion is correlated with lower subsequent moral behavior
for temperamentally fearful preschoolers but not for fearless preschoolers.
This follows from Bell’s model in that power-assertive tactics will be needed
more often for children with difficult temperaments (compare the competent
and oppositional children in Figure 4.1). Oppositional children need a bal-
ance of appropriate verbal corrections and power assertion, whereas power
assertion is less necessary with easily managed children and may even hinder
their moral internalization (Morris & Gibson, 2011). Accordingly, authorita-
tive parents use minimally sufficient power for cooperation, whereas author-
itarian parents use too much forcefulness, and permissive parents use too
little (Baumrind et al., 2010, p. 185). Power assertion can be minimized more
readily in Kochanska’s fearful children for two reasons. First, milder verbal
corrections are more likely to be effective without power assertion for fearful
children than for fearless children. Second, parents can rely on background
power assertion more quickly for fearful children than for fearless children.
In either case, a combination of moderate power assertion and reasoning
would be more effective for fearless children (Hoffman, 2000, p. 154). The
role of moderate power assertion in defiant situations outside the laboratory
would have been missed by correlational analyses of the mild power assertion
observed in Kochanska’s lab.
The other side of the continuum predicts that the greater effective-
ness of power assertion over exclusively positive tactics will be most evident
among clinically referred children. Patterson (1982) initially tried to help
parents manage their disruptive children by emphasizing reinforcement of
appropriate behaviors. By 1982, he concluded, “If I were allowed to select
only one concept to use in training parents of antisocial children, I would
teach them how to punish more effectively” (p. 111), referring to time out as
his punishment of choice. This does not mean that positive parenting skills
are unimportant with oppositional children: Showing enthusiasm about what
the child is interested in was shown to be a common component of more
effective behavior management interventions in a meta-analysis (Kaminski,
Valle, Filene, & Boyle, 2008). The effectiveness of positive parenting actions
A Preventative Sequence
Bell’s model also suggests the possibility of applying the conditional
sequence in a preventative manner. Whereas power-assertive tactics can be
used to enforce cooperation with verbal tactics, the conditional sequence
model suggests that verbal tactics can also be used to reduce the need for
power-assertive tactics. This was especially true of Baumrind’s (1971) har-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
monious parents, 5% of her sample in which the parents “had control but did
not exercise control . . . [but] they focused . . . upon developing principles for
resolving differences and for right living” (p. 99, italics in original). Although
authoritative parents were more likely to use negative sanctions when neces-
sary to achieve a desirable conclusion to a discipline episode, they preferred
using appropriate verbal disciplinary responses, which reduces the need for
power-assertive tactics. Similarly, maternal cooperation with 6- to 9-year-
olds’ desires predicted child cooperation in a cleanup task, but only for non
resistant children (Davidov & Grusec, 2006). Mothers’ accuracy in predicting
children’s evaluations of disciplinary responses predicted greater cooperation
in initially resistant children. This contrast fits well with Bell’s model in that
the most crucial parenting skills vary by stage in the control system process.
An Authoritative-Parenting Synthesis
for Responding to Misbehavior
In this chapter, we have used four sources to clarify the specific ways that
authoritative parents respond to misbehavior in young children: informative
studies by Baumrind, reasoning-focused research, consequence-focused research,
and research combining reasoning with consequences. Whereas authoritative
parents respond with reasoning but combine it with some power assertion when
warranted, the two major areas of scientific parenting research often emphasize
either reasoning or power assertion with little attention to an authoritative syn-
thesis of the two. Clarifying the specific ways that authoritative parents respond
to misbehavior requires combining the strengths of both perspectives.
Bell’s control system model explains why the two perspectives empha-
size distinct responses to misbehavior and how reasoning and power-assertive
tactics can support each other in a complementary fashion consistent with
authoritative parenting. Although it incorporates a sequence of power-assertive
consequences such as the ones used in behavior management, it shows that par-
ents can reduce the need for power assertion tactics by skillful use of the mild
verbal disciplinary responses emphasized in the reasoning-focused perspective.
outcomes (Grusec & Davidov, 2007; Grusec et al., 2000). Such clarification
would help them fit their disciplinary responses to the situation, another
characteristic of authoritative parenting (Smetana, 1995).
Authoritative parents also use power assertion more effectively than other
parents, but the crucial distinctions have not been clarified as well. Bell’s model
accounts for the difficulty of finding any correlational research to support power
assertion, although there are some exceptions consistent with authoritative par-
enting. For example, two studies overcame the bias against corrective actions
and found that power-assertive tactics predicted self-regulation in rural Black
6- to 9-year-olds and competency in preschoolers but only when combined
with parental nurturance (Brody & Flor, 1998; Towe-Goodman & Teti, 2008,
respectively). The correlational bias against corrective actions explains why
child outcomes are similar for nonphysical punishments, physical punishment,
and professional interventions, raising doubts about differentiating effective
from counterproductive power assertion on the basis of the tactic used. How
and when disciplinary tactics are implemented seems to be a more important
distinction between effective and counterproductive kinds of power assertion.
Baumrind et al. (2010) investigated power-assertive distinctions directly
and found that verbal hostility, psychological control, severe physical punish-
ment, and arbitrary discipline were the most harmful kinds of power asser-
tion and represent four distinct ways that power assertion can be misused.
Authoritative parents used confrontive discipline without resorting to those
harmful kinds of power assertion. Based on its factor loadings, the confron-
tive discipline used by authoritative parents involved dealing directly with
misbehavior and insisting on compliance in response to child coercion or
defiance. Although authoritative parents preferred achieving those goals
with appropriate verbal tactics, they were willing to apply negative sanctions
unambivalently when warranted. Although Baumrind (1967) did not specify
the negative sanctions used, her authoritative parents probably varied their
sanctions to fit the situation. Consistent with the conditional sequence, they
reduced their spanking frequency by the time children were 8 years old to a
level slightly lower than most other parents, including those who had been
permissive parents during the preschool years.
tactics are used to support parents’ preference for using appropriate verbal dis-
ciplinary responses for misbehavior. Skillful use of a systematic sequence of
disciplinary consequences can be extended to support the broader range of
verbal disciplinary responses used by authoritative parents. It could be that
authoritative parents use a clear signal to distinguish give-and-take verbal
conflict resolutions from occasions when the parent is insisting on an accept-
able conclusion to the misbehavior episode. The single warning could serve
as this signal. Their ability to then use a version of the conditional sequence
to obtain an acceptable conclusion when desired would enhance their con-
fidence about getting appropriate cooperation when needed. As Bugental
(2009) showed, such parental confidence reduces the extent to which parents
resort to power-assertive tactics, which would free authoritative parents to
explore a wider range of verbal resolutions that fit the situation. Background
power assertion would cause the child to pay more attention to verbal reso-
lution attempts by the parent, further enhancing the parent’s effectiveness
(Hoffman, 2000).
Conclusion
References
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Multiple pathways to compliance: Mothers’ will-
ingness to cooperate and knowledge of their children’s reactions to discipline.
Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 705–708. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.705
Davies, G. R., McMahon, R. J., Flessati, E. W., & Tiedemann, G. L. (1984). Verbal
rationales and modeling as adjuncts to a parenting technique for child compli-
ance. Child Development, 55, 1290–1298. doi:10.2307/1129998
de Leeuw, R. N. H., Scholte, R. H. J., Sargent, J. D., Vermulst, A. A., & Engels,
R. C. M. E. (2010). Do interactions between personality and social-environmental
factors explain smoking development in adolescence? Journal of Family Psy-
chology, 24, 68–77. doi:10.1037/a0018182
Deptula, D. P., Henry, D. B., & Schoeny, M. E. (2010). How can parents make a dif-
ference? Longitudinal associations with adolescent sexual behavior. Journal of
Family Psychology, 24, 731–739. doi:10.1037/a0021760
Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., & Boggs, S. R. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treat-
ments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal of Clinical
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 215–237. doi:10.1080/15374410701820117
Forgatch, M. S., & Patterson, G. R. (2010). Parent Management Training—Oregon
Model: An intervention for antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In
J. R. Weisz & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and
adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 159–177). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., & Clements, M. (2009). Parent learning support and child
reading ability: A cross-lagged panel analysis for developmental transactions. In
A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and con-
texts shape each other (pp. 203–220). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association. doi:10.1037/11877-011
Gershoff, E. T., Grogan-Kaylor, A., Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Zelli, A., Deater-Deckard,
K., & Dodge, K. A. (2010). Parent discipline practices in an international sam-
ple: Associations with child behaviors and moderation by perceived normative-
ness. Child Development, 81, 487–502. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01409.x
Grolnick, W. S. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How well-meaning parenting
backfires. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grusec, J. E. (1997). A history of research on parenting strategies and children’s
internalization of values. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and
children’s internalization of values (pp. 3–22). New York, NY: Wiley.
Hembree-Kigin, T. L., & McNeil, C. B. (1995). Parent–child interaction therapy. New York,
NY: Plenum Press.
Hill, N. E., & Tyson, D. F. (2009). Parental involvement in middle school: A meta-
analytic assessment of the strategies that promote achievement. Developmental
Psychology, 45, 740–763. doi:10.1037/a0015362
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Holden, G. W. (1997). Parents and the dynamics of child rearing. Boulder, CO: Westview
Press.
Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E. P., Johnson, D. J., Stevenson, H. C., & Spicer, P.
(2006). Parents’ ethnic-racial socialization practices: A review of research and
directions for future study. Developmental Psychology, 42, 747–770. doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.42.5.747
Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., Filene, J. H., & Boyle, C. L. (2008). A meta-analytic
review of components associated with parent training program effectiveness.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 567–589. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-
9201-9
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with differ-
ent temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33,
228–240. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.228
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Joy, M. E. (2007). Children’s fearfulness as a modera-
tor of parenting in early socialization: Two longitudinal studies. Developmental
Psychology, 43, 222–237. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.222
Kuczynski, L. (1982). Intensity and orientation of reasoning: Motivational determi-
nants of children’s compliance to verbal rationales. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 34, 357–370. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90066-2
Kuczynski, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral conceptual frameworks for
understanding dynamics in parent–child relations. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Hand-
book of dynamics in parent–child relations (pp. 3–24). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Larzelere, R. E., Cox, R. B., Jr., & Smith, G. L. (2010). Do nonphysical punish-
ments reduce antisocial behavior more than spanking? A comparison using the
strongest previous causal evidence against spanking. BMC Pediatrics, 10(10).
doi:10.1186/1471-2431-10-10
10, 375–398.
Roberts, M. W., & Powers, S. W. (1990). Adjusting chair timeout enforcement pro-
cedures for oppositional children. Behavior Therapy, 21, 257–271. doi:10.1016/
S0005-7894(05)80329-6
Shaffer, D. R., & Kipp, K. (2007). Developmental psychology: Childhood & adolescence
(7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Advantage Books.
Smetana, J. G. (1995). Parenting styles and conceptions of parental authority during
adolescence. Child Development, 66, 299–316. doi:10.2307/1131579
Steinberg, L. (2004). The 10 basic principles of good parenting. New York, NY: Simon
& Schuster.
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, S. M.
(1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents
from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child
Development, 65, 754–770. doi:10.2307/1131416
Steiner, P. M., Cook, T. D., Shadish, W. R., & Clark, M. H. (2010). The importance
of covariate selection in controlling for selection bias in observational studies.
Psychological Methods, 15, 250–267. doi:10.1037/a0018719
Timmermans, M., van Lier, P. A. C., & Koot, H. M. (2009). Pathways of behavior
problems from childhood to late adolescence leading to delinquency and under-
achievement. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 630–638.
doi:10.1080/15374410903103502
Towe-Goodman, N. R., & Teti, D. M. (2008). Power assertive discipline, maternal
emotional involvement, and child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 22,
648–651. doi:10.1037/a0012661
Van Houten, R. (1983). Punishment: From the animal laboratory to the applied
setting. In S. Axelrod & J. Apsche (Eds.), The effects of punishment on human
behavior (pp. 13–44). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Wahler, R. G. (1969). Oppositional children: A quest for parental reinforce-
ment control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 159–170. doi:10.1901/
jaba.1969.2-159
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1979). Child rearing and children’s
prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319–330.
doi:10.2307/1129406