Chelkowska-Zacharewicz Music Emotions and Associations PDF
Chelkowska-Zacharewicz Music Emotions and Associations PDF
MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ
MATEUSZ PALIGA
University of Silesia in Katowice, Institute of Psychology, Poland
The aim of the present study was to investigate which musical emotions and associations appeared
while listening to leitmotifs in film music. A sample of 157 participants took part in the study, in
which musical associations and emotions were analysed in relation to seven groups of musical
themes from The Lord of the Rings films (LOTR). The LOTR soundtrack is a good example of the
use of symbols in music to represent ideas, characters, etc. The results show that both the respon-
dents’ associations and the musical emotions they experienced were related to the characteristics of
musical motifs. The results are discussed in relation to the assumed features of musical motifs
composed by Howard Shore.
Keywords: film music; leitmotif; music-induced emotions; affect; The Lord of the Rings.
INTRODUCTION
symbols encompassed in music are strongly related to certain emotions (Juslin &
Sloboda, 2013). The emotional aspect of music seems indisputable. Music has
been used to induce moods (Västfjäll, 2001), express emotions (Juslin, 2013a),
and regulate emotional state (Saarikallio, 2011).
Research on music emotions has a strong and broad tradition (Juslin &
Sloboda, 2010), which has led to numerous studies in the last 20 years.
Simultaneously with music emotion research, attention was devoted to film
music, a fine example of emotionally strong music genre. Although the analysis
of the emotional aspect of film music was somehow neglected, it has been
implied that this genre is supposed to evoke strong emotions. The available
literature attests to an interest in how music influences the perception of movies
or the absorption of the viewer, etc. (for more, see: Cohen, 2010, 2014) rather
than in what kind of emotions are felt when listening to film music.
The assumption that music has a strong effect on film perception is the basic
reason for the presence of music motifs in movies (Green, 2010). However, to
our knowledge, it has scarcely been tested in music emotion research if certain
emotions indeed appear in listeners of film music themes.
Eerola, 2014). Brain stem reflex, rhythmic entrainment, and emotional contagion
relate to reactions that occur with low cultural influence (Juslin, 2013b). By con-
trast, visual imagery, episodic memory, and evaluative conditioning involve ele-
ments that are not directly related to music, such as associations, past events, or
expectations that are results of subjects’ knowledge and experience and are there-
fore strongly influenced by culture (Juslin, 2013b). Musical expectancy and aes-
thetic judgement are also related to subjects’ knowledge and cultural background.
An indication that associations may be a possible mediator in the induction
of emotional reactions by music (Konečni, 2008) and that they may be respon-
sible for interpersonal differences in the emotional experience of music appears
not only in the BRECVEMA model. Personal stories including a situational con-
text, memories, and knowledge may be triggered by music and lead to different
emotional reactions that change according to these factors (Gabrielsson, 2010).
Leitmotif
a gesture or sound) and the signified (an image or concept the signifier refers to).
Among six types of signs, namely: symptoms, signals, icons, indexes, names,
and symbols, the last one is of particular interest in explaining the concept of
musical leitmotif. A symbol is a sign that stands for its referent in an arbitrary,
conventional way. Many signifiers can be symbolic—words, objects, figures, or
sounds, to name a few (Sebeok, 2001). Music, including film music, can also
serve as a symbol, and the musical leitmotif seems to be a paramount example.
The primary role of a leitmotif is, in essence, to stand for the character, object,
or place it represents (i.e., a sign function). A leitmotif is therefore established by
the composer through signifier–signified proximity (Chattah, 2006).
Leitmotif (Ger. “leading motif”) is a recurring short musical theme (Green,
2010) or idea (Hanning, 2002; Whittall, 2001). As a music-based signifier, it can
be identified as a simple melody, usually only a few measures in length (Green,
2010; Nagari, 2015). It refers to a signified, which can be a person, an object,
a place, idea, a state of mind, a supernatural force, or any other ingredient in
a dramatic work, including an emotion, plot development, or recurring dramatic
themes (DePree, 2017; Provenzano, 2008). Musical leitmotifs that signify
symbolic reality, such as certain characters, places, ideas, etc., subsequently
appear every time that the symbolic reality is shown or is about to be shown on
the screen (Walus, 2012). To establish the connection of a leitmotif with its
referent, the former needs to be repeated a few times for the audience to
understand the association (Green, 2010). Once the basic identifications are
made, leitmotifs can undergo various changes, including rhythmic, intervallic, or
harmonic changes, due to the development of storyline or characters in the film
(Hanning, 2002; Walus, 2012). However, the changes cannot be major ones, so
that leitmotifs retain their identity (Vilaro & Orero, 2013), remain recognizable
for the audience (Green, 2010), and play particular roles in the films they are part
of. Musical leitmotifs provide the signified objects with a new narrative
dimension (Otero, 2015), by using music in the objects’ stories (Nagari, 2015).
Serving as a signifier of changes happening to the character, the motif reappears
throughout the film, generating meaning and evoking emotions. Being a musical
representation of a character or idea, a leitmotif can symbolize that character
even if he or she appears in disguise (Green, 2010). Finally, a leitmotif can work
as a metonymy, having the power to fully replace the element it signifies (Vilaro
& Orero, 2013) if the signified is absent in the visuals or dialogue (Chattah,
2006). The overall significance of leitmotifs was noted by Richards (2016, p. 1),
who pointed out that musical themes “can become part of an entire film’s
156 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
identity, to the point where they are often referred to as ‘the theme from’ the film
in question.”
The purpose of this study was to analyze the emotions and associations that
appear while listening to film music, with motifs from the LOTR soundtrack as
the music material. Although there is some literature on leitmotifs and their
function in film and dramatic music, film music studies relate more broadly to
the functions of film music as such (Cohen, 2010, 2014). To our knowledge,
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 157
there has been little focus in research on how emotions and associations are
related to leitmotifs. Therefore, this study is an exploratory one.
Leitmotifs in the LOTR soundtrack can be organized into groups that relate
to certain characters or contexts. For example, there is a group of musical motifs
connected with the artefact called the ring of power, or with the fictional race of
hobbits (Tolkien, 2002). Each motif may be recognized as a symbol of some
ideas, features, and emotions. The first aim of this study was to find out what
kind of music-induced emotions emerge when listening to motifs from each
group. To address this problem, the first question was formulated: Q1: What
music emotions are felt when listening to groups of motifs from the LOTR
soundtrack?
The musical motif groups mentioned above are different in their character
and in the message that they are supposed to deliver. These differences can be
observed in music structure (Adams, 2010; Marchwica, 2018) and in the inter-
pretation of music itself (see Table 1). However, there remains the question of
whether these differences can translate into feelings that appear while listening
to music. The second question concerned this matter and read as follows: Q2: Do
the music-induced emotions that appear while listening to musical motif groups
differ across these groups?
To deepen this exploratory study, our aim was also to analyze what kind of
thoughts, ideas, and associations appear when listening to the music themes from
the motif groups. In order to supplement the analysis of emotional reactions to
film music and to investigate if Shore’s leitmotifs are interpreted in accordance
with his intention, we formulated the third question: Q3: What associations occur
when listening to the motif groups from the LOTR soundtrack?
METHOD
The study was conducted via Concerto v4, a platform for online testing. The
participants were recruited via convenience sampling from February to August
2017. The link to the study website was distributed among the LOTR forum
users, on Facebook, and on the website of a Polish film music radio RMF
Classic. The survey started with a consent form, followed by the explanation of
the study timeline. Each subject: (1) listened to one randomly chosen musical
motif from the LOTR for 24 to 59 seconds (the listening time depended on the
158 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
musical theme); (2) assessed the emotions felt while the music was being played
in a loop for the second time; (3) responded to the question about associations
while the music was being played in a loop for the third time; and (4) filled the
sociodemographic data and familiarity with the LOTR music. Stages 2 and 3
were repeated twice, which means each participant listened to two random music
pieces. However, some of the participants chose to listen to only one music
excerpt.
The participants in the study were 95 women, 31 men, and 31 subjects who
did not indicate their sex; they ranged in age from 16 to 56 (M = 26.83, SD =
= 6.61). Familiarity with the LOTR films was average in the sample (Me = 5.00
on a scale of 1 to 10) and familiarity with the LOTR soundtrack was low (Me =
= 2.00 on a scale of 1 to 10). The participants provided 219 to 230 answers on
felt emotions and 203 sets of answers on associations.
1 The Being Moved (Pol. Wzruszenie) scale consists of items related to tearfulness in GEMS,
and might be interpreted in GEMS-PL as a feeling similar to but not the same as being
moved / touched.
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 159
from .75 to .92. The respondent evaluates the intensity of feelings when listening
to a music excerpt on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very strongly).
Measurement of associations. We measured the associations occurring
while listening to the LOTR music motifs by asking the participants an open-
ended question: “What comes to your mind when you are listening to this music
fragment? What associations do you have with it?”. No specific number of
associations was required.
Musical excerpts. The musical excerpts contained audio material only and
were cut out of the original the LOTR soundtrack in Audacity 2.1.2 software.
The length of each piece varied from 24 to 59 seconds (see Table 1) depending
on the motif length. To assure that the most important symbols of the LOTR
story were represented, we chose seven groups according to the analysis
presented by Adams (2010) with a total of 15 the LOTR musical motifs as their
representations (see Table 1). Table 1 includes the information about musical
symbols and the symbolized reality represented in each group, which was used in
further analysis to compare the target emotions and associations with the actual
music-induced emotions and associations that occurred when listening to LOTR
music motifs.
Table 1. Musical Motif Groups With Encompassing Musical Motifs, Their Music Symbols, and
Symbolized Reality
Musical motifs
No. Motif group Music symbols (sign) Symbolized reality (referent)
(Adams, 2010)
1 Good / Nature Nature’s Reclamation —soprano theme; rising —power, purity
(38) (28’’, 17), The White melody —hope of the good
Rider (27’’, 21) —clear orchestral colors
(Adams, 2010); links with
Nature’s Reclamation theme
2 Evil (43) Orc Theme (24’’, 18), —harsh; low brass melody —the instruments used by
The Five Beat Pattern (Young, 2007) Orcs (Young, 2007)
(37’’, 8), Ring Wrights —strong metric and rhyth- —the power and terror of
(59’’, 17) mic patterns; low register; those who want to destroy
lower brass instruments the world
(Marchwica, 2018) —fear-evoking
—percussion instruments —the threat of incoming war
(anvils, metal bell plates, —dread and malice (Adams,
bass drum etc.; Adams, 2010; Reymore, 2018)
2010)
—choral, mono-rhythmic,
contourless in structure
(Adams, 2010)
—falling and rising pitch
contour (Reymore, 2018)
160 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
3 The Ring The Evil of the Ring —Eastern-tinted —aesthetics of prior age
(26) (36’’, 12), History of harmonics, augmented (Adams, 2010)
the Ring (49’’, 14) second (Adames, 2010) —lore and lie that is the
—Moroccan double reed Ring’s nature, villainous
instrument (Adams, 2010) inclinations represented in
the Ring (Adams, 2010)
4 Hobbits (42) Shire Pensive Setting —folk leading instruments —home recollection; folksy,
(24’’, 22), Shire Rural (the fiddle, the whistle, the serene life (Adams, 2010),
Setting (36’’, 20) flute, strings, or wood- —interest in worldly goods:
winds) food and drink; safety and
—light orchestration creat- friendship (Tolkien, 2002)
ing simple, dance-like —unconstrained joy,
melodies in major mode carefree fun (Marchwica,
(Marchwica, 2018; Young, 2018)
2007)
5 Men (32) The Rohan Fanfare —French horns (Young, —the former glory and the
(48’’, 15), The Realm 2007) coming of the re-established
of Gondor (41’’, 17) —brass instruments kingdom
(Adams, 2010) —Tolkien’s description of
—Nordic influences (i.e., the blowing of the horns of
Hardanger fiddle) in the Rohan (Adams, 2010)
Rohan Fanfare (Adams,
2010)
7 The The Fellowship of the —melody in minor mode —the stoutheartedness and
Fellowship Ring (30’’, 14) —harmonization in major kindness of the members of
(14) mode the Fellowship
—orchestration that —heroic and triumphant
evolves along with the plot (DePree, 2017)
(Adams, 2010)
Note. The numbers in parentheses indicate (1) the frequency of occurrence of a given musical motif in the study
and (2) the length of the music piece (in seconds) encompassing the motif.
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 161
RESULTS
Each question was analyzed separately. The first and second questions are
analyzed in the Felt music-induced emotions section, and the third question is
analyzed in the Associations section.
Figure 1. Box plots with median and 5-95 percentiles for GEMS-PL in 7 motif groups.
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 163
Table 2. Results of the Kruskal-Wallis Rank Test for Seven Motif Groups
Mean ranks
H
(df = 6) Good / The
Elves Hobbits Ring Men Evil
Nature Fellowship
Joyful Activation 41.65*** 115.89 73.88 154.49 84.72 96.61 97.37 139.00
(N = 37) (N = 32) (N = 39) (N = 25) (N = 31) (N = 38) (N = 14)
c, g b, d, e, f c c c b
Being Moved 17.29** 100.01 118.88 106.03 124.50 122.40 78.68 109.96
(N = 36) (N = 32) (N = 38) (N = 25) (N = 31) (N = 37) (N = 14)
Column name A B C D E F G
Associations
(2) discussing any differences between the created categories and the assignment
of responses, and (3) creating the final categories and establishing response
assignments upon mutual agreement. The categories encompass associations
with films (LOTR, The Hobbit, and others; see Table A1 in Appendix) and non-
-film associations (see Table A2 in Appendix).
Out of 56 associations with films, the most popular ones were those with the
LOTR trilogy (Table A1 in Appendix). The association appeared mostly in
the Hobbits theme group. The category “other movies” included both general
answers such as “movie about the Middle Ages,” “Irish movie,” “historical film,”
or “a good movie,” and specific responses with titles such as Braveheart,
Pocahontas, Avatar, or Star Trek. The third group of film associations were those
with The Hobbit movie.
The 332 non-film associations reported by the participants (Table A2 in
Appendix) included ideas (131 answers, 39%), objects (99 answers, 30%),
emotions (52 answers, 16%), states of mind (27 answers, 8%), space or place
(14 answers, 4%), supernatural elements (6 answers, 2%), and characters
(3 answers, 1%). Table A2 in Appendix presents all general associations (ideas,
objects, states of mind, emotions, space, characters, and supernatural elements)
and the respective specific associations (i.e., victory, nature, hope, and positive
emotions), as well as subjects’ sample answers.
Some differences could be observed among participants presented with
music themes from different groups. Subjects listening to Good/Nature themes
were likely to report associations with ideas (28 answers, 55%), objects (10 an-
swers, 20%), states of mind (5 answers, 10%), emotions (5 answers, 10%), and
space (3 answers, 6%). Listening to Elves theme music came with connotations
of ideas (27 answers, 44%), objects (12 answers, 20%), emotions (7 answers,
11%), space (5 answers, 8%), supernatural elements (5 answers, 8%), characters
(3 answers, 5%), and state of mind (2 answers, 3%). In the case of Hobbits music
themes, the most frequent associations were objects (21 answers, 45%) and
emotions (16 answers, 34%). The Ring themes were mostly associated with ideas
(16 answers, 41%) and emotions (9 answers, 23%). Evil music themes were
generally associated with ideas (20 answers, 34%), objects (24 answers, 41%),
and emotions (10 answers, 17%). For the themes of Men, the most frequent
associations were ideas (23 answers, 40%) and objects (21 answers, 37%).
The Fellowship of the Ring motifs correlated mostly with thoughts of ideas
(13 answers, 68%).
166 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
DISCUSSION
They were in line with Shore’s idea of musically illustrating the Ring’s
mysterious character, the ability to control minds of others, its association with
evil (Young, 2007), and villainous inclinations (Adams, 2010). However, the
listeners’ music emotions were somehow contradictory to these associations, as
their Sadness score was higher and the Power score was lower than in other
music groups. Power in GEMS (Zentner et al., 2008) relates to victory and
triumph, while in the Ring motifs the power was rather related to danger and evil
strength (Adams, 2010). Therefore, another possible explanation involves
sadness as the listener’s complementary reaction to the power perceived in music
rather than as a reflection of his or her feelings of power.
While listening to the Elves motifs, participants indicated that they thought
of ideas such as mystery, majesty, beauty, and good, objects connected with
nature, mostly positive emotions, space and places such as a castle, and super-
natural elements of magic and fantasy (see Table A2 in Appendix). Participants
also explicitly reported an association with the elf characters. These unique
associations, different than others because they involve supernatural elements
and the notion of elves, reflect Shore’s idea of musically painting the unreal,
dehumanized elvish world, beyond time and reality (Marchwica, 2018). At the
same time, there were strong emotional reactions in the case of Transcendence
and weak ones in the case of Joyful Activation, which is consistent with the
characteristics of elves, bringing to mind nostalgia, threat, and the mystery of the
wise and dignified race from Tolkien’s Middle-earth (Adams, 2010).
The last three groups scored similarly high on Power compared to other
groups. Men, Fellowship, and Good/Nature music listeners did not obtain other
strong or distinctive results regarding music-induced emotions. Although in all
these groups there appeared thoughts connected with victory, the subjects asso-
ciated music motifs from these groups differently. The listeners of Good/Nature
motifs reported associations with ideas such as victory, majesty, and power, as
well as objects connected with nature. They also thought of space, hope, and
positive emotions (see Table A2 in Appendix). Such associations are in line with
the plot of the LOTR, where Eagles or Ents join the fight in the battle scenes
(Adams, 2010) and help to eventually defeat the evil (Tolkien, 2002).
Participants listening to the Men group of motifs indicated thoughts such as
ideas of victory and journey, objects connected with nature and battle, and states
of mind such as reflection and positive emotions (see Table A2 in Appendix).
The results show strong relations to the intentions of the race of Men in The Lord
of the Rings (Tolkien, 2002) and to their deeds that led to the post-battle re-estab-
168 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
The sample in the study was not very large, and the study design did not
allow us to ensure an equal number of participants in all motif groups. As the
music was selected randomly by the software, different motif groups were
presented unevenly, the number of listeners in a group ranging from 14 to 42.
The results are therefore limited and should be interpreted carefully; further
analysis is needed, with a larger sample and an equal count of participants in
motif groups.
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 169
For the purpose of the study, we included the rating of familiarity with the
LOTR music and movies. The question about the familiarity was asked at the
very end of the procedure in order to avoid bias among participants. Familiarity
level was average in the case of the movies and low in the case of the music.
These results, along with the fact that the music evoked relatively few
associations strictly related to the LOTR movies, allowed us to assume that the
results were not mainly influenced by the participants’ knowledge of the plot.
Nevertheless, the results for emotion scales as well as for associations may have
been biased by this knowledge and should be interpreted with caution. Therefore,
future research should control for the familiarity of the LOTR movies.
Unfortunately, due to the small sample size, this kind of analysis would not be
robust for the data set collected in the present study.
REFERENCES
Adams, D. (2010). The music of The Lord of the Rings films. Van Nuys: Alfred Publisher.
Bolivar, V. J., Cohen, A. J., & Fentress, J. C. (1994). Semantic and formal congruency in music and
motion pictures: Effects on the interpretation of visual action. Psychomusicology, 13, 28–59.
Boltz, M. G. (2001). Musical soundtracks as a schematic influence on the cognitive processing of
filmed events. Music Perception, 18, 427–454.
Boltz, M. G. (2004). The cognitive processing of film and musical soundtracks. Memory &
Cognition, 32, 1194–1205.
Boltz, M. G., Schulkind, M., & Kantra, S. (1991). Effects of background music on the
remembering of filmed events. Memory & Cognition, 19, 593–606.
Bordwell, D., & Thompson, K. (2008) Film art: An introduction (8th ed.). Boston, MA, US:
McGraw Hill.
Chattah, J. (2006). Semiotics, pragmatics, and metaphor in film music analysis. Retrieved from
http://purl.flvc.org/fsu/fd/FSU_migr_etd-3874
Chełkowska-Zacharewicz, M. (2017). Reagowanie na muzykę—afektywne podstawy i rola
kontekstu poznawczego [Reacting to music: The affective basis and the role of the cognitive
context] (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Silesia, Katowice, Poland.
Chełkowska-Zacharewicz, M., & Janowski, M. (in review). Polish adaptation of Geneva Emo-
tional Music Scale (GEMS) – factor structure and reliability.
Chełkowska-Zacharewicz, M., & Janowski, M. (2016). Polska adaptacja Geneva Emotional Music
Scale (GEMS) – badania wstępne. Polskie Pismo Muzykoterapeutyczne, 3, 44–63.
Cohen, A. J. (1993). Associationism and musical soundtrack phenomena. Contemporary Music
Review, 9, 163–178.
Cohen, A. J. (1998). The functions of music in multimedia: A cognitive approach. In Proceedings
of the Fifth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 13–20). Seoul:
Western Music Research Institute, Seoul National University.
170 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
Cohen, A. J. (2010). Music as a source of emotion in film. In J. A. Sloboda & P. N. Juslin (Eds.),
Handbook of music and emotion. Theory, research, application (pp. 879–908). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Cohen, A. J. (2014). Film music from the perspective of cognitive science. In D. Neumeyer (Ed.),
The Oxford handbook of film music studies (pp. 96–130). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Coutinho, E., & Scherer, K. R. (2017). Introducing the Geneva Music-Induced Affect Checklist
(GEMIAC): A brief instrument for the rapid assessment of musically induced emotions. Music
Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 34(4), 371–386.
Davies, S. (2010). Emotions expressed and aroused by music. Philosophical perspectives. In
P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research,
applications (pp. 15–43). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
DePree, K. M. (2017). Cue the music: Music in movies. The Research and Scholarship
Symposium, 5, 1–25.
Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same or different? Musicae Scientiae,
Special Issue, 123–147.
Gabrielsson, A. (2010). Strong experiences with music. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.),
Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 547–574). New York,
NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Green, J. (2010). Understanding the score: Film music communicating to and influencing the
audience. The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 44(4), 81–94.
Hagen, E. (1971). Scoring for films: A complete text (vol. 1). New York, NY, US: Criterion Music
Corp.
Hanning, B. (2002). Concise history of western music. New York, NY, US: W. W. Norton & Co.
Huron, D. (2007). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge, MA,
US: The MIT Press.
Juslin, P. N. (2013a). What does music express? Basic emotions and beyond. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 596.
Juslin, P. N. (2013b). From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: Towards a unified theory of
musical emotions. Physics of Life Reviews, 10(3), 235–266.
Juslin, P. N., Harmat, L., & Eerola, T. (2014). What makes music emotionally significant?
Exploring the underlying mechanisms. Psychology of Music, 42(4), 599–623.
Juslin, P. N., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., & Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling
study of emotional reactions to music: Listener, music, and situation. Emotion, 8(5), 668–683.
Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (2010). Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, and
applications. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (2013). Music and emotion. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of
music (pp. 583–645). San Diego, CA, US: Elsevier Academic Press.
Juslin, P. N., & Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider
underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 559–575; discussion 575–621.
Kallinen, K., & Ravaja, N. (2006). Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same and different.
Musicae Scientiae, 10(2), 191–213.
Konečni, V. J. (2008). A skeptical position on “musical emotions” and an alternative proposal.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 582–584.
Marchwica, W. M. (2018). Musical scenery: Utopia vs. arcadia in The Lord of the Rings (dir. Peter
Jackson). Musica Iagellonica, 9, 127–142.
Marshall, S. K., & Cohen, A. J. (1988). Effects of musical soundtracks on attitudes toward
animated geometric figures. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 95–112.
EMOTIONS IN FILM MUSIC LISTENING 171
Meyer, L. B. (1974). Emocja i znaczenie w muzyce [Emotion and meaning in music]. Kraków:
Polskie Wydawnictwo Muzyczne.
Nagari, B. (2015). Music as image: Analytical psychology and music in film. New York, NY, US:
Routledge.
Neumeyer, D. (Ed.) (2013). The Oxford handbook of film music studies. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Otero, T. P. (2015). Composition, variation and functions of the leitmotiv in the universe Indiana
Jones. Revista de Comunicación de la SEECI, 19(37), 238–267.
Prendergast, R. M. (1992). Film music. A neglected art: A critical study of film music in films. New
York, NY, US: W. W. Norton & Company.
Provenzano, C. (2008). Towards an aesthetic of film music: Musicology meets the film soundtrack.
Music Reference Services Quarterly, 10, 79–94.
Reymore, L. (2018). Musical expression and embodiment: Fear, threat, and danger in the music
of The Lord of the Rings. In R. Parncutt & S. Sattmann (Eds.), Proceedings of ICMPC15/
ESCOM10. Graz, Austria: Centre for Systematic Musicology, University of Graz.
Richards, M. (2016). Film music themes: Analysis and corpus study. Music Theory Online
– A Journal of the Society for Music Theory, 22(1), 1–27.
Saarikallio, S. (2011). Music as emotional self-regulation throughout adulthood. Psychology of
Music, 39(3), 307–327.
Sebeok, T. (2001). Signs: An introduction to semiotics (2nd ed.). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Tan, S. L., Spackman, M. P., & Bezdek, M. A. (2007). Viewers’ interpretation of film characters’
emotions: Effect of presenting film music before and after a character is shown. Music
Perception, 25(2), 135–152.
Tan, S. L., Spackman, M. P., & Wakefield, E. M. (2017). The effects of diegetic and nondiegetic
music on viewers’ interpretation of a film scene. Music Perception, 34(5), 135–152.
Thayer, J., & Levenson, R. (1983). Effects of music on psychophysiological responses to
a stressful film. Psychomusicology, 3, 44–54.
Tolkien, J. R. R. (2002). Władca Pierścieni. T. 1: Drużyna Pierścienia [The Lord of the Rings.
Vol. 1: The Fellowship of the Ring] (Maria Skibniewska, Trans.). Warsaw: Warszawskie
Wydawnictwo Literackie MUZA SA.
Västfjäll, D. (2001). Emotion induction through music: A review of the musical mood induction
procedure. Musicae Scientiae, 5(1 suppl), 173–211.
Vilaro, A., & Orero, P. (2013). Leitmotif in audio description: Anchoring information to optimise
retrieval. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 3(5), 56–64.
Walus, B. P. (2012). A new modular approach to the composition of film music. Retrieved from
https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.au/dspace/bitstream/2440/84425/8/02wholev1.pdf.
White, D. (2016). Middle-earth music: The sonic inhabitation of a fantasy world. Participations:
Journal of Audience and Reception Studies, 13(1), 488–511.
Whittall, A. (2001). Leitmotif. In The new grove dictionary of music and musicians (2nd ed.,
vol. 14, pp. 527–530). London: Macmillan.
Young, M. D. (2007). Projecting Tolkien’s musical worlds: A study of musical affect in Howard
Shore’s soundtrack to Lord of the Rings. Saarbrücken: AV Akademikerverlag.
Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music:
Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8(4), 494–521.
172 MARIA CHEŁKOWSKA-ZACHAREWICZ, MATEUSZ PALIGA
APPENDIX
Motif group
Film
association Good / Fellowship
Elves Hobbits Ring Men Evil Total
Nature of the Ring
The Lord
2 4 10 4 04 5 3 32
of the Rings
Other
4 2 02 2 05 1 2 18
movies
The Hobbit 0 0 05 0 01 0 0 06
Total 6 6 17 6 10 6 5 56
Glory / victory
“victory”
(2; 7%)
Majesty (5; 19%) “sublimity,” “grandeur”
Power (3; 11%) “power,” “feeling of inner power”
Journey (1; 4%) “journey beyond the horizon”
Danger (3; 11%) “danger and threat”
Beauty (4; 15%) “beauty,” “beauty of the world”
Mystery (5; 19%) “mystery,” “something mysterious”
Idea (27; 44%) Good (4, 15%) “triumph of the good”
Elves (3; 5%) Elves (3; 100%) “Elf,” “elves”
“sailing on a ship in the far sea,”
Nature (9; 75%) “blooming flowers,” “green mountains,”
“flying birds”
Battle / battle
“battle with oneself”
Object (12; 20%) objects (3; 25%)
Reflection
“reflection that one needs to be strong”
(1; 50%)
State of mind Anticipation
“sort of anticipation”
(2; 3%) (1; 50%)
Positive emotion “happiness,” “endless happiness,”
(5; 71%) “excitement”
Emotion Negative emotion
“a sad moment”
(7; 11%) (2; 29%)