Zlib - Pub Psychology Express Biological Psychology
Zlib - Pub Psychology Express Biological Psychology
Psychology
guide series will help you to understand key
concepts quickly, revise effectively and make
sure your answers stand out.
‘A great little guide that gives you the main points and encourages
further reading . . . it’s concise, clear and open to a variety of abilities.’
Katie Towers, Psychology student at Anglia Ruskin
Biological Psychology
Prepare for exams and Q Sample question Essay
coursework using sample Critically evaluate the risks to development during the prenatal period
questions and assessment advice
Biological
FAS. The researchers were able to distinguish children with FAS from a control group
not exposed to alcohol prenatally with 92% accuracy. More importantly they were able
to distinguish children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure but without FAS and non-
exposed controls with 84.7% accuracy. Overall the neuropsychological test battery was
more successful at distinguishing the groups than IQ testing. Measures of executive
function and spatial processing were found to be especially sensitive to prenatal alcohol
exposure.
‘All of the revision material I need in one place – a must for psychology undergrads.’
Andrea Franklin, Psychology student at Anglia Ruskin University
‘Very useful, straight to the point and provides guidance to the student, while helping
them to develop independent learning.’
Lindsay Pitcher, Psychology student at Anglia Ruskin University
‘Engaging, interesting, comprehensive . . . it helps to guide understanding and boosts
confidence.’
Megan Munro, Forensic Psychology student at Leeds Trinity University College
‘Very useful . . . bridges the gap between Statistics textbooks and Statistics
workbooks.’
Chris Lynch, Psychology student at the University of Chester
‘The answer guidelines are brilliant, I wish I had had it last year.’
Tony Whalley, Psychology student at the University of Chester
‘I definitely would (buy a revision guide) as I like the structure, the assessment advice
and practice questions and would feel more confident knowing exactly what to revise
and having something to refer to.’
Steff Copestake, Psychology student at the University of Chester
‘The clarity is absolutely first rate . . . These chapters will be an excellent revision
guide for students as well as providing a good opportunity for novel forms of
assessment in and out of class.’
Dr Deaglan Page, Queen’s University, Belfast
‘Do you think they will help students when revising/working towards assessment?
Unreservedly, yes.’
Dr Mike Cox, Newcastle University
‘The revision guide should be very helpful to students preparing for their exams.’
Dr Kun Guo, University of Lincoln
‘A brilliant revision guide, very helpful for students of all levels.’
Svetoslav Georgiev, Psychology student at Anglia Ruskin University
Psychology
Biological
Psychology
Emma Preece
University of Worcester
Series editor:
Dominic Upton
University of Worcester
Pearson Education Limited
Edinburgh Gate
Harlow
Essex CM20 2JE
England
The right of Emma Preece to be identified as author of this Work has been asserted by her
in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval
system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying,
recording or otherwise, without either the prior written permission of the publisher or
a licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom issued by the Copyright
Licensing Agency Ltd, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS.
Pearson Education is not responsible for the content of third-party Internet sites.
ISBN 978-0-273-73722-3
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
15 14 13 12
Acknowledgements vii
Introduction ix
Guided tour xii
Guided tour of the companion website xiv
Key research studies xvi
v
[Page vi -- Post-TOC web box]
supporting resources
[COPY TO COME]
Visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress to find valuable
online resources.
For more information please contact your local Pearson Education sales
representative or visit www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
vi
acknowledgements
author’s acknowledgements
Thanks are due firstly to the editor Professor Dominic Upton for giving me the
opportunity to write this text. Thank you to Dr Jonathan Catling for his support
during the writing process and to Mr Lee Badham who designed most of the
artwork. Special thanks are also due to my family, Angela Huddart, Michelle
Hallard, Catherine Moreland, Charlotte Taylor, Helena Darby, Carole Hender,
Gemma Taylor, Tracey Price, Laura Scurlock-Evans, Emma Jackson, Chris
Leck, Daniel Kay and all other friends and colleagues for their support and
encouragement.
Publisher’s acknowledgements
Our thanks go to all the reviewers who contributed to the development of this
text, including students who participated in research and focus groups which
helped to shape the series format.
Dr Paul Hitchcott, Southampton Solent University
Dr Minna Lyons, Liverpool Hope University
Dr Deaglan Page, Queen’s University Belfast
Dr Jonathon Reay, Northumbria University
Dr Julia Robertson, Buckinghamshire New University
Dr Mark Scase, De Montfort University
Dr Sonia Tucci, University of Liverpool
Student reviewer:
Katie Towers, Psychology student at Anglia Ruskin University
vii
introduction
Not only is psychology one of the fastest growing subjects to study at university
worldwide, it is also one of the most exciting and relevant subjects. Over the
past decade the scope, breadth and importance of psychology have developed
considerably. Important research work from as far afield as the UK, Europe,
USA and Australia has demonstrated the exacting research base of the topic
and how this can be applied to all manner of everyday issues and concerns.
Being a student of psychology is an exciting experience – the study of mind and
behaviour is a fascinating journey of discovery. Studying psychology at degree
level brings with it new experiences, new skills and knowledge. As the Quality
Assurance Agency (QAA, 2010) has stressed:
psychology is distinctive in the rich and diverse range of attributes it develops – skills
which are associated with the humanities (e.g. critical thinking and essay writing) and
the sciences (hypotheses-testing and numeracy). (QAA, 2010, p. 5)
Recent evidence suggests that employers appreciate the skills and knowledge
of psychology graduates, but in order to reach this pinnacle you need to
develop your skills, further your knowledge and most of all successfully
complete your degree to your maximum ability. The skills, knowledge and
opportunities acquired during your psychology degree will give you an edge
in the employment field. The QAA stresses the high level of employment skills
developed during a psychology degree:
due to the wide range of generic skills, and the rigour with which they are taught,
training in psychology is widely accepted as providing an excellent preparation for many
careers. In addition to subject skills and knowledge, graduates also develop skills in
communication, numeracy, teamwork, critical thinking, computing, independent learning
and many others, all of which are highly valued by employers. (QAA, 2010, p. 2)
This book is part of the comprehensive new series, Psychology Express, that
helps you achieve these aspirations. It is not a replacement for every single
text, journal article, presentation and abstract you will read and review during
the course of your degree programme. It is in no way a replacement for your
lectures, seminars or additional reading. A top-rated assessment answer is likely
to include considerable additional information and wider reading – and you
are directed to some of these in this text. This revision guide is a conductor:
directing you through the maze of your degree by providing an overview of your
course, helping you formulate your ideas, and directing your reading.
Each book within Psychology Express presents a summary coverage of the
key concepts, theories and research in the field, within an explicit framework
of revision. The focus throughout all of the books in the series will be on how
you should approach and consider your topics in relation to assessment and
exams. Various features have been included to help you build up your skills and
ix
00 • Chapter title goes here
Introduction
knowledge ready for your assessments. More details of these features can be
found in the guided tour for this book on page xii.
By reading and engaging with this book, you will develop your skills and
knowledge base and in this way you should excel in your studies and your
associated assessments.
Psychology Express: Biological Psychology is divided into nine chapters and your
course has probably been divided up into similar sections. However we, the series
authors and editors, must stress a key point: do not let the purchase, reading and
engagement with the material in this text restrict your reading or your thinking. In
psychology, you need to be aware of the wider literature and how it interrelates
and how authors and thinkers have criticised and developed the arguments of
others. So even if an essay asks you about one particular topic you need to draw
on similar issues raised in other areas of psychology. There are, of course, some
similar themes that run throughout the material covered in this text, but you can
learn from the other areas of psychology covered in the other texts in this series
as well as from material presented elsewhere.
We hope you enjoy this text and the others in the Psychology Express series,
which cover the complete knowledge base of psychology:
MM Biological Psychology (Emma Preece): covering the biological basis
of behaviour, hormones and behaviour, sleeping and dreaming, and
psychological abnormalities.
MM Cognitive Psychology (Jonathan Ling and Jonathan Catling): including key
material on perception, learning, memory, thinking and language.
MM Developmental Psychology (Penney Upton): from pre-natal development
through to old age, the development of individuals is considered. Childhood,
adolescence and lifespan development are all covered.
MM Personality and Individual Differences (Laura Scurlock): normal and abnormal
personality, psychological testing, intelligence, emotion and motivation are all
covered in this book.
MM Social Psychology (Jenny Mercer and Debbie Clayton): covering all the key
topics in Social Psychology including attributions, attitudes, group relations,
close relationships and critical social psychology.
MM Statistics in Psychology (Catherine Steele, Holly Andrews and Dominic Upton):
an overview of data analysis related to psychology is presented along with
why we need statistics in psychology. Descriptive and inferential statistics and
both parametric and non-parametric analysis are included.
MM Research Methods in Psychology (Steve Jones and Mark Forshaw): research
design, experimental methods, discussion of qualitative and quantitative
methods and ethics are all presented in this text.
MM Conceptual and Historical Issues in Psychology (Brian M. Hughes): the
foundations of psychology and its development from a mere interest into a
scientific discipline. The key conceptual issues of current-day psychology are
also presented.
x
Introduction
This book, and the other companion volumes in this series, should cover all
your study needs (there will also be further guidance on the website). It will,
obviously, need to be supplemented with further reading and this text directs
you towards suitable sources. Hopefully, quite a bit of what you read here you
will already have come across and the text will act as a jolt to set your mind at
rest – you do know the material in depth. Overall, we hope that you find this
book useful and informative as a guide for both your study now and in your
future as a successful psychology graduate.
Revision note
MM Use evidence based on your reading, not on anecdotes or your ‘common
sense’.
MM Show the examiner you know your material in depth – use your additional
reading wisely.
MM Remember to draw on a number of different sources: there is rarely one
‘correct’ answer to any psychological problem.
MM Base your conclusions on research-based evidence.
xi
Guided tour
1 Introduction to
biopsychology: origins,
approaches and
applications
M Understand key concepts quickly
• Philosophy
The origins of
biopsychology • Medicine
• Technology
Introduction to
biopsychology:
origins,
approaches and
applications Start to plan your revision
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
using
The origins of biopsychology
• Physiological psychology and the Topic maps.
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming psychophysiology
• Neuroscience
Approaches in
Biological mechanisms controlling the timingThe biological psychology that we know today is the product of several
of sleep
• Neuropsychology
biopsychology
theoretical shifts, centuries of research and numerous methodological
• Psychopharmacology
The timing of sleep is controlled by an internal hypothetical
• Behavioural mechanism(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). Indeed, biopsychology has links to
genetics
adaptations
known as the circadian clock, which regulates •sleep–wake homeostasis, and
Comparative psychology
philosophy, science, medicine and technology and can be tracked back as far
zeitgebers, which reset this clock. This mechanism works in conjunction with
as ancient Greece and humorism (Burton, 1989/1994). These influences have
the neurotransmitter adenosine, the hormone melatonin and temperature
contributed towards the diversity of contemporary biopsychology. Indeed, the
fluctuations to stimulate the feeling of tiredness and determine the ideal time
origins and development of biopsychology are also intertwined with the other
for sleep. However, it is important to remember that in humans the amount and
schools of thought in psychology, such as cognitivism and behaviourism. As
timing of sleep can also be controlled by the individual to some extent and it is
such, it is impossible to fully summarise the complete history of biopsychology
subject to significant individual and group differences. For example, newborns
in this book, but links to further reading are provided at the end of this section.
tend to sleep the most while adults sleep the least.
A from
A printable version of this topic map is available brief review of some of the contributions which have been made towards the
Research has suggested that the regulation of circadian rhythms, including the
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress origin and development of biopsychology is provided below.
sleep–wake cycle, may be mediated by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), raphe
nucleus, pons and the locus coeruleus area of the reticular formation in the brain 1
stem. This suggests that these regions may correspond with the Key termsof the
location
Grasp Key terms quickly using
circadian clock. Indeed, Aston-Jones and Bloom (1981) observed increased
Humorism: An ancient school of thought concerned with the balance of the body’s
activity of noradrenergic neurons in this region three secondsnaturally before produced
animals substances: bile, blood and phlegm.
the handy definitions. Use
awoke. However, you should also remember that the hypothalamus has been
Cognitivism: A school of thought concerned with human cognition.
the flashcards online to test
associated with regulating sleep–wake cycles (e.g. Saper, Scammell & Lu, 2005).
Behaviourism: A school of thought concerned with purely observable and
yourself.
Key terms
measureable human and animal behaviour.
M revise effectively
according to a set pattern, such as the sleep–wake cycle.
several of the prominent and prevailing debates in psychology (Valentine,
Zeitgebers: Stimuli which can reset the circadian clock and circadian rhythms. These
usually come in the form of changing light.
1992). For example, the Critical Focus box below details how biopsychology is
routed in the mind–body problem. This has been a common theme for debates
since the origins of philosophy and features in religious scriptures that discuss
the soul. The debate concerns the relationship between the mind (or, more
KEY STUDY
generally, any incorporeal aspect) and the body. Contemporary biopsychology
The suprachiasmatic nucleus and circadian rhythms tends to adopt a dualist perspective, which argues that cognition and bodily
responses
Silver, LeSauter, Tresco and Lehman (1996) identified that surgical removal are both
of the SCNrelevant to human experience, rather than a monist
perspective,
of hamsters eradicated their circadian rhythms, suggesting that this which
structure may argues that there is only one reality and that is either physical
be the
locus of the circadian clock. Furthermore, when SCN tissue was (materialism)
transplanted or psychological
into the (idealism) in nature.
hamsters’ third ventricles using small semi-permeable capsules, the circadian rhythms
recommenced. However, the notable issue with this finding is that while chemicals and
Key terms Quickly remind yourself of
nutrients could pass through the capsule to replenish the SCN tissue, the tissue itself was
not able to establish synaptic connections to the surrounding tissue due to the nature the Key studies using the
Dualist/dualism: A perspective which states the mind and body both exist as
of the capsules. This suggests that the SCN may control circadian rhythms through
chemicals rather than electrical signals although the nature of these chemicals is yet to special boxes in the text.
separate entities which contribute toward the sense of reality.
Idealism: A perspective which states that the only reality is that created by the mind.
be determined.
t
106
xii
perception mismatch in tone-deafness. Current Biology, 18(8),
331–332. by cross-cultural studies?
7.21 Which hemisphere is more influential in the recognition of emotion?
Auditory perception Shamma, S. A., & Micheyl, C. (2010). Behind the scenes of auditory
perception. Current Opinions in Neurobiology,Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
20(3), 361–366.
Auditory perception
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2009). Auditory event files: Integrating
and action planning auditory perception and action planning. Attention, Perception and Guided tour
Psychophysics, 71(2), 352–362.
5.10 Where is the primary auditory cortex located? knowledge and Sample
¡¡Attempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
Answers to these questions can be found on the companionanswer
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
website at:
guidelines below.
question features.
¡¡Go to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards. You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Somatosensation
Answer guidelines
Somatosensation refers to bodily sensations and comprises three interacting
systems, which are the exteroceptive system (see previous section on the
Sample question
general properties of perception, sensation and attention), proprioceptive Essay
Compare your responses with
system and interoceptive system.
Compare and contrast two biological approaches to studying human emotion.
the Answer guidelines in the 91
text and on the website.
Approaching the question Examples in neuropsychology
Electroconvulsive shock
132
Patients who undergo electroconvulsive shock (ECS) for conditions such as
obsessive compulsive disorder and major depression experience a degree of
retrograde amnesia and a period of confusion following the treatment. Most
interestingly, ECS appears to be a useful technique for investigating the time
required for memory consolidation in that longer gradients of retrograde
amnesia would suggest that memory consolidation is a longitudinal process.
Chapter summary
This–appears
pulling it all
to together
be the case, suggesting that memories become gradually more
resilient to damage and interference (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Pinel, 2003).
Approaching the question
This question is asking you to evaluate the methodologies,Medial
insights,temporal
strengths amnesia
and limitations of evidence provided by studies of individuals with brain damage
As you will remember from the discussion of patient H.M., damage to the
or other cognitive impairments.
medial temporal area can result in severe anterograde amnesia, but this pattern
Important points to include of impairment is not all-encompassing and there are types of knowledge which
can still be acquired. Several studies with laboratory animals have demonstrated
Your essay should begin with a clear and concise summary of the arguments,
that bilateral surgical removal of the hippocampus and rhinal cortex can severely
evidence and interpretations you will discuss in your response to the question.
Important things to consider include: impair object recognition memory. For example, rats demonstrate severe deficits
provided by neuropsychology.
149
Guided tour of the companion website
xiv
Guided tour of the companion website
M revise effectively
xv
key research studies
MM Corkin, S., Sullivan, E. V., Twitchell, T. E., & Grove, E. (1981). The amnesic
patient H. M.: Clinical observations and a test performance 28 years after
operation. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 7, 235.
MM Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
MM Galton, F. (1865). Hereditary talent and character. Macmillan’s Magazine, 12,
157–166. Available: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Galton/talent.htm.
MM Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1977). Functional architecture of macaque
monkey visual cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 198, 1–59.
MM Lashley, K. S. (1930). Basic neural mechanisms in behaviour. Psychological
Review, 37 (1), 1–24. Available: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Lashley/neural.htm
MM Madsen, P. L., Holm, S., Vorstrup, S., Friberg, L., Lassen, N. A. & Wildschiodz, G.
(1991). Human regional cerebral blood flow during rapid-eye-movement sleep.
Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 11, 502–507.
MM Sperry, R. W. (1966). Brain bisection and consciousness. In J. Eccles (Ed.),
Brain and conscious experience. New York: Springer-Verlag.
MM Weiskrantz, I. (1987). Residual vision in the scotoma: A follow-up study of
‘form’ discrimination. Brain, 110, 77–92.
1 Introduction to
biopsychology: origins,
approaches and
applications
• Philosophy
The origins of
biopsychology • Medicine
• Technology
Introduction to
biopsychology:
origins,
approaches and
applications
1
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Introduction
This chapter will provide an overview of some of the main areas of biological
psychology and will draw your attention to research techniques, theories and
prominent research within these areas. Although you need to understand the
general origins, nature and scope of biological psychology it is also important
to remember that it consists of several significantly different approaches. These
include physiological psychology, neuropsychology, cognitive neuroscience,
behavioural genetics and psychopharmacology (see Figure 1.1).
Each of these approaches focuses on different levels of analysis and employs
different techniques and interpretations. However, you should also understand
that these approaches are applied and combined to provide several levels of
analysis when examining the biological aspects of behaviour and experience. It
is not enough to merely be descriptive and list a series of facts, techniques and
approaches; you must also think and write critically. Consequently, this chapter
will encourage you to compare and contrast these approaches and will highlight
some of the prevailing issues and debates which arise when studying human
behaviour and experience from a biological perspective.
By the end of this chapter you should feel more confident in your understanding
of the approaches, methodologies and applications of biological psychology
but you should also be able to discuss some of the similarities, differences,
strengths and limitations of these approaches.
Cognitive
neuroscience
Psychophysiological Behavioural
approach genetics
Physiological
approach Psychopharmacology
Comparative
Neuropsychology Biopsychology psychology
2
Introduction
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions for introductory biopsychology will usually ask you to compare
and contrast two or more approaches or research techniques with reference to a
specific phenomenon. For example, you may be asked to evaluate and compare
neuropsychological and physiological approaches to studying perception. For
questions like this you will need to understand the similarities, differences,
techniques and principles of these approaches. You will also need to cite specific
examples of these approaches in action.
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
3
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Key terms
Humorism: An ancient school of thought concerned with the balance of the body’s
naturally produced substances: bile, blood and phlegm.
Cognitivism: A school of thought concerned with human cognition.
Behaviourism: A school of thought concerned with purely observable and
measureable human and animal behaviour.
Philosophy
The mind–body problem
The philosophical origins of biopsychology are notably eclectic and incorporate
several of the prominent and prevailing debates in psychology (Valentine,
1992). For example, the Critical Focus box below details how biopsychology is
routed in the mind–body problem. This has been a common theme for debates
since the origins of philosophy and features in religious scriptures that discuss
the soul. The debate concerns the relationship between the mind (or, more
generally, any incorporeal aspect) and the body. Contemporary biopsychology
tends to adopt a dualist perspective, which argues that cognition and bodily
responses are both relevant to human experience, rather than a monist
perspective, which argues that there is only one reality and that is either physical
(materialism) or psychological (idealism) in nature.
Key terms
Dualist/dualism: A perspective which states the mind and body both exist as
separate entities which contribute toward the sense of reality.
Idealism: A perspective which states that the only reality is that created by the mind.
t
4
The origins of biopsychology
Materialism: A perspective which states that the only reality is that experienced by
the body.
Mind–body problem: A philosophical debate concerning the relationship between
and dominance of the mind and the body.
Monist/monism: A perspective which states that either the mind or the body exists
independently. There is only one reality and that may be through the mind (idealism)
or the body (materialism).
CrItICAl foCus
5
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Key terms
Evolutionary theories: Theories that behaviour and experience have developed
through centuries of genetic mutation, evolution and survival of the fittest.
functionalism: An approach which is concerned with identifying the functions which
behaviour and experience serve.
Introspection: A technique developed by Wundt (1902) to study the subjective
experience of patients based on their description of their thoughts and feelings.
structuralism: An approach which is concerned with studying how the structure and
organisation of the mind influence behaviour and experience.
Psychology as a science
You have probably already encountered the debate surrounding whether
psychology can be considered to be a science. This debate has been ongoing
since the emergence of early research and theorisation concerning the mind–
body debate and is likely to continue due to the permanently evolving status
of psychology (Hergenhahn, 2009; James, 1890; Valentine, 1992). For the
purpose of this chapter it is important to note that biopsychology possesses
more explicit links to science, medicine and technology than any other area of
psychology. Indeed, while its origins are linked to philosophy, its development
has been highly determined by scientific influences which are discussed
later in this section. However, it is also important that you remember that
biopsychology is also concerned with explaining phenomena which cannot
always be observed and quantified. Sciences such as physics and biology have
also experienced similar problems and technological advances have resolved
several of these limitations.
Nature–nurture debate
The nature–nurture debate permeated psychology and refers to whether
behaviour is determined by biological or environmental factors (Galton, 1865).
However, in most cases the extremist views which arose as a product of monism
have since been replaced by dualist perspectives (Valentine, 1992). Indeed, the
6
The origins of biopsychology
Key terms
Diathesis–stress: A theoretical model which states that behaviour and experience are
produced by both biological and environmental factors.
Nature–nurture: A theoretical debate surrounding the topic of whether an individual’s
behaviour and experience are determined and inevitable due to biological factors, or
undetermined and changeable due to environmental factors.
Neurotransmitter: A naturally occurring chemical produced in the body at the terminal
buttons of neurons which facilitates the transmission of action potentials across synaptic
gaps. The activation threshold and compatibility of the postsynaptic receptor cells will
determine their efficiency. Neurotransmitters can have excitatory or inhibitory effects.
Medicine
Biopsychology also has explicit bases and links to medicine because both
disciplines examine how biology influences behaviour. For example, in ancient
Greece, humorism stated that imbalance between the body’s humors (believed
to be yellow bile, black bile, blood and phlegm) could result in both physical
and mental illness (Burton, 1989/1994). Based on this assumption, techniques
such as bloodletting and trepanning (drilling holes in the skull) were used for
centuries to relieve imbalance and pressure respectively. Over the centuries
several other medically based approaches have been adopted in the study
of human experience and behaviour, including the use of medication to alter
physiological states and the use of cerebral lesions to treat psychological
impairments. While it is important to remember that many of these approaches
would be reprehensible today, the general principles remain prevalent in both
medicine and biopsychology. For example, cerebral lesions to areas of the brain
are still occasionally performed to reduce the symptoms of severe unipolar
depression and obsessive compulsive disorder. Therefore, it is important
to remember that both early and contemporary biological psychology is
concerned with how genetics, physiology and anatomy influence both normal
and maladaptive behaviour (Burton, 1989/1994; Carlson, 2004; Galton, 1865;
James, 1890; Lashley, 1930; Pinel, 2003). As such, the links between medicine
and psychology are most pronounced in this area and these links are further
strengthened by the contemporary reliance on technology.
7
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Key terms
Bloodletting: An ancient technique of releasing blood from the body in an attempt
to restore balance.
lesions: The severing of connections or damage to structures in the brain.
obsessive compulsive disorder: A psychological condition in which individuals
experience extreme anxiety, obsessive thoughts and compulsions to perform actions
which they believe relieve the anxiety.
trepanning: An ancient technique in which holes are drilled in the skull in an attempt
to relieve pressure.
unipolar depression: A psychological condition characterised by unusually low mood,
lethargy, negative thoughts and negative emotions.
Technology
Biopsychology is highly influenced by and often dependent on technological
advancement. Indeed, due to the association with medicine and the need to
understand how both normal and impaired functions arise it has often utilised
the tools of medicine. However, it is important to remember that technology is
constantly evolving and contemporary biopsychology is significantly different
from the rudimentary approaches discussed previously. For example, cognitive
neuroscience relies on complex neuroimaging technologies such as positron
emission tomography (PET) to understand which physiological processes and
anatomical structures are associated with human behaviour and experience. In
addition, neuropsychology adopts numerous technologies including conventional
experimentation using computers, laboratory equipment and neuroimaging.
These approaches are discussed in more detail in the following section, but it
is important that you remember the development of biopsychology is highly
dependent upon the tools and measures that are available.
Key term
Neuroimaging: A procedure in which neurological imaging technology is used to
visualise and record the activity of the brain.
8
Approaches in biopsychology
1.1 How has philosophy contributed towards the origins and development
of biopsychology?
1.2 How has medicine contributed towards the origins and development of
biopsychology?
1.3 To what extent has the emergence of biopsychology been subject to
the rate of technological advancement?
1.4 Does contemporary biopsychology still resemble early biological
psychology?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Approaches in biopsychology
9
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Key term
Converging operations: Combining two or more different approaches or techniques
to study the same phenomenon at different levels of analysis.
Key terms
Aphagia: A condition in which a neurologically damaged individual ceases to eat.
Physiological psychology: An approach in psychology which attempts to identify the
neural correlates of behaviour and experience, often in laboratory animals.
Psychophysiology: An approach in psychology which investigates the correspondence
between physiological activity, behaviour and experience in human subjects.
10
Approaches in biopsychology
Technique Description
Technique Description
Neuroscience
Cognitive neuroscience attempts to identify the neural correlates of cognition
using a combination of physiological measures (see Tables 1.1 and 1.2),
neuroimaging techniques (see Table 1.3), conventional laboratory experiments
11
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Technique Description
X-ray Contrast x-rays produce an image of the brain after a substance has been
injected into the respective area which absorbed x-rays more or less than the
surrounding tissue.
and information technology (Bear, Connors & Paradiso, 2007; Cabeza & Nyberg,
2000; Gazzaniga, 2003). It occasionally also employs computer models to
simulate the human brain. This facilitates practices and investigations which
would otherwise be unethical and inhuman. For example, a connectionist
(computational) network can be used to simulate the micro-structure of the
human brain and allows researchers to mimic learning processes and cognition
and to map the effects of brain damage. This is a useful technique which permits
researchers to investigate the nature of phenomena which occur infrequently
or those which could otherwise not be studied. However, criticisms of cognitive
neuroscience include:
MP Biological approaches are reductionist and as such reduce complex behaviour
to physiological responses.
MP Neuroimaging techniques are not always time-sensitive and elements of the
response can be lost in this interval.
MP While techniques are usually non-invasive, they may still cause distress and
this can influence physiology and confound results.
MP The brain is highly interconnected and as such any effect seen in one cerebral
region may have originated from or contributed towards the processing
of another. Hence, these regions may also be associated with the form of
processing which is being investigated.
12
Approaches in biopsychology
Key terms
Connectionist: A computational model used to simulate human performance and
neural activity.
Neuroscience: An approach in psychology which attempts to identify the neural
correlates of cognition using a combination of physiological, experimental and
computational measures.
reductionist/reductionism: Attempting to explain a higher order function based on
lower order processes.
You aim to investigate which areas of the brain are associated with human
memory. How would you go about designing and conducting an experiment
in neuroscience? You will need to consider who your participants will be,
which technique you would need to use, what measures you would obtain
and any strengths and weaknesses of your approach.
Neuropsychology
Researchers in neuropsychology attempt to identify how cerebral structures
contribute towards cognitive processing by studying what happens when the
cerebral region has been damaged. In other words, it attempts to identify
how cerebral structures influence both normal and impaired functioning.
Neuropsychology primarily draws upon case studies of individuals who have
13
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Key terms
Aphasia: A deficit in language usually produced through brain damage.
Double dissociation: Observed when one brain-damaged patient shows one pattern
of impairment while another shows a different pattern of impairment. Potentially due
to the damage of different cerebral structures.
Neuropsychology: An approach in psychology which attempts to identify both normal
and impaired human function, usually through studying the effects of brain damage.
14
Approaches in biopsychology
Technique Description
Case study The detailed study of an individual who has experienced brain
damage or infection, resulting in deficits for memory, perception,
attention etc. Tends to be longitudinal and can extend to how
well the brain can demonstrate reorganisation and plasticity
during the period of rehabilitation.
Cohort study The study of groups of individuals who are matched on all other
possible factors except the variable under investigation.
Psychopharmacology
Psychopharmacology investigates the effects of drugs on physiological activity,
behaviour and experience (Coull, 1998; Meyer & Quenzer, 2004; Vitiello, 2007).
The majority of research in this field is conducted in medicine with an applied
focus of improving quality of life and reducing maladaptive behaviours. For
example, psychopharmacology is prevalent in abnormal psychology which will
be discussed in more detail in Chapter 9. Medications can be used to reduce the
severity of symptoms in cases of anxiety, depression and schizophrenia, and also
in some personality disorders. Trials in psychopharmacology attempt to identify
the beneficial and potentially harmful effects of substances, the best method
for administration, drug metabolism, correct doses and suitable applications.
However, the regulation and testing of psychopharmacology products continue
after the trials and previously unobserved side-effects are regularly reported
15
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
even after the use of the drug has been approved. Clinical trials are highly
regulated and are usually longitudinal. As such, research normally begins in a
laboratory setting with cultures of tissue and laboratory animals and will only
proceed to human volunteers if maximum safety has been ensured. However,
occasionally errors do occur and can have disastrous consequences.
Some of the general limitations of psychopharmacology include:
MP It is an expensive procedure which requires numerous experts, funding
bodies, ethics committees and organisations.
MP It is a time-consuming process which can span several years.
MP Significant ethical issues arise both in the use of animals and in administering
drugs to humans in clinical trials.
MP Many medications have serious side-effects and restrictions of use.
MP Several medications were designed for use in one specific condition and
the administration in other cases is often explorative. Discoveries that a
medication reduces symptoms in other disorders are often serendipitous.
Key terms
Anxiety disorder: A psychological condition characterised by high levels of stress and
anxiety, usually elicited by an external stimuli but also influenced by internal processes.
Clinical trial: The procedure by which medications are tested and legalised.
Psychopharmacology: An approach in psychology which is concerned with the
effects of medication on behaviour and experience.
serendipitous: Findings which were observed but were not originally the subject of
the investigation.
schizophrenia: A severe condition in which sufferers experience hallucinations,
delusions, speech impairment, irrationality, unusual motor activity and impairment in
most aspects of their lives.
16
Approaches in biopsychology
Behavioural genetics
Behavioural genetics attempts to identify what proportion of the variance in a
trait or behaviour can be attributed to genetics and to the environment (Plomin,
1988; Stabenau & Pollin, 1993; Zuckerman, 1991). This relationship is referred
to as heredity and is a prominent aspect of several areas of biopsychology
research. Indeed, you may be aware that research has often investigated if
there are genetic components to personality traits, intelligence and mental
illness. However, it is important to remember that there is a constant interaction
between genes and the environment (Jaffee et al., 2005). For example, a genetic
predisposition can determine which environments we seek while environmental
factors can also determine whether a gene pool survives (Darwin, 1859). This
is in accordance with evolutionary theories that behaviour tends to serve the
purpose of promoting survival, but also incorporates the principle of diathesis–
stress which states that both biology and environment influence behaviour.
Contemporary behavioural genetics often relies on techniques such as twin
studies, adoption studies and family studies (Table 1.5) and employs a variety
of parametric tests. Indeed, you may already be aware that monozygotic twins
share the same DNA, while dizygotic twins only share around half of their DNA.
Therefore, if genes are influential in determining behaviour, monozygotic twins
are more likely to share the same traits and behaviours. However, adoption
studies can also be used to investigate heredity. For example, by comparing
monozygotic and dizygotic twins who have been raised either together or
apart, researchers can calculate the proportion of the variance in a trait which
can be explained by the environment and genetics. While behavioural genetics
can contribute towards our understanding of how genetics and environment
contribute towards determining our behaviour, there are several significant
limitations with this approach:
MP Behavioural genetics have previously been used to support prejudiced views
and discrimination.
MP Ethical issues arise surrounding related topics such as eugenics (the argument
that intelligent individuals should have more children).
MP Behavioural genetics can be seen as a deterministic perspective in which the
individual is passive.
MP The genetic code is extremely complex and it is unlikely that a trait will arise
solely due to one chromosome.
MP The environment is constantly changing and as such so are the influences on
our behaviour.
MP Behavioural genetics often focuses on the extremes of a given trait because
these are more observable.
MP Parametric tests are not infallible and vary considerably in reliability and validity.
MP The distinction between normal and abnormal behaviour is culturally and
temporally relative.
17
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Technique Description
Twin study Investigates the extent to which scores on a trait correlate between
monozygotic and dizygotic twins reared together. A test of heredity.
Adoption study Investigates the extent to which scores on a trait correlate between
monozygotic and dizygotic twins reared either together or apart. A
test of heredity.
Genetic engineering The manipulation or cloning of the genome within laboratory settings.
Key terms
Adoption study: The comparison of siblings reared together or apart to assess heredity.
Behavioural genetics: An approach in psychology which attempts to identify what
proportion of the variance in a trait or behaviour can be attributed to genetics and to
the environment.
Dizygotic twins: Non-identical twins who only share half of their DNA.
Eugenics: A school of thought in which intelligent people are encouraged to
reproduce in an attempt to improve the species.
family study: A research technique in which the prevalence of a trait or type within a
family is assessed in regards to heredity.
Genetic engineering: The manipulation or cloning of the genome within laboratory
settings.
Heredity: The proportion of variance in a given trait or type which can be accounted
for by genetics and the environment.
Monozygotic twins: Identical twins who share the same DNA.
targeted mutation: Intended genetic mutations are produced in a laboratory and
injected into laboratory animals to produce the desired mutation.
twin study: The comparison of siblings on a specific measure to assess heredity.
KEY stuDY
Plomin (1988)
Plomin (1988) reanalysed the vast amount of literature concerning the heredity
of intelligence derived from twin, adoption and family studies and identified the
following findings:
MP the IQ scores of identical twins reared apart correlated at 0.74
MP the IQ scores of identical twins reared together correlated at 0.87
MP the IQ scores of non-identical twins reared together correlated at 0.53
t
18
Approaches in biopsychology
This suggests that genetics contributed 68 per cent of the variance in IQ, whereas the
environment explained only 19 per cent. However, it also means that 13 per cent of the
variance in scores was not explained by either genetics or the environment. Interesting
results were also obtained for unrelated children living together (0.23) and between
unrelated children and their adoptive parents (0.20). This suggets that the environment
explained 20–25 per cent of the variance in intelligence scores.
Comparative psychology
Comparative psychology is concerned with the general biology of behaviour and
performs comparisons across species (Dewsbury, 1990). However, while there
are similarities between comparative psychology and behavioural genetics, in
that both attempt to identify genetic components of behaviour, there are also
significant differences. Indeed, practitioners in comparative psychology are
influenced by the evolutionary perspective to a greater extent than those in
behaviour genetics. For example, they often believe that humans have evolved
from other organisms and share some degree of their genetics, biology and
behaviour with other animals (Darwin, 1859). In contrast, behavioural genetics
is primarily concerned with how human DNA influences their behaviour and
traits. There has also been an extension of comparative psychology to animal
cognition, demonstrating influences of cognitive psychology (Wasserman, 1993).
However, it is important for you to remember that although a large amount
of research occurs in laboratory settings, animals are also studied in their
natural environment. This is called ethological research and forms a significant
proportion of contemporary research in comparative psychology.
Key terms
Comparative psychology: An approach in psychology which is concerned with the
general biology of behaviour and performs comparisons across species.
Ethological research: A research technique in which animals are studied in their
natural environment with little intervention by the researcher.
19
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
CrItICAl foCus
Comparative psychology
While comparative approaches were prominent areas of research during the early days
of biological psychology, its influence has subsequently declined and few studies are
currently conducted in this area (Carlson, 2004; Darwin, 1859; Pinel, 2003). This may
be due to technological advances, which allow us to study human behaviour in much
more detail than was previously possible, and changes in ethical guidelines concerning
research with laboratory animals. It may also be due to theoretical shifts towards
including environmental factors in the equation for behaviour. For example, the nature–
nurture debate has undermined several aspects of biological comparative approaches
(Jaffee et al. 2005). Indeed, the majority of contemporary comparative research is
accomplished through cross-cultural studies rather than cross-species comparisons.
These studies usually adopt a very different perspective from that of biological
approaches. For example, contemporary cross-cultural studies tend to assume that
the environmental differences between cultures give rise to different behaviours and
attitudes. This is in direct contrast to biological comparative psychology which argues
that genetics is responsible for behavioural differences and similarities. However, it is
significant that cross-cultural studies also once assumed that these differences were
due to genetic factors, demonstrating significant developments in theory and practice.
Key term
Cross-cultural study: The study and comparison of groups of people from different
cultural backgrounds.
20
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
21
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Notes
22
Notes
Notes
23
1 • Introduction to biopsychology: origins, approaches and applications
Notes
24
2 Structure and function of
the central and peripheral
nervous systems
Structure and
function of the
central and
peripheral
nervous
systems
25
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Introduction
This chapter will provide you with a summary of the structures and functions
of the central nervous system (CNS) and the peripheral nervous system (PNS).
In regards to the CNS, this includes the brain and spinal cord. However, you
should remember that some texts also include the retinas – whether these are
included in this system will depend on your course. The PNS includes all of
the nerves, neurons, muscles and organs which are located beyond the central
nervous system. The endocrine system, which is responsible for releasing and
regulating hormones and related chemicals, is covered in Chapter 3. Together,
these structures form the main regulatory and control systems responsible for
facilitating all forms of cognition, experience and movement. They also play a
large role in regulating bodily functions through a complex system of neurons
which either send signals from the nervous system to trigger responses in the
body or transmit signals back to the nervous system. However, it is important to
remember that you are studying psychology and not biology and as such these
structures need to be understood in the context of behaviour rather than solely
based on anatomy. Consequently, this chapter will help you to understand how
the brain and spinal cord function within the body to enable behaviour, and also
to appreciate what happens when the nervous system is damaged.
Revision checklist
Key terms
Central nervous system: A complex system which governs all top-down processes
and consists of the brain and the spinal cord.
Endocrine system: The network of glands and organs which release and regulate
hormones.
Hormones: Endogenous substances produced by the glands of the body.
Peripheral nervous system: A complex system which governs all bottom-up
processes and consists of all of the nerves, muscles and organs beyond the CNS.
26
The central nervous system
Assessment advice
Essay questions concerning the nervous system will often ask you to discuss
how one of these vital systems enables behaviour and experience. For example,
you may be asked to evaluate the extent to which the central nervous system
determines perception. Although this question would not explicitly mention the
peripheral nervous system, it would be prudent to consider multiple influences and
establish an academic debate in your essay. This implies that it would be a good
idea to consider whether the peripheral nervous system and non-biological factors
complement, counteract or override the functions of the central nervous system.
Alternatively, you could be explicitly asked to compare and contrast the structures
and functions of the peripheral and central nervous systems in regards to one or
two specific behaviours. For questions like these you will need to understand the
structures, functions, research techniques for investigating these systems and the
prominent findings which have been provided by research. You will also need to
cite specific examples from the literature. Any essays in this area will also need to
demonstrate that you can draw appropriate conclusions based on the evidence
and correct interpretation of the nervous system.
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
To what extent does the central nervous system determine the course of
human behaviour? Discuss with reference to at least two aspects of behaviour
(for example, perception and memory).
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
The central nervous system consists of the brain and the spinal cord. Although
some texts include the retinas, there isn’t a consensus concerning this inclusion.
As such, they are not included in this revision guide, but you should consult
your course text to determine whether your module includes these aspects. You
27
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
should remember that the central nervous system is responsible for all forms of
cognition including perception, attention and memory in addition to reflexes,
compilation of somatosensory information and prompting motor movement.
Key term
Somatosensory: Information derived from the bodily senses.
Critically discuss the structures and functions of the brain and the spinal cord.
28
The central nervous system
Normal function
The primary function of the spinal cord is to transmit neural signals between the
brain and the rest of the body including the somatosensory system, muscles and
glands. However, it is important to remember that it also has some autonomy
and can function independently as demonstrated by reflexes.
How would injury to the spinal cord impair normal functions in humans?
Key term
Atrophy: The decay or wasting of a structure, organ or system.
29
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
2.1 What are the names given to the three levels of spinal meninges?
2.2 In addition to the meninges, which other structures and substances
protect the spinal cord?
2.3 What substance is contained within the subarachnoid space?
2.4 How might trauma to the spinal cord occur and what are the
consequences?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
The brain
The brain is the most important organ in the human body and is the subject of
study in most areas of psychology. It is also by far the most complex structure
in the human body. The brain plays a role in regulating other organs and largely
determines our ability to think, move, use language, perceive the world, maintain
consciousness, interact with each other and experience the world around us.
Several theorists also believe that the brain stores our intelligence and personality
and as such determines who we are. It is vitally important that you can understand
both the normal structure and functions of this organ and also appreciate what
happens when it is damaged. While it is impossible to fully cover this complex
organ in the scope of this revision guide, it will provide you with a coherent and
concise summary to guide your revision and test your understanding.
30
The central nervous system
Critically review the extent to which the central nervous system is protected
by biological factors.
The brain could simply be split along the corpus callosum and divided into two
hemispheres where the right side of the brain synthesises information and the
left side of the brain analyses information. However, several other attempts have
been made to group regions of the brain according to structures which share
commonalities in regards to either their location or their function. For example,
Table 2.1 demonstrates how the brain can be divided into those falling in the
forebrain, midbrain and hindbrain regions. Secondly, the brain can also be
organised according to more specialised structures, such as those summarised in
Figure 2.1 in addition to the specifications provided in Table 2.2.
Key term
Corpus callosum: A bundle of neural fibres connecting the left and right hemispheres
of the brain.
To what extent has the brain been organised according to specialism and
localisation of function?
31
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Telencephalon
Diencephalon
Mesencephalon
Metencephalon
Myelencephalon
Myelencephalon Medulla.
Function
Several factors may determine which regions of the brain specialise in which
function. Biological factors can include evolution, genetics and anatomical
predisposition. However, you should also remember that experience will
32
The central nervous system
determine the strength of connections in the brain, how well certain functions
are performed and it can also result in the redistribution of functions in a manner
which is not conventionally seen. You should also remember that the brain
is highly interconnected. As such, while it may appear that certain areas are
dominant in performing certain functions this may actually result from activity in
other areas of the brain, or be due to the connections between regions. Table
2.3 provides an overview of some of the regions which have been consistently
associated with certain functions. You should remember that this list is not
exhaustive so consult further reading accordingly. Figure 2.2 should also
help you to visualise where these structures are located in the brain and their
proximity to other regions.
Structure Function
Cerebral cortex Associated with most of the higher order functions such as
cognition.
33
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Central sulcus
Superior frontal gyrus Postcentral gyrus
Posterior ramus of
lateral sulcus Transverse occipital
sulcus
Superior temporal gyrus
Superior temporal sulcus Inferior temporal gyrus
34
The peripheral nervous system
The peripheral nervous system includes all of the cranial, spinal, motor and
sensory neurons, organs and neurotransmitters located beyond the central
nervous system. Hormones also play a role in the peripheral nervous system
but are covered in greater detail in Chapter 3. The peripheral nervous system is
responsible for transmitting signals to the central nervous system from the body
though the afferent neurons (with the exception of visual information which is
transmitted directly via the optical nerve) and also receives feedback from the
central nervous system via the efferent neurons. You should also remember that
there is constant interaction and communication between these systems and the
rest of the body, including the glands and muscles which receive signals from
the efferent neurons. There are also 12 pairs of cranial nerves which serve the
function of transmitting sensory information from the head and neck back to the
central nervous system. Somatosensory information from the peripheral nervous
system is typically received by the central nervous system via unipolar neurons.
However, the structure and functions of the peripheral nervous system are easier
to understand when considered in the various subdivisions of this nervous system.
35
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Key terms
Action potential: An electrical signal which is transmitted along neurons.
Somatic nervous system: A division of the peripheral nervous system which is
responsible for monitoring and interacting with the external world.
Key terms
Autonomic nervous system: A division of the peripheral nervous system which is
responsible for governing responses which are largely beyond conscious control.
Enteric nervous system: A division of the autonomic nervous system which is
responsible for maintaining the gastrointestinal system.
Parasympathetic nervous system: A division of the autonomic nervous system which
promotes the conservation of resources.
Stress response: The physiological, cognitive and behavioural response to threat and
anxiety characterised by action readiness for ‘fight-or-flight’.
Sympathetic nervous system: A division of the autonomic nervous system which
promotes action readiness.
36
The peripheral nervous system
Parasympathetic Sympathetic
Dilates pupil
Vagus Dilates
nerve bronchi
Solar
Constricts plexus
bronchi
Inhibits
peristalsis
Stimulates and
peristalsis secretion
and
secretion
Conversion
Chain of of glycogen
Stimulates sympathetic to glucose
release of ganglia
Secretion of
bile
epinephrine and
norepinephrine
Contracts Inhibits
bladder bladder
contraction
Figure 2.3 The organisation and functions of the autonomic nervous system
37
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
MP dilates pupils
MP inhibits salivation
MP dilates bronchioles in the lungs
MP expands airways
MP speeds heart rate
MP stimulates sweating
MP stimulates glucose release
MP constricts blood vessels in the skin
MP inhibits the digestive system
MP stimulates secretion of the catecholamine hormones epinephrine and
norepinephrine by adrenal medulla
MP relaxes bladder
MP stimulates sexual arousal.
Compare and contrast the structures and functions of the sympathetic and
parasympathetic branches of the peripheral nervous system with reference
to stress.
38
The peripheral nervous system
Key term
Homeostasis: The naturally balanced state of the body. This is the ideal state
and the parasympathetic nervous system strives to restore this equilibrium when
physiology is imbalanced.
CriTiCAl FoCuS
Enteric system
The enteric system is responsible for controlling the gastrointestinal system
and operates autonomously from the other branches of the nervous system.
Indeed, it continues to function even when the vagus nerve is severed. This
is potentially due to the vital nature of the gastrointestinal system for survival.
The enteric system contains efferent, afferent and inter-neurons and utilises the
neurotransmitters acetylcholine, dopamine and serotonin.
39
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
40
The peripheral nervous system
Dendrites
Axon terminal
Synaptic vesicles
Axon
Neurotransmitter
Cell body
Receptor site
Synaptic space
Table 2.4 provides an overview of some of the neurotransmitters you will need
to know during your undergraduate degree, in addition to brief summaries
concerning their primary functions. However, it is important for you to remember
that this list is not exhaustive and neurotransmitters serve multiple functions.
Neurotransmitter Function
Norepinephrine and epinephrine Operate in arousal and reward systems (Carlson, 2004).
41
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
However, you should remember that neurotransmitters are not the only
chemicals which are active in the nervous system and that there are several
subdivisions of influential substances:
MP Neurotransmitters: These are endogenous (naturally occurring) chemicals
which transport signals between neurons.
MP Hormones: Endogenous chemicals produced mostly by the endocrine system
(covered in Chapter 3). Hormones including steroids, testosterone and
androgen can be influential in behaviour, mood and cognition.
MP Amino acids: These include glutamate, GABA and glycine. All of these
chemicals are influential in behaviour.
MP Monoamines: These are biogenic amines which include dopamine,
norepinephrine, epinephrine, histamine and serotonin. These substances
are influence in reward mood/emotion, learning, motivation, behaviour and
cognition.
MP Neuroactive peptides: These chemicals often function in conjunction with
neurotransmitters.
42
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Key term
Peripheral neuropathy: A condition in which the nerves that stimulate muscles to
move are damaged and can result in muscle atrophy and facial palsy.
2.10 What are the divisions of the autonomic nervous system and how do
they differ?
2.11 What are the functions of the somatic nervous system?
2.12 What is the primary function of the enteric system?
2.13 How do neurotransmitters function?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
To what extent does the central nervous system determine the course of
human behaviour? Discuss with reference to at least two aspects of behaviour
(for example, perception and memory).
43
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Notes
44
Notes
Notes
45
2 • Structure and function of the central and peripheral nervous systems
Notes
46
3
The endocrine system:
hormones and behaviour
Glands
Hormones
Regulation
of the
endocrine
system
The endocrine
system: hormones
and behaviour
Developmental
aspects of the
endocrine
system
Hormones
and sexual
Disorders behaviour
of the
endocrine
system
47
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
Introduction
This chapter will provide you with an overview of the structures and functions of
the endocrine system and how it influences human behaviour and experience.
The endocrine system is a series of cells, glands and organs that produce
and release hormones which consequently circulate throughout the body.
You should remember from the previous chapter that hormones are naturally
occurring chemicals which influence bodily functions, physical development
and emotions. This chapter will be primarily concerned with how the endocrine
system influences sexual behaviour, although it can also influence other vital
motivations such as eating and sleeping. By the end of this chapter you should
be able to correctly describe the endocrine system and understand how certain
hormones function. However, as you are studying psychology and not biology
you will also need to contextualise this with reference to the implications for
behaviour. For example, do certain hormones predispose certain behaviours?
Are they released as a product of a behaviour or are they involved in performing
the action? You will also need to put aside many of your misconceptions
about hormones. For example, despite substantial anecdotal evidence there
are few scientific examples of hormones determining any aspect of male and
female gender roles. Indeed, androgens and estrogens are present in everyone
regardless of gender. Hormones do, however, play a large role in determining
physical and sexual development across the lifespan.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions in this area will usually ask you to evaluate the extent to which
hormones influence certain behaviours or contribute towards differences
between groups. For example, you may be asked to evaluate the extent to
which hormones determine aggression or you could be asked to discuss whether
hormones contribute towards gender typical behaviour. With questions like
these you will need to demonstrate that you can synthesise evidence from
various sources and critically evaluate the strength of this evidence. You must
also consider other possible influences for these behaviours and differences.
For example, this can include other biological factors, cognition, social factors,
48
Glands
personality or intelligence. You will also need to consider whether the evidence
suggests there is a causal relationship between hormones and behaviour or
just a relationship between them (in which case behaviour could also result in
the release of the hormone). Any essays in this area should include a balanced
consideration of all possible sides of the argument and your conclusions should
be based on a critical evaluation of this evidence.
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Glands
49
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
MP Thyroid gland: Releases thyroxin and calcitonin. Regulates the rate of growth,
metabolism and calcium levels in the blood.
MP Parathyroid gland: Releases parathormone and promotes the homeostasis
of calcium.
MP Thymus: Releases thymosins and T-lymphocytes which promote the
development of the immune system.
MP Adrenal glands: Release epinephrine, norepinephrine, aldosterone and
cortisol. Mediate the conversion of proteins into sugars and are involved in
the stress response.
MP Pancreas: Secretes insulin, glucagon and somatostatin to regulate sugar
metabolism.
MP Ovaries: The female gonads produce estrogens and progesterone to promote
the development of external, physical, female characteristics. Fluctuations in
gonadal hormones can also result in fluctuations in mood.
MP Testes: The male gonads produce testosterone and promote external,
physical, male characteristics. Fluctuations in gonadal hormones can also
result in fluctuations in mood.
Key terms
Endocrine glands: Glands which secrete hormones directly into the bloodstream
having fast-acting and concentrated effects.
Exocrine glands: Glands which secrete substances into ducts, from which they pass
from cell to cell through diffusion.
50
Hormones
3.1 Which glands secrete their chemicals directly into the bloodstream?
3.2 What are the functions of the adrenal gland?
3.3 What are the functions of the pituitary gland?
3.4 What chemicals are produced by the pancreas?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Hormones
You may be aware that hormones are chemicals which arise naturally in the body
and are associated with moods, behaviour and development (Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003). Hormones can be divided into three categories consisting of amino
acid derivatives, peptides and proteins and finally steroids. A summary of these
types of hormones is provided in Table 3.1 to help you refresh your memory.
Amino acid derivatives These substances are synthesised from epinephrine and are
involved in the synthesis and transmission of peptides, proteins
and neurotransmitters.
Peptides and proteins These hormones are short and long chains of amino acids and
facilitate physiological, biochemical and growth processes.
51
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
52
Regulation of the endocrine system
Key term
Topic: The term assigned to hormones which stimulate or inhibit the release of other
hormones.
There are significant gender differences in the regulation of the endocrine system
by the hypothalamus. For example, while the level of hormones in males remains
relatively stable across time, female gonadal hormones are cyclical (Carlson,
2004; Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer & Hellhammer, 1999; Pinel, 2003).
This cycle is on average 28 days and you will probably know that it stimulates
the menstrual cycle including the development and release of an ovum and
menstruation in the absence of conception. However, during pregnancy and for
a brief time after birth there is a period of amenorrhoea in which menstruation
is suspended, allowing time for the uterus to recover. The process can also
be controlled with hormonal birth control which simulates the natural gonadal
hormone progesterone. Regulation of the endocrine system can be also
performed by the central nervous system, peripheral nervous system and other
biological chemicals such as glucose, calcium and sodium.
3.9 Which structures and substances are involved in the regulation of the
endocrine system?
3.10 What characteristic of hormones means that they can be called topic?
3.11 What is the main difference between the levels of hormones in males
and females?
3.12 How can the endocrine system be regulated by engineered substances?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
53
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
You have probably already realised that steroids such as androgen, estrogen,
testosterone and progesterone play a large role in sexual development and
behaviour (Escobar, Obregón & Rey, 2004; Handwerger & Freemark, 2000).
Indeed, we have already identified that steroids are able to penetrate cell
membranes and influence gene expression (how characteristics manifest). These
chemicals are also involved with the production of sperm and ova by the male
and female gonads respectively. This facilitates the production of a zygote at
conception which will eventually grow into a foetus influenced by all of these
hormones. In females, progesterone also prepares the uterus for carrying a baby
and facilitates breast feeding.
Up to six weeks after conception, all foetuses have the same primordial gonads
with the potential to develop ovaries or testes (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
However, at this point ovaries automatically develop and the male Y chromosome
triggers the synthesis of the protein H-Y antigen which stimulates the development
of testes. External genitalia develop from the second month of pregnancy
although there are recursors from the glands, urethral folds, lateral bodies and
labioscrotal swellings. You should also be aware that at six weeks after conception
all foetuses have two sets of reproductive ducts. These are the male Wolffian
system and the female Müllerian system. However, at the third month of foetal
development the testes release testosterone and a Müllerian-inhibiting hormone
which promote the development of the Wolffian system while inhibiting further
development of the Müllerian system. The development of the precursors are all
triggered by hormones and are listed below:
MP Glans: Head of penis in males or the clitoris in females.
MP Urethral folds: Fuse in males, enlarge and become the labia minora in females.
MP Lateral bodies: The shaft of the penis in males or hood of the clitoris in females.
MP Labioscrotal swellings: Scrotum in males or labia majora in females.
In addition to the sexual differences which arise due to hormones there are
also differences in male and female brains. For example, you should know
that the male brain is on average 15 per cent larger than the female brain and
there are structural differences in the hypothalamus, corpus callosum, anterior
commissure, thalamus and cerebral cortex. These changes are believed to arise
due to hormones in the form of perinatal androgens occurring near the time
of birth. However, it is important to remember that all sex steroids are derived
from cholesterol and can be converted to other sex steroids via a process called
54
Developmental aspects of the endocrine system
Key term
Aromatisation: The process by which sex steroids derived from cholesterol are
converted into other sex steroids.
55
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
3.13 How does the endocrine system influence the development of a foetus?
3.14 How does the endocrine system influence development in adolescence?
3.15 How would steroids influence gene expression?
3.16 What other factors may influence physical development?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
The previous belief that sex steroids promote male and female stereotypical
behaviour is not held in contemporary psychology due to a notable lack of
evidence (Carlson, 2004; Orwoll & Klein, 1995; Pinel, 2003). Indeed, all of these
substances are present in both males and females although the evolutionary
role of the hormones may not be transparent. It is important to remember that
early research linked the perinatal hormones to animal reproductive behaviour.
For example, injections of testosterone near the time of birth were believed
to masculinise and defeminise female copulation behaviour of the offspring.
However, it is important to remember that this research makes the assumption
that the behaviour would not have been masculine without testosterone injections
and that the findings may not be generalised from laboratory animals to humans.
Indeed, a significant degree of the research conducted with laboratory animals
has investigated the effects of pheromones on the menstrual cycle and sexual
behaviour. Pheromones are chemicals released by an animal which are perceived
by another via smell or taste. Research has demonstrated that these chemicals
can indicate when a female is likely to conceive. In females, pheromones can
stimulate ovulation. However, it is important to remember that this research is not
directly applicable to humans who do not rely on these substances. Table 3.2 will
refresh your memory concerning some of the effects of pheromones observed in
laboratory animals.
Key term
Pheromone: A chemical substance transmitted from one animal to another via smell
or taste, usually to signal receptivity, availability, challenge or threat.
56
Hormones and sexual behaviour
Effect Description
Lee–Boot effect The slowing and cessation of the menstrual/estrous cycle if all
female animals are housed together. It is caused by a pheromone
carried in urine.
Vanderbergh effect The early onset of puberty in female rats housed with a male.
57
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
3.17 How does removal of the ovaries influence human sexual behaviour?
3.18 How does removal of the testes influence human sexual behaviour?
3.19 What are the main limitations of early studies investigating the effects
of hormones on sexual behaviour?
3.20 What are the main hormonal differences between heterosexual and
homosexual people?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Disorders of the endocrine system are common and are usually detected
by imbalanced levels of hormones, observable physical indications or when
examining other illnesses (Counsell & Ruddell, 1994; Guzick, 2004; Legro,
Kunselman, Dodson & Dunaif, 1999; Manolagas, 2000; Orwoll & Klein, 1995).
Table 3.3 provides a summary of some of these conditions. However, it is
important to remember that these conditions can co-occur and often also result
in psychological distress such as depression and anxiety.
Disorder Description
Diabetes This is one of the main disorders of the endocrine system and arises
due to the failure of the pancreas to produce sufficient amounts of the
hormone insulin or due to the body’s inability to use insulin correctly. If
undiagnosed, consequences can be severe due to the body’s reliance
on insulin to convert sugars and starch to energy.
Osteoporosis This is a disorder in which bones become fragile and easily breakable.
It can result from abnormally low levels of estrogen or testosterone.
Diagnosis is often prompted by recurrent injuries.
Growth disorders As the endocrine system regulates growth imbalances, impaired function
or damage to this system can result in excessive or deficient growth.
Thyroid disorders There are numerous types of thyroid disorders including hyperthyroidism
(excessive amount of hormones), hypothyroidism (insufficient hormones),
thyroid nodules and thyroid cancer. These conditions also significantly
reduce autoimmunity and often result in weight changes.
Polycystic ovary PCOS is also a very common disorder of the endocrine system which
syndrome (PCOS) results in numerous cysts developing on the ovaries. This disrupts
the menstrual cycle, can cause infertility and is often characterised by
hirsutism (excessive hair growth), acne, diabetes and hypertension.
58
Disorders of the endocrine system
This table only provides a summary of the disorders which can arise due to the
malfunction or impairment of the endocrine system. You should also remember
that many other conditions can arise though the imbalance of hormones and that
these conditions can result in or co-occur with other health complaints.
59
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
60
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Notes
61
3 • The endocrine system: hormones and behaviour
Notes
62
4
Biological basis of
language
Cognitive
neuroscience
and the
physiology of
language
63
4 • Biological basis of language
Introduction
This chapter will provide you with an overview concerning how the structures
and functions of the human body enable language acquisition, comprehension
and production. You should remember that language acquisition,
comprehension and production are some of the most complex processes which
have generated a considerable amount of research and theoretical debate.
Indeed, the ability to acquire, produce and comprehend language is often cited
as one of the factors which differentiate humans from other animals (Gaskell,
2007; Harley, 2007). The initial sections of this chapter will provide a general
overview of the features and nature of language to refresh your memory of these
topics. These sections will guide your revision of the components and processes
of language and communication. The latter sections will discuss the issues of
cerebral lateralisation, cerebral localisation, methodologies and several disorders
which are characterised by language deficits. This chapter is primarily concerned
with human communication. Although animal communication is discussed briefly,
the often outdated nature of this research means that it may not be covered in
all language modules. The sample essay questions and ‘Test your knowledge’
questions provided throughout this chapter and on the companion website will
help you to test your understanding of these topics. However, it is important
to remember that language is a complex phenomenon made up of various
processes which span across cognitive psychology, developmental psychology
and biological psychology. Many of these issues are beyond the scope of this
text which is primarily concerned with the biological aspects of these processes.
However, it is vital that you can establish and maintain an understanding of
various influences on the acquisition, comprehension and production of language
– the suggested further reading should begin this process.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions in this area will usually ask you to evaluate the extent to which
nature and nurture determine language development or the extent to which
language is dependent on biological factors. For example, you may be asked to
64
Defining language
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress.
Defining language
65
4 • Biological basis of language
MP Displacement means that the messages are not restricted to a specific time.
MP Discreteness means that there is a notable range of speech sounds within a
language.
MP Duality of structure means that phonemes can be combined or recombined in
a potentially infinite number of ways.
MP Productivity means that novel messages can be created using the rules and
symbols of the language.
MP Interchangeability means that those who share the language are both
producers and receivers of messages.
MP Semanticity means that meaning can be transmitted through the symbols.
MP Total feedback means that the speaker has immediate auditory feedback.
MP Specialisation means that the sounds of the language are distinct from other
sounds in that they convey meaning.
MP Transitoriness means that the messages fade quickly after they have been
uttered.
MP Vocal–auditory channel refers to the transmission of language using these two
sensory modes.
Key term
Linguistic universals: The 13 principles of language which Hockett (1960) argued
could be observed across all languages.
66
Defining language
Unit/process Description
Lexicon The complete set of morphemes (see below) available within the
language. Usually conceptualised as a store in memory.
Morpheme The smallest unit of language which possesses meaning on its own
through a combination of phonemes.
Phoneme The smallest unit within a language which forms the building blocks of
other utterances.
Syntax The rules for creating longer utterances. This includes the order in which
words should be placed in a sentence.
67
4 • Biological basis of language
Species Description
Gorilla The gorilla Koko was trained to understand over 300 American Sign
Language gestures (Patterson, 1978, 1981).
Parrot The African grey parrot Alex could identify 50 different objects, count
to six, distinguish seven colours and differentiate five shapes. Alex also
possessed a vocabulary of over 100 words and could combine these words
to form new utterances (Pepperberg, 2006a, 2006b).
Chimpanzee The chimpanzee Sarah was trained to communicate through the use of
coloured plastic chips representing words. Language-trained chimpanzees
also appeared to have various problem-solving abilities which non-trained
chimpanzees did not (Premack, 1983).
Chimpanzee The chimpanzee Lana was trained to use a computer-based sign language
to form short meaningful sentences (Savage-Rumbaugh, Rumbaugh &
McDonald, 1985; Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins &
Rupert 1986).
Bonobo The bonobo Kanzi learnt to communicate using a sign board by observing
another ape being trained (Gardner, Gardner & Van Contfort, 1989;
Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1985, 1986).
Chimpanzee Nim Chimpsky lived with a human family from birth and was able to
understand over 100 American Sign Language gestures (Terrace, 1979).
These cases should inspire you to wonder whether human language is as unique
and advanced as we often believe it to be. The decision on that debate is still
to be determined. However, you should keep in mind that verbal and physical
communication rely on different biological factors and cognitive processes
(Carlson, 2004; Gaskell, 2007; Harley, 2007; Pinel, 2003).
68
The anatomy of human language
Lateralisation of language
Theories which argue that language is a lateralised function are essentially
arguing that the left hemisphere of the brain is the locus of these functions
(Carlson, 2004; Harley, 2007; Pinel, 2003). As you should remember from
the earlier chapters, the hemispheres can indeed be dominant in certain
functions and serve minor roles in others. However, while this means that the
left hemisphere appears to exert the greatest influence in the acquisition,
comprehension and production of language, you should remember that
language is extremely complex, activity is widely dispersed in the brain and
the right hemisphere still shows levels of activity during language processes
(Harley, 2007; Pinel, 2003). Indeed, although language is the most lateralised
of all human functions, it is not even absolute in this case (Harley, 2007; Pinel,
2003). You should also remember that the hemispheres of a cerebrally intact
individual are able to communicate via the corpus callosum and cerebral
69
4 • Biological basis of language
Dichotic listening
A dichotic listening test is a non-invasive technique in which participants are
presented with two separate, distinctive auditory messages. One is presented
to the left ear (right hemisphere) and the other is presented to the right ear
(left hemisphere). The participant is then asked to recite both of the messages.
Most participants are only able to recite the message presented to the right ear
which sends signals to the left hemisphere (Carlson, 2004; Harley, 2007; Pinel,
2003). This should suggest to you that the left hemisphere is dominant in speech
perception and memory for language.
70
The anatomy of human language
times more likely to develop deficits in language than females. This suggests
that men’s language functions are more lateralised than females (Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003).
Split-brain studies
You should already be aware that split-brain studies form the majority of evidence
for the lateralisation of language abilities. In these studies, the two hemispheres
have been surgically separated in attempts to reduce medical problems such as
epilepsy through the severing of the corpus callosum (Carlson, 2004; Penfield
& Roberts, 1959; Pinel, 2003; Somers et al., 2011). Both hemispheres are able
to function independently but communication between these two structures is
severely limited and often impossible. This means that each hemisphere retains
the processes for which it was dominant but is unable to integrate functions and
processes which required the use of both hemispheres. These studies produce
very interesting insights into the lateralisation of language which you will need to
understand in your assessments.
MP When words are presented to the right visual field and left hemisphere,
patients are able to tell the experimenter what they are viewing or reach with
their right hand. Performance is at chance when using the left hand, suggesting
that performance is no better than if they were guessing the answer.
MP When words are presented to the left visual field and right hemisphere,
patients claim that nothing was presented although the left hand is still able
to reach for and identify the object.
MP There appears to be a degree of cross-curing (indirect communication
between hemispheres) because when the left hemisphere is wrong the right
hemisphere appears to provide suitable bodily cues which encourage the left
hemisphere to re-evaluate. For example, the left hemisphere may provide
an incorrect verbal response while the right hemisphere triggers a frown or
shake of the head and the left hemisphere will trigger an alternative response
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
MP There is even a ‘helping-hand’ phenomenon in which the left hand
(controlled by the right hemisphere) will reach out and grab the correct
object even when the left hemisphere is providing the incorrect answer
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
MP For most split-brain patients the left hemisphere controls most daily activities
and the right hemisphere has no ability to act with obvious intention.
However, in some patients the right hemisphere remains active and can
produce impulsive, socially unacceptable, obstinate, mischievous and
disturbing gestures. In such cases, patients often report hatred towards their
left side and right hemisphere because their left hemisphere is unable to
understand what is happening (Pinel, 2003).
71
4 • Biological basis of language
Key terms
Dextral: Right-handed.
Dichotic listening test: A task in which dual messages are presented to the left and
right ear and participants must try to recite both messages.
Lateralisation: The theory that one hemisphere of the brain is dominant in a given
process while the other serves only minor roles.
Localisation: The theory that specialised structures of the brain facilitate specific
functions.
Sinestral: Left-handed.
Sodium amytal test: A test in which one hemisphere of the brain is anaesthetised to
test the performance of the other hemisphere.
Split-brain study: A study which examines the performance of people who have had
their hemispheres surgically separated.
Theories of lateralisation
There are three main interpretations concerning the function of lateralisation.
However, you should be aware that there is evidence which both supports and
refutes these perspectives (Gaskell, 2007; Harley, 2007; Soares & Grosjean, 1981).
MP The first perspective is that the left hemisphere has evolved to be more
logical and analytic while the right hemisphere has evolved to serve the
function of synthesising information.
MP The second perspective is that the left hemisphere has evolved to control fine
motor skills of which speech is only one example.
MP The final perspective forms the linguistic theory and proponents argue that
the left hemisphere’s primary function is language in all of its various forms.
The debate surrounding these theories is ongoing and I recommend that you
direct your further reading accordingly. However, the contemporary view is that
the early lateralisation and concept of hemispheric dominance is too simplistic to
account for the complex phenomenon which is human language (Gaskell, 2007;
Harley, 2007; Pinel, 2003; Pinel & Dehaene, 2010).
72
The anatomy of human language
73
4 • Biological basis of language
what is conduction aphasia ?
MP Damaging the connection between Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas can cause
conduction aphasia in which individuals struggle to repeat words they have
heard.
What is agraphia and alexia ?
MP Damage to the angular gyrus can result in an inability to read (alexia) and an
inability to write (agraphia).
A comprehensive list of language deficits which can arise due to damage to
specific regions of the brain can be found in Table 4.3 and also in the Glossary
at the end of this book. You should remember that if a function is impaired after
damage to a cerebral structure that structure may be the locus of the function or
may be part of the pathway responsible for the function.
Condition/term Description
Conduction aphasia Inability to repeat words which are heard, while still being able
to speak normally. Caused by damage to the arcuate fasciculus
which connects Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area.
Phonological dysgraphia A writing disorder in which individuals are unable to sound out
words and write them phonetically.
Phonological dyslexia Ability to read familiar words but deficits in the ability to read
unfamiliar words and pronounceable non-words.
Pure alexia Inability to read without the loss of the ability to write produced
by brain damage.
Pure word deafness The ability to speak, hear, write and read without being able
to comprehend the meaning of speech. Caused by damage to
Wernicke’s area and the disruption of auditory input.
t
74
The anatomy of human language
Surface dyslexia Ability to read words phonetically but deficits in the ability to
read irregularly spelled words.
Word form dyslexia An individual is only able to read words after spelling out the
individual letters. Also known as spelling dyslexia.
A patient has been referred to you with deficits in the ability to comprehend
and produce meaningful spontaneous speech but they appear to be able to
repeat the phrases you produce when instructed. How would this patient’s
condition be diagnosed and how is it different from other disorders of
language? You will need to prepare a report which discusses this form of
impairment and differentiates it from other conditions.
75
4 • Biological basis of language
76
Cognitive neuroscience and the physiology of language
77
4 • Biological basis of language
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
78
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Notes
79
4 • Biological basis of language
Notes
80
5
Mechanisms of perception
and sensation
General
properties of
perception,
sensation and
attention Visual
perception
and
attention
Auditory
Mechanisms
perception
of perception
and
and sensation
attention
Somatosensation
Chemical
senses
81
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Introduction
This chapter will provide you with an overview of the biological structures,
mechanisms and processes which underlie human perception, sensation and
ultimately attention. Special attention will be paid to the general properties
of perception and sensation, visual perception, auditory perception,
somatosensory systems and the chemical senses. However, it is important
to remember that additional information which is relevant to this topic was
covered in Chapters 1–3. You should also remember that perception, sensation
and attention are intrinsically linked. For example, we must perceive a stimulus
in order to allocate our attention to this item. Our attention also facilitates the
experiences of sensation and perception. You should also remember that the
structures of the brain do not only serve one function. Perception, sensation
and attention are also linked to memory, consciousness, language and social
interaction. You will probably also be aware that while these processes are
linked they can also be dissociated and operate independently. However this
chapter is primarily concerned with the biological factors which facilitate or
are observed during perception and sensation. The cognitive mechanisms and
social functions of these phenomena are beyond the scope of this text but it is
advisable that you expand your understanding to include other non-biological
influences on behaviour. By the end of this chapter, you should be able to recall
some of the physiological, neural and anatomical factors associated with human
perception and sensation.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions in this area will usually ask you to compare, contrast or evaluate
two forms of perception, sensation or attention. For example, you may be asked
to compare the biological contributions towards visual and auditory perception
and attention. Alternatively, you may be asked to compare and contrast biological
and non-biological factors which influence human perception and attention. For
these types of questions you will need to adopt a critical and evidence-based
82
General properties of perception, sensation and attention
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress.
The key terms box below presents definitions of attention, perception and
sensation. You should be able to clearly identify how these processes are related
but at the same time function independently. There are five exteroceptive
sensory systems which rely on these processes. These are vision, audition
(hearing), touch (somatosensation), smell (olfaction) and taste (gustation). Each
of these systems receives sensory information originally derived from external
sources, pressures upon the body and internal cues. This can either direct
or be informed by attention and perception (Broerse, 2001; Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003). Indeed, an interesting case study of how these processes can be
dissociated is also presented below.
83
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Key terms
Attention: The allocation of cognitive resources to stimuli.
Exteroceptive sensory systems: The systems responsible for the senses of touch,
smell, taste, hearing and vision.
Perception: Higher-order processes of integrating, reorganising and interpreting a
complete pattern of sensation.
Sensation: The process of detecting a stimulus.
Visual agnosia: A neuropsychological condition characterised by deficits in perception.
KEY STUDY
Sacks (1985), The man who mistook his wife for a hat
An interesting insight into the differentiation of sensation and perception and the nature
of visual agnosia was provided by the case study of Dr. P. which was presented by
Sacks in 1985. Dr. P. is famous for making numerous perceptual errors such as mistaking
inanimate objects for people and mistaking body parts for clothing, including believing
that his foot was in fact his shoe and that his wife’s head was actually a hat. This pattern
of impairment suggests that while the patient appeared to have unimpaired sensation
because he was able to experience the stimuli, his perception of these stimuli was
severely impaired.
84
General properties of perception, sensation and attention
processing could only proceed in one direction and was identical in nature
across each cerebral structure. You should remember that this perspective was
based on early experimental methods which lacked the advanced technology
which is available today (Pinel, 2003; Rayner, 2009). It cannot fully explain the
complex nature of human perception, sensation and attention and it fails to
consider how an early process may be impaired while later processes remain
intact (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). In contrast, the current perspective is that
while the structures are indeed organised hierarchically, they are functionally
segregated and rely on parallel processing (Broerse, 2001; Carlson, 2004;
Goldstein, 2009; Grossman et al., 2000; Kourtzi & Kanwisher, 2000; Pinel,
2003). This means that there is a division of labour, with cerebral structures
serving qualitatively and quantitatively different functions with multiple levels
of processing. These functions are performed simultaneously, and while most
pathways carry information from lower to higher systems this is not universal
because there is feedback between the cerebral structures (Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003; Shaw, Lien, Ruthruff & Allen, 2011). Jeannerod, Arbib, Rizzolatti
and Sakarta (1995) also argued that there are two distinct parallel streams of
information in the sensory systems in which one influences behaviour without
conscious awareness (for example, in the case of reflexes) and another which
influences behaviour by engaging our conscious awareness and attention
(such as in intentional touch). You should remember from earlier chapters that
neuropsychological patients do appear to have specific deficits while other
abilities remain intact, processing does appear to be parallel in nature and action
and awareness can indeed be dissociated (such as in the cases of blind-sight and
split-brain studies). The following sections will also refresh your memory of these
organisational properties with specific reference to each of the sensory systems.
85
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
5.1 How did early theories describe the cerebral structures and processes
of perception and attention?
5.2 How do current theories describe the cerebral structures and processes
of perception and attention?
5.3 What are the five exteroceptive sensory systems?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
86
Visual perception and attention
CriTiCAl FoCUS
However, you will also probably be aware that visual perception and reality are
not always synonymous in the visual system. Indeed, visual illusions can confuse
this system into perceiving items which are not actually present. This suggests that
the processes seen in patients with scotomas are present in unimpaired individuals
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). Researchers have also identified over a dozen
separate, functional areas of the visual cortex using fMRI and PET scans suggesting
that the visual system is functionally segregated (Grossman et al., 2000; Kourtzi &
Kanwisher, 2000).
Key terms
Scotoma: An area of blindness produced through damage to the primary visual cortex.
Perimetry task: A task which identifies the location and scope of a scotoma by asking
participants to identify when they are able to see a series of dots presented in the
visual field.
87
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Key term
Prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological condition in which people are unable to
recognise faces.
88
Auditory perception and attention
Transcranial stimulation Grosbras, M., & Paus, T. (2002). Transcranial magnetic stimulation
and visual perception of the human frontal eye field: Effects on visual perception and
attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1109–1120.
Neuropsychology and Huberle, E., Driver, J., & Karnath, H. (2010). Retinal versus physical
visual perception stimulus size as determinants of visual perception in
simultanagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1677–1682.
Visual perception Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind?
and attention A review of how face perception and attention interact.
Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 75–92.
Audition (hearing) is based on molecule vibrations for which humans are able to
hear between 20–20,200 hertz (Goldstein, 2009; Pinel, 2003). Sounds are made
up of physical dimensions with corresponding physical stimuli and perceptual
dimensions. These properties are presented in Table 5.1. The physical dimensions
reflect the sensation of the stimuli and the perceptual dimensions reflect what is
perceived. However, it is important to remember that the physical dimensions are
on a scale rather than dichotomised at two extremes and pure tones only exist in
laboratories and recording studios (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
89
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
The process of audition is complex and involves a range of structures which are
summarised below to refresh your memory (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003; Shamma
& Micheyl, 2010):
MP Sound waves travel down the auditory canal causing the tympanic membrane
(eardrum) to vibrate.
MP These vibrations are transferred to the three ossicles (bones of the middle ear)
consisting of the malleus (hammer), incus (anvil) and the stapes (stirrup).
MP This results in vibrations of the oval window and cochlear internal membrane
which acts as a receptor for the organ of Corti.
MP The organ of Corti consists of two membranes called the basilar and tectorial
membranes. The hair cells on the basilar membrane act as receptor cells
while the tectorial membrane rests on top of the hair cells. These structures
are responsible for triggering action potentials in the axons of the auditory
nerves, beginning the transmission of the signal from the ear to the brain.
MP The vestibular system of the ear also carries information about the direction
and intensity of head movements, making it vital for balance.
MP From the ear, the action potentials are carried along the auditory nerves
to the primary auditory cortex. However, unlike the visual system there is
no major pathway. The auditory system is more like a network of pathways
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
MP The axons of each auditory nerve synapse in the ipsilateral cochlear nuclei
leading to the superior olives, lateral lemniscus, inferior colliculi and medial
geniculate nuclei.
90
Somatosensation
nature of the auditory system (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003; Stevens, Fanning,
Coch, Sanders & Neville, 2008). It is also difficult to track and isolate the effects
of brain damage since the location of the auditory cortex makes it virtually
impossible to damage without also destroying other structures.
Somatosensation
91
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Key terms
Proprioceptive system: The structures and processes responsible for processing
information concerning the position of the body.
interoceptive system: The structures and processes responsible for processing
information concerning conditions within the body.
There are four types of cutaneous receptors found in both hairy and hairless skin
which facilitate somatosensation (Lumpkin & Caterina, 2007; Pinel, 2003). The
free nerve endings are sensitive to temperature changes and pain. Secondly,
the Pacinian corpuscles adapt rapidly and respond to the displacement of skin.
Finally, the Merkel’s disks and Ruffini endings both adapt slowly and respond
to skin indentation and stretching. Nerve fibres carrying information from these
cutaneous receptors gather together into dorsal roots which feed into the spine.
However, consistent with the current perceptive on the organisation of the
perceptual system you should also remember that the somatosensory system has
two main, parallel routes to the cortex through either the dorsal column-medial
lemniscus route or the anterolateral system.
MP Dorsal column-medial lemniscus: This pathway carries information relating
to light touch, vibration, pressure and the position of the body. The nerves
enter the spinal cord via a dorsal root ascending ipsilaterally in the dorsal
columns and synapse in the dorsal column nuclei of the medulla. Here
they cross over and ascend to the medial lemniscus to the ventral posterior
nucleus of the thalamus.
MP Anterolateral system: This pathway carries information relating to pain
and temperature and consists of three separate tracts. These are the
spinothalamic, spinoreticular and spinotectal tracts. Firstly, the spinothalamic
tract culminates in the thalamus and is important for the localisation of
painful and thermal stimuli. Secondly, the spinoreticular tract culminates
in the reticular formation and is responsible for facilitating alertness and
physiological arousal in response to pain. Finally, the spinotectal tract
culminates in the tectum and directs attention toward the stimuli.
92
Somatosensation
93
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Chemical senses
The chemical senses are those which provide information concerning the
chemical content of the environment (Anderson et al., 2003; Gibson & Garbers,
2000; Goldstein, 2009; Pinel, 2003). These include olfaction (smell) and gustation
(taste), although you should remember that these senses are intrinsically linked.
These systems are also organised hierarchically according to functionally
segregated structures and incorporate parallel processing. Gibson and Garbers
(2000) have argued that there are over 1000 olfactory receptors in the nose
which each respond to different types of odour. These receptors are located in
a layer of mucus-covered tissue called the olfactory mucosa. The axons of these
receptors pass through the cribriform plate of the skull into the olfactory bulbs
where they synapse on the neurons of mitral cells, which project via the olfactory
tracts to several structures in the medial temporal lobes including the amygdala
and piriform cortex (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). You should be aware that the
correct term for an inability to detect odours is anosmia which can be caused
through damage to this system.
94
Chemical senses
In contrast, the receptors for taste are located on the tongue and are often
found around small protuberances called papillae. Conventionally, there are
five different tastes, which are sweet, salty, sour, bitter and unami (savoury or
meaty) although it is notable that many flavours cannot be experienced by
combining these tastes, suggesting that this list is incomplete (Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003). Unlike olfaction receptors, these taste buds do not have their own
axons but rather cluster together. These afferents leave the mouth as part of the
facial, glossopharyngeal and vagus cranial nerves and terminate in the solitary
nucleus of the medulla. Here they synapse on neurons which project to the
ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus and subsequently to the primary and
secondary gustatory cortex. The correct term for an inability to taste is called
ageasia which can be caused through damage to this system.
95
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
96
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Notes
97
5 • Mechanisms of perception and sensation
Notes
98
6
Biological mechanisms of
sleep and dreaming
• Normal wakefulness
• Stage 1 sleep
Stages of • Stage 2 sleep
awareness • Slow-wave sleep: stages 3 and 4
and sleep
• REM sleep
Sleep
disorders
99
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Introduction
The topics of sleep, dreaming and levels of awareness have been prominent
areas of research and theorisation in philosophy and science for centuries.
Indeed, these areas form significant components of research investigating
the nature and scope of consciousness and have generated considerable
research and debate (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005; Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
Sleep can be defined as a natural and potentially vital state which inevitably
follows a period of wakefulness in healthy mammals, birds, reptiles, fish and
amphibians. Sleep can also be defined as a series of transient states, meaning
that the individual can rapidly switch from various stages of sleep to normal
wakefulness unlike in other states of altered consciousness such as coma and
unconsciousness (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005; Espa, Ondze, Billiard & Bessett,
2000). This transition is usually easily observed and is signalled by a variety
of physiological and behavioural indicators. Sleep can be characterised as a
state in which normal wakefulness, awareness, responsiveness, consciousness,
sensation and voluntary control of the body is no longer evident (Pace-Schott,
Solms, Blagrove & Harnald 2003). Indeed, unlike in wakeful states an individual
is less able to respond to mild or moderate stimuli but may still perceive more
pronounced changes via the somatosensory system if in the stages of waking.
It is this inability to interact with the environment around us while in a reduced
state of consciousness which limits the chance of injury. However, it is important
to remember that the brain and our physiology responses remain active
(although often altered) throughout the stages of sleep and it is this activity
which facilitates the ability to dream (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005; Stickgold,
2005). Dreams can be defined as the perception of sensory information
while sleeping although the mechanisms and processes which give rise to
dreaming are still unascertained. It is also important to remember that sleep is
characterised by a heightened anabolic state which promotes the recovery and
growth of bodily systems, demonstrating its importance for development and
maintenance of the body.
You may also be aware that sleep and dreaming have been studied from
numerous perspectives in psychology, including from the biological, cognitive
and psychodynamic perspectives. However, this chapter will primarily provide
you with a summary of the main features of sleep, dreaming and awareness
according to a biological perspective. It will be concerned with the physiological,
anatomical and neurological aspects of these phenomena rather than non-
biological factors. Furthermore, while the area has been investigated since the
early days of psychology, this chapter will provide you with a contemporary
summary of sleep and dreaming, not an historical overview. While early studies
tended to investigate the effects of brain lesions on sleep and dreaming
in laboratory animals, this technique is mostly redundant due to the more
advanced techniques and measures used in human sleep laboratories and will
100
Introduction
not be discussed in detail (Pace-Schott et al., 2003). You should also remember
that it would be impossible to incorporate all of the information on sleep and
dreaming in one revision chapter and that the area is constantly evolving as new
research and theories are established. As such, you should remember to direct
your further reading to maintain a contemporary understanding.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions in the area of sleep and dreaming will typically ask you
to discuss the validity and reliability of certain theories or methodologies,
evaluate the insights into normal sleep and dreaming based on the study of
sleep disorders, or critically evaluate the extent to which research in this area
can contribute towards understanding consciousness. For essays like this you
will need to demonstrate a broad understanding of the theories, techniques,
research findings, strengths and limitations of this field of study. This will mean
clearly defining all of the technical terms and theories that you mention in your
essay, supporting all of your claims with evidence and critically evaluating the
theories and research you incorporate. However, you should also remember
that it has generated considerable amounts of literature and while your initial
understanding may be based on older research you should make sure that you
have an understanding of recent theoretical and methodological developments.
Important points to remember include:
MM Conduct a detailed literature review which is relevant to the question.
MM Make sure you understand the literature and how it applies to the issues you
are discussing. An essay plan will help you to organise your ideas.
MM Take an evidence-based approach. This means supporting all of your claims
and points with appropriate citations in text.
MM Take a critical approach. This means that you must evaluate all of the theories
and evidence and frame your argument accordingly. For example, does a
certain study support your argument while most of the others refute it?
MM Stay on topic. It is easy to get lost in your own narrative but you need to state
explicitly how each section has answered the question.
101
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
MM Stick to the conventions of your institution. Assignment time is not the time to
try out new (and often incorrect) styles, formats or referencing.
MM Draw conclusions. The ability to draw conclusions based on the evidence is
often part of the marking critera.
MM Proof-read your essay. Carelessness, errors and omissions will be detected by
your marker and, depending upon your institution’s guidelines, could reduce
your mark. This is also a good practice to get into if you ever intend to submit
work for publication.
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress.
102
Stages of awareness and sleep
skipped an individual can feel as though they have not slept. These stages will
now be discussed in more detail to refresh your memory. They consist of:
MM normal wakefulness
MM non-REM sleep (stages 1–4 which includes slow-wave sleep)
MM REM sleep (stage 5).
Key terms
Non-REM sleep: Constituting stages 1–4 of normal sleep which is not characterised
by rapid eye movements.
Normal wakefulness: Full consciousness and engagement with the environment.
Polysomnography: The measurement of physiological activity during sleep.
REM sleep: Stage 5 of sleep characterised by rapid eye movement.
Normal wakefulness
Normal wakefulness refers to the conscious state we are in most of the time.
In this state we can think, perceive, move, vocalise, problem solve and reason
by conscious thought. It is characterised by two forms of EEG. These are called
beta activity and alpha activity. The following sections and key terms box should
refresh your understanding of these patterns of activity.
Stage 1 sleep
This is the initial stage observed as individuals begin to fall asleep. EEG indicates
that brain activity gradually slows to a frequency of 3–7.5 Hz. This level of
activation is referred to as theta waves. It is occasionally also called somnolence
or drowsy sleep.
Stage 2 sleep
Stage 2 sleep is characterised by waves of activity at the same frequency as that
observed during stage 1 sleep, but also features small bursts of faster activity and
large spikes of activity. These are referred to as sleep spindles and K complexes
respectively. There is a notable decrease in muscle activity and conscious
experience of external stimuli. However, individuals are unlikely to realise that
they have been asleep if they are disturbed during the first two stages of sleep.
103
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
REM sleep
After stage 4 the sleep cycle returns to stage 1. However, at this point the
muscles are relaxed to a much greater degree, preventing any voluntary
movement. At the same time, heart rate, breathing and the activity of
sympathetic nervous system all increase to the level usually observed during
physical exertion. At this stage beta activity is also observed (Pinel, 2003),
demonstrating that the brain appears to be actively processing information.
This stage of sleep gains its name from the observable phenomenon of rapid
eye movements in which the eyeball can be seen to move quickly despite eyes
being closed. REM sleep is also distinct from the other stages of sleep in that
it is the stage in which dreams are experienced. Indeed, EEG has revealed that
the hippocampus which is involved in memory processes produces a theta of
4–7 Hz during REM sleep, suggesting that some form of memory retrieval or
consolidation may occur during this stage of sleep (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005;
Stickgold, 2005). While individuals in REM sleep are often difficult to waken and
will appear more disoriented than if woken from earlier stages of sleep, they
remain receptive to personally salient information.
Key terms
Alpha activity: This is the pattern of brain activity observed when an individual is
in a state of relaxation. It is observed while the eyes are closed, implying that the
individual is also in a state of relative inactivity. This activity is considerably slower
at 8–12 Hz and the levels of activity observed in the various areas of the brain is
relatively synchronised.
Beta activity: A pattern of activity observed when actively engaging in mental activity
characterised by 13–30 Hz. Activity is desynchronised in that areas of the brain vary in
their levels and pattern of activity.
t
104
Biological correlates of sleep and dreaming
Delta activity: This is brain activity at less than 3 Hz but with higher amplitude than
the earlier stages of sleep. Observed during stages 3 and 4 of sleep.
K complexes: Large spikes of activity initially observed during stage 2 sleep and
declining in stage 3.
Sleep spindles: Bursts of faster activity initially observed during stage 2 sleep and
declining in stage 3.
Slow-wave sleep: The combined stages 3 and 4 in which brain activity is less than 3 Hz.
Theta activity: This is brain activity observed during stage 1 sleep and is
characterised by a frequency of 3–7.5 Hz.
You will need to understand and be able to discuss some of the biological
factors which may control or regulate sleep and dreaming. The following
sections and ‘Test your knowledge’ questions should refresh your memory on
these topics.
105
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Key terms
Circadian clock: A hypothetical biological mechanism which is theorised to control
sleep–waking patterns and the other biological prerequisites for sleep (such
as temperature change, the release of growth hormones and the secretion of
neurotransmitters).
Circadian rhythm: A behavioural or physiological process which changes daily
according to a set pattern, such as the sleep–wake cycle.
Zeitgebers: Stimuli which can reset the circadian clock and circadian rhythms. These
usually come in the form of changing light.
KEY STUDY
106
Biological correlates of sleep and dreaming
107
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Functions of sleep
Several theories have been proposed to explain the functions of sleep and
dreaming. To help you establish academic debates in your assignments and
exams, those discussed in this text will enable you to revise the biological and
psychological theories of sleep and dreaming.
108
Functions of sleep
aware, if sleep were not required for survival, researchers would have observed
animals which do not require sleep or benefit from it, or suffer consequences
when deprived of sleep. This is not the case, suggesting that sleep is of vital
importance for wellbeing and survival of all animal species. Table 6.1 shows the
main theories concerning the functions of sleep.
Theory Description
Restorative and reparative The restorative theories of sleep state that sleep is needed to
functions enable the repair of bodily structures, systems and functions.
This includes the endocrine, central and peripheral nervous
systems. For example, Zager, Anderson, Ruiz, Antunes and
Tufik (2007) observed that sleep-deprived rats had 20 per
cent fewer white blood cells than rats which had slept. This
demonstrated a significant difference in the immune system
but may have arisen due to stress rather than sleep deprivation
per se. Growth hormones are released during sleep,
promoting the restoration of bodily structures and functions.
Neurotransmitters may also be replenished during REM sleep
which is discussed in more detail in the final chapter. However,
Horne (1978) reviewed over 50 sleep deprivation studies
and observed that it does not appear to impair the ability
to perform physical activities and does not produce a stress
response. This is significant evidence against the restorative
theories of sleep and dreaming.
Neurobiological functions These theories state that sleep and dreaming serve the
function of promoting and restoring cognitive functions such
as the storage, integration, organisation and processing
of information. For example, the reverse learning theory
proposed that dreams function as a means of sorting the
information encountered during the day and filtering out any
information which is no longer needed. This would conserve
storage (Crick & Mitchison, 1983). However, it is important to
remember that dreams can be incoherent and meaningless.
Another possibility is that sleep has a beneficial effect on
working memory. For example, Turner, Drummond, Salamat
and Brown (2007) also observed that performance on working
memory tests showed a 38 per cent decline in individuals
who were deprived of sleep compared to control subjects.
However, you should remember that these effects may result
from general tiredness rather than processes which occur
during sleep.
109
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
KEY STUDY
110
Functions of sleep
111
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Sleep deprivation
There are several different ways in which an individual may become sleep
deprived in the absence of a sleep disorder. These include intentional
deprivation in laboratory or similar settings, jet lag and unsociable work
patterns (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005). The effects of sleep deprivation are still
open to debate and you should remember that hundreds of studies have been
conducted in this area with variable results.
KEY STUDY
Sleep debt
Gulevich et al. (1966) reported a study of a boy who stayed awake for 264 hours
to obtain a place in the Guinness Book of Records. Despite this significant sleep
deprivation, the boy only needed to sleep for almost 15 hours the following night
before waking and feeling rested. He slept just over ten hours the subsequent night
and nine hours on the third night demonstrating that 67 hours of lost sleep were never
recovered. However, what was most interesting about this pattern of sleep was that the
percentage of hours recovered was not equal across all of the stages of sleep. There
was also evidence for the ‘REM rebound’ in which more time is spent in REM sleep than
earlier stages of sleep after deprivation:
MM 7 per cent of stage 1–2 sleep
MM 68 per cent of slow-wave sleep
MM 53 per cent of REM sleep.
While the boy did not appear to suffer long-term consequences of sleep deprivation,
he did experience slurred speech, visual impairment and mild paranoia during his time
awake. However, you should remember that there are significant individual differences
in how a person responds to sleep deprivation and there have been reported cases
in which individuals have experienced delusions, hallucinations, severe cognitive
impairment and other psychological difficulties (Belcher & Moorcroft, 2005; Pace-
Schott et al., 2003).
112
Functions of sleep
Shift work
Individuals who work shifts may suffer several psychological impairments
including in their abilities to concentrate and perform tasks correctly (Gallo &
Eastman, 1993; Nicholson & D’Auria, 1999; Sack & Lewy, 1997). In severe cases
shift-work sleep disorder can also develop, which is characterised by insomnia
and excessive drowsiness if shifts are repeatedly scheduled during normal
sleeping times. Even when rotating shifts, altering the circadian rhythm is difficult
to do without eliciting consequences on the shift worker’s health (Gallo &
Eastman, 1993; Nicholson & D’Auria, 1999; Sack & Lewy 1997). You should also
remember that the pattern of shift work is important if the timetable rotates. For
example, it is easier for the employee to do an early then a late shift than if they
do a late shift followed by an early one.
Jet lag
Jet lag is a problem which arises when flying long distances into a different
time zone which displaces the circadian clock, resulting in tiredness and the
drive to sleep at what would be the normal time in the original time zone (Cho,
Ennaceur, Cole & Suh, 2000; Reilly, Atkinson & Waterhouse, 1997). The circadian
clock normally runs at 25 hours (rather than 24 hours) meaning that it is easier
to stay awake past the normal time we sleep than to wake up at what feels like
an earlier time. This can result in symptoms of sleep deprivation when travelling
long distances in an eastern direction but few detrimental effects when travelling
in a western direction. Cho et al. (2000) also observed that chronic jet lag (as
experienced by cabin crew) can result in cognitive deficits.
113
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Critically discuss the claim that sleep deprivation can seriously impair the
ability to function normally.
Sleep disorders
To what extent does the study of sleep disorders inform the understanding
of the normal mechanisms, processes and functions of sleep?
114
Sleep disorders
Condition Description
115
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Sleep paralysis Sleep paralysis is the condition in which the muscle atonia which
normally occurs during REM sleep actually occurs when going to
sleep (hypnagogic) or waking up (hypnopompic). It can cause states
of extreme panic, and in severe cases it can occur in conjunction with
terrifying hallucinations. However, while it is important to remember that
most individuals will experience this at some point in their lives, others
experience it much more frequently.
Nocturia Nocturia refers to frequent waking due to the need to urinate, which can
result in sleep deprivation. This condition is caused by drinking too much
or due to certain medical conditions.
Enuresis Enuresis refers to persistent bedwetting. However, it does not refer
to childhood bedwetting which is often the result of inexperience in
interpreting physiological cues or inability to wake at appropriate times.
In primary enuresis, bedwetting has not stopped since childhood, while
in secondary enuresis the condition arises after a long period of dryness.
Possible causes of secondary enuresis include emotional distress, while
primary enuresis may arise due to a lack of nocturnal anti-diuretic
hormone or bladder problems.
MCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
MAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
MGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
116
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Answer guidelines
117
6 • Biological mechanisms of sleep and dreaming
Notes
118
7
Biological aspects of
emotion
Feeling
emotion: the
James–Lange
theory
The
biology
of fear
The
Biological
biology of
aspects of
anger and
emotion
aggression
Hormones
and
emotion
Communication
of emotion
119
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
Introduction
This chapter will aid your revision of the biological factors associated with
emotional experiences and expressions. You should already remember that
emotion can be defined as a positive or negative feeling in response to an
internal or external stimulus which is characterised by physiological changes
and species-typical behaviour (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). There are also
three distinct and interacting components of emotion which will be discussed
in this chapter. These are the behavioural, autonomic and hormonal aspects
of emotional states. In particular, this chapter focuses on the biological
factors associated with states such as fear and anger. These are the most
studied emotional states in biological psychology because they can be clearly
differentiated from baseline measures and have the clearest evolutionary origins
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). In contrast, positive emotions are considerably
harder to define and measure as they are less pronounced, and these states tend
to be studied in other areas such as social psychology. This chapter also reviews
the biological factors associated with the expression of emotions and revisits the
theme of hormonal influences on behaviour which were previously covered in
Chapter 3. You should also remember that the content of Chapters 1–3 is also
relevant to this topic because emotion relies heavily on the CNS, PNS, endocrine
system and approaches in biological psychology. More extreme, maladaptive
states are considered in Chapter 9. However, again you should remember that
cognition, social influences and individual differences exert significant influences
on the experience and expression of emotion and you should read around the
topic to acquire a comprehensive understanding of emotion.
Revision checklist
Key terms
Emotion: A positive or negative feeling in response to an internal or external stimulus
which is characterised by physiological changes and species-typical behaviour.
120
Feeling emotion: the James–Lange theory
Assessment advice
Essay questions on the topic of emotion will usually ask you to compare, contrast
and evaluate two different approaches to studying emotion or to evaluate
theoretical perspectives. This may include discussions of neuropsychology,
physiological psychology, cognitive neuroscience, pharmacology, cognitive
psychology, social psychology and the James–Lange theory. This means that
you will need to acquire a broad understanding of the topic. Most essays will
also expect you to discuss how these approaches or theories explain two or
more emotional states using examples from the literature. For example, you
would need to discuss whether one approach explains more about an emotional
state than others, whether each approach provides a complementary level of
analysis or contradicts another and whether there are significant methodological
issues with any of the approaches. It is also vital that you adopt a critical and
evidence-based view which reviews all of the approaches and evidence included
in your response. Remember that your essays should also have an ‘hour-glassed’
structure. This means that the essay should start as a broad introduction which
narrows down to the specific issues and points in the middle of the essay and
then builds on your argument until you reach your conclusion.
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
121
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
states on behavioural and physiological cues rather than the alternative view that
emotional states cause behavioural and physiological changes (Carlson, 2004;
Oatley et al., 2006). This may be difficult to understand, but if you consider
the number of times you have found yourself to be happy, sad, angry, stressed
or anxious without knowing the cause you may be able to appreciate this
perspective. You may have also found yourself crying in a situation where you
cannot understand why you have been affected although your mood may have
consequently changed (Carlson, 2004). Interestingly, the physiological feedback
from facial expressions does appear to alter the activity of the autonomic
nervous system (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006). The following Key Study will
help you to understand this phenomenon.
KEY STUDY
Therefore, based on these findings and consistent with the James–Lange theory,
physiological feedback from the behavioural components of emotion did appear
to inform emotional states. However, it is vital that you consider both sides of
this argument and it is equally possible that causation works in the opposite
direction or that both are possible (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006; Pinel,
2003). For example, the participants may have guessed which expression they
were meant to portray and induced a suitable, temporary mood-state to help
them to express this emotion. They may have already been in the mood-state
and there is nothing to say that the components of emotion have causal rather
than correlational effects (Oatley et al., 2006; Mauss & Robinson, 2009; Russell,
2003). It is also significant that this is based on anecdotal evidence and artificial
laboratory experiments. Performance may be different in more naturalistic
settings.
Key term
James–Lange theory: A theory in which emotional behaviour and physiological
responses are directly elicited by situations and that ‘feelings’ are the result of
feedback from these behavioural and physiological responses.
122
The biology of fear
123
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
refresh your memory of the complete system and these physiological responses,
consult Figure 2.3 in Chapter 2. This section will primarily discuss the cerebral
structures associated with the fear response to expand your revision of this topic.
You will probably already be aware that the amygdala is one of the most
significant cerebral structures in the fear response and shows significantly
increased levels of activation in response to threat (Carlson, 2004; Phelps et
al., 2001). This structure has also been associated with the perception of pain
and the processing of potentially harmful stimuli, demonstrating its importance
for fight-or-flight and survival (Carlson, 2004; Phelps et al., 2001; Pinel, 2003).
However, the amygdala can be divided into several regions which serve different
functions and pathways during the fear response (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
Firstly, the medial nucleus receives sensory information and relays these signals
to the medial basal forebrain and the hypothalamus. Secondly, the lateral nucleus
receives sensory information from the neocortex, thalamus and hippocampus
and projects these signals to the basal, accessory basal and central nucleus
regions. Finally, the central nucleus receives information from various regions and
transmits these signals to numerous cerebral regions including the hypothalamus,
midbrain, pons and medulla. The central nucleus and associated structures
are vital for the expression of all three components of emotion (behavioural,
autonomic and hormonal). If the central nucleus is damaged, animals (including
humans) are no longer able to show the fear or startle responses, they tend
to behave tamely and in the case of humans do not show the usual effects
of emotion on memory such as the formation of emotionally driven flashbulb
memories and mood-state dependent memory (Carlson, 2004). However, while
they show lower levels of stress they are also more likely to be harmed by their
environment through lack of fear (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). In contrast, if the
central nucleus is electrically stimulated, animals show fear, aggression, arousal
and agitation, making them more confrontational and more likely to both harm
and be harmed by others in the environment (Carlson, 2004). This demonstrates
the importance of the balance in this structure for facilitating survival.
However, you should remember that the amygdala transmits signals to several
other regions of the brain which are also associated with the fear response.
These include:
MP Lateral hypothalamus: Involved in the activation of the sympathetic nervous
system. Increases heart rate, blood pressure and paleness.
MP Dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus: Involved in the activation of the
parasympathetic nervous system. Associated with the formation of ulcers,
urination and defecations.
MP Parabrachial nucleus: Increases respiration.
MP Ventral tegmental area: Involved in the regulation and release of dopamine
and promotes behavioural arousal.
MP Locus coeruleus: Involved in regulation and release of norepinephrine and
promotes vigilance.
124
The biology of fear
Critically discuss the insights into fear which have been provided by biological
psychology.
125
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
7.5 Which area of the amygdala relays signals to the basal medial forebrain?
7.6 Which area of the amygdala relays signals to the thalamus and
hippocampus?
7.7 What functions are served by the lateral hypothalamus in the fear
response?
7.8 Which types of receptors have unusually high concentrations in the
amygdala?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Anger and aggression are observed in nearly every animal species and appear
to have neural, physiological and genetic mediators (Carver & Harmon-Jones,
2009; Huer & Kravitz, 2010; Potegal, Stemmler & Spielberger, 2010). Anger can
also occur in conjunction with fear and anxiety but exists as an emotional state in
its own right (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009). However, the expression of anger
and aggression is species-typical and may consist of threat behaviours, defensive
behaviours and submissive behaviours (Carlson, 2004). Typically, threatening
displays outweigh actual attacks and some form of agreement is reached prior
to physical violence. Some species also show predation in which stalking and
aggression are exhibited towards members of another species. However, this is
the means of acquiring food and is usually a product of necessity rather than a
display of anger (Carlson, 2004; Potegal et al., 2010).
You should remember that the physical expression of anger and aggression
appears to be restricted by the biological constraints of the species, their neural
circuits, the hypothalamus, amygdala and by the overall limbic system which
receives sensory information (Huer & Kravitz, 2010; Potegal et al., 2010). For
example, predatory and defensive behaviours can be induced through the
stimulation of the periaqueductal grey matter, amygdala and hypothalamus
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). It is also significant that increasing the activity of
serotonergic synapses of rats inhibits anger and aggression, while damaging the
serotonergic axons in their brains elicits anger and aggression (Carlson, 2004;
Vergnes, Depaulis, Boehrer & Kempf 1988). Similar effects of serotonin have
also been observed in species, even to the extent of predicting their survival
(Bouwknecht et al., 2001; Carlson, 2004).
126
The biology of anger and aggression
For example, animals with high levels of serotonin tend to be less aggressive
and less confrontational meaning that they are more likely to survive (Carlson,
2004; Pinel, 2003). However, you should already be thinking critically and be
considering whether these findings can be generalised to humans who live in
potentially more constrained societies. Interestingly, this is a false assumption
and serotonin does appear to mediate human anger and aggression in a similar
way to that of other animals (Bouwknecht et al., 2001; Carlson, 2004; Carver
& Harmon-Jones, 2009; Huber & Kravitz, 2010; Pinel, 2003; Potegal et al.,
2010). You will already know that increasing serotonin levels through the use of
agonists such as Prozac reduces irritability, aggression, impulsive behaviours,
anxiety and depression (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006; Pinel, 2003). This
suggests that serotonin serves a significant inhibitory effect for anger and
aggression in most species.
However, most of us are able to control how we express anger in most situations
because we are also guided by social norms, morality, cognition, reason and
signals from other cerebral structures. Most importantly, the recognition of
the emotional significance of situations, regulation of emotional expression,
rationality and control of arousal are some of the main functions of the prefrontal
cortex (Anderson, Bechara, Damasio, Tranel & Damasio, 1999; Carlson, 2004;
Pinel, 2003; Potegal et al., 2010). The orbitofrontal cortex in particular receives
information from the dorsal medial thalamus, temporal cortex, ventral tegmental
area and amygdala. This structure acts as an interface between automatic
emotional responses and the control of more complex behaviours so is able
to regulate emotional expression (Carlson, 2004; Huber & Kravitz, 2010; Pinel,
2003; Potegal et al., 2010). In addition, lesions or deficits in the prefrontal cortex
can result in poor moral judgement, insensitivity to consequences of one’s own
actions, anti-social behaviour and aggression (Anderson et al., 1999; Carlson,
2004; Pinel, 2003; Raine et al., 2002).
To what extent are the experience and expression of anger dependent upon
biological factors?
127
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
7.9 How does serotonin relate to the level of anger and aggression?
7.10 What is the role of the prefrontal cortex in anger and aggression?
7.11 What are species-typical behaviours?
7.12 What would a low level of serotonin produce?
7.13 What can lesions to the prefrontal cortex produce?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
128
Hormones and emotion
2009). This can make people more susceptible to these emotions as the neural
circuits become sensitive to lower thresholds through repeated activation
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
In regards to anger and aggression, it is interesting that androgens are released
prenatally but then decline until puberty when the levels of these hormones
significantly increase. It is also in this period that increased irritability, anger,
sadness, anxiety and aggression are often observed in most species including
humans (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006; Pinel, 2003). Early exposure to
androgens also has a lasting effect in that it configures neural pathways to be
more responsive to testosterone which facilitates aggression in most species
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). In regards to humans, you will already be aware that
boys tend to be more assertive and aggressive than girls. As aggression in every
other species is mediated by androgens it would be parsimonious to assume
that hormones also influence human anger and aggression (Carlson, 2004;
Oatley et al., 2006; Pinel, 2003; Potegal et al., 2010).
However, you should keep in mind that socialisation and the adoption of gender
roles is likely to play a large role in determining the expression of emotion in
humans (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003; Potegal et al., 2010). It is also difficult to
assess the effects of hormones on human emotional experience and expression
because it would be unethical to alter levels of hormones for any significant
amount of time and studies would not be able to isolate the effects of hormones
from those of other factors, meaning that cause and effect could not be inferred.
129
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
Communication of emotion
The expression of emotion is one of the most common human behaviours and,
although it is often unconscious and unintentional, some aspects of emotional
expression can be produced voluntarily. For examples, humans express emotion
through facial expressions, touch, gestures and language (Carlson, 2004;
Hertenstein, Holmes, McCullough & Keltner, 2009; Lewis, 2011). Other animals
are also able to communicate their emotions using pheromones, which were
discussed previously. You may already know that the expression of emotion
may have served evolutionary functions of promoting closeness through love,
affection and happiness and promoting survival through anger, aggression and
fear (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). Several findings appear to support this claim:
MP Infants actively seek, observe and imitate emotional expressions of those
around them from very shortly after birth (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
MP Matsumoto and Willingham (2009) have observed spontaneous emotional
facial expression in blind individuals despite the lack of visual cues for the
behaviours (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006).
MP The communication and recognition of core emotional expressions appears
to be consistent across cultures despite different cultural exposure, although
there are variations in the display rules for which emotions are socially
acceptable in a culture (Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006).
This suggests that the motivation to seek, learn and communicate emotion may
be pre-programmed into the human genome and supports Darwin’s theory
that emotional expressions are innate, evolved, unlearnt and complex gestures
(Carlson, 2004; Oatley et al., 2006; Pinel, 2003). However, it also demonstrates
how these predispositions can be mediated by cognitive and social motivations
(Oatley et al., 2006).
In biological terms, the right hemisphere is more dominant in the expression
and recognition of emotions and this includes both visual and auditory cues
(Carlson, 2004). For example, Blonder, Bowers and Heilman (1991) observed that
130
Communication of emotion
Critically discuss the claim that the communication of emotion is innate and
relies purely on biological factors.
131
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
132
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Notes
133
7 • Biological aspects of emotion
Notes
134
8 The biology of learning
and memory
What are
learning and
memory?
Where are
memories
stored in
the brain?
The biology
of learning
and memory
Examples in
neuropsychology
Limitations of
neuropsychology
135
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Introduction
This chapter will aid your revision of the theories and biological aspects of learning
and memory. It will discuss some of the insights which have been gained from
approaches such as neuropsychology, neuroscience and physiological psychology.
You should already have a good understanding of these approaches from previous
chapters. More specifically, it will allow you to revise some of the biological
structures and mechanisms which facilitate, contribute towards or arise during
normal and impaired learning and memory processes. However, as you will already
be aware, there are strong interdisciplinary links with topics such as cognitive
psychology. This chapter is directed towards providing a review of the more
applied aspects of this topic due to the focus on biological factors and you should
consult Cognitive Psychology (Jonathan Ling and Jonathan Catling) in this series
for alternative approaches to studying memory and learning.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
Essay questions in this area will usually ask you to discuss which biological
factors have been associated with human memory or to evaluate the
contributions of an approach such as neuropsychology or neuroimaging towards
an understanding of memory and learning. Important aspects to remember
when writing essays in this area include:
MM Provide a clear introduction which summarises the evidence and arguments
you will discuss. This includes informing the marker of the perspective you
intend to adopt and demonstrating that you have understood the question.
MM Summarise the theories and the evidence in a coherent, well-informed and
critical manner. This means explicitly comparing and contrasting perspectives
and alternative evidence and drawing conclusions concerning the validity and
reliability of these factors.
MM Your essay should demonstrate a balance between description and evaluation.
It is not enough to only include descriptions of research and theories while
not evaluating these influences. At the same time, it is not enough to write
critically while neglecting the details of the research and theories.
136
What are learning and memory?
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could arise
on this topic.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
You should already know that learning refers to how experiences of the world
change the structure and activity of neural networks within the brain (Carlson,
2004). In contrast, memory refers to how these changes are stored and how
the information is subsequently reactivated during the retrieval of memories
(Henke, 2010). However, it is important for you to know that memory can also
be subdivided in several different ways. These terms are defined in the Glossary
and include:
MM Short-term memory: Information which is held in consciousness for a brief
amount of time, usually through immediate and continuous rehearsal (Baddeley,
Eysenck & Anderson, 2009). This includes iconic memory and echoic memory.
MM Long-term memory: Information which is stored in the brain for a long period
of time and retrieved at a later date despite lacking continuing rehearsal
in the interval between encoding, storage and retrieval (Baddeley et al.,
2009). These memories can be elaborated over time as new information is
encountered and processed.
MM Implicit/procedural memory: Knowledge which is not easily vocalised;
this includes procedural knowledge such as how to ride a bike and most
information which is not declarative (Henke, 2010; Voss & Paller, 2008).
137
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Key terms
Echoic memory: Short-term memory for information in the auditory field.
Iconic memory: Short-term memory for information in the visual field.
Learning: Learning is concerned with the process in which our experiences of the
environment change the pattern and structure of activation in brain.
Memory: How the changes in the brain experienced during learning are stored
and subsequently reactivated during the retrieval of information. Memory has been
subdivided into various forms including autobiographical, semantic, episodic, implicit,
explicit and prospective memory.
138
Where are memories stored in the brain?
139
8 • The biology of learning and memory
areas of the brain. However, you should always remember that this doesn’t
discount the possibility that other areas of the brain are also involved in these
forms of processing (Cohen, Johnstone & Plunkett, 2002). The following bullet
points should refresh your memory concerning some of the areas which have
been associated with certain forms of memory:
MM Hippocampus: Associative memory and knowledge concerning the
relationship between objects and places (Henke, 2010).
MM Rhinal cortex: Object recognition memory (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003).
MM Inferotemporal cortex: The secondary sensory cortex located in this area has
been associated with the storage and processing of sensory memories, while
the broader inferotemporal cortex is associated with visual perception of
objects and the storage of visual patterns (Naya, Yoshida & Miyashita, 2001).
MM Amygdala: Associated with the encoding and storage of emotional memories
and startle responses (Carlson, 2004).
MM Prefrontal cortex: Associated with procedural knowledge, working memory
and remembering the sequences of events (Smith, 2000).
MM Cerebellum: Associated with the processing and storage of implicit memories
and sensorimotor skills (Salmon & Butters, 1995).
MM Striatum: Associated with habit learning and the processing and storage
of memories concerning the relationship between stimuli and responses
(Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003; Salmon & Butters, 1995).
However, it is also important for you to understand how memories are stored
in these areas. The simple answer is through the processes which have already
been discussed. Long-term potentiation results in lasting changes to the efficacy
of synaptic transmission, facilitating memory formation and changing the neural
pathways of the structures involved in their acquisition (Carlson, 2004; Pinel,
2003; Salmon & Butters, 1995; Smith, 2000). This means that memories would
not be stored as unified, abstract packages of information; instead they are
the product of various neurons firing at the same time and are stored diffusely
in these connections (Baddeley et al., 2009; Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). It
is also important to remember that the neurotransmitter glutamate and the
corresponding N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors significantly contribute
towards learning and memory (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). This is the main
excitatory system in the brain and the NMDA receptors respond maximally when
these two events occur simultaneously. Two events form the basis of learning
and typically require high-frequency and high-intensity stimulation as observed
during intentional learning:
MM Glutamate binds to the NMDA receptors.
MM The postsynaptic neuron is already partially depolarised (activated) triggering
action potentials and calcium ions which are required for long-term
potentiation.
140
Examples in neuropsychology
Critically evaluate the claim that memories are stored diffusely in the cerebral
structures which were involved in their acquisition.
Examples in neuropsychology
141
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Key terms
Anterograde amnesia: Inability to learn and remember information encountered after
brain damage.
Retrograde amnesia: Inability to retrieve memories which were acquired before brain
damage.
Patient H.M.
You will probably already know that the multitudes of studies which have
assessed patient H.M. have arguably provided the most extensive insights into
the biological basis of memory and learning (Baddeley et al., 2009; Pinel, 2003).
H.M. was an intelligent adult who suffered from severe epilepsy with seizures
originating from both temporal lobes. As a result, doctors performed a bilateral
medial temporal lobotomy in which most of the hippocampus, amygdala and
adjacent cortex were removed to reduce the number and severity of seizures. He
initially adjusted well after the surgery, with his intelligence quotient increasing
from 104 to 118, presumably due to the reduction of seizures. H.M.’s memory
for events prior to his surgery was relatively intact but he suffered severe
anterograde amnesia. For example, if he met someone who subsequently left
the room, he could not remember this person when they returned and would
not be able to remember what they had been doing. More specifically, H.M.
was unable to form explicit long-term memories, demonstrating deficits in the
ability to consolidate information. Table 8.1 summarises H.M.’s performance on
several tasks and demonstrates that while he was able to hold a small amount
of information in short-term memory, any distraction, longer interval of time or
exposure to larger amount of information resulted in H.M. being unable to recall
the information.
Task Performance
Digit span +1 This tests short-term memory and requires that participants recite a
gradually increasing list of digits. While most people’s performance
improves, H.M. was unable to recall eight digits even after 25 trials.
This suggests that H.M. was unable to hold information in short-term
memory or that he was unable to consolidate the information to form
long-term memories.
Block tapping This tests memory span and requires participants to copy the
experimenter who indicates a sequence of blocks. H.M. was able
to learn the sequence of five blocks but was unable to learn the
sequence of six, even after 12 attempts. This suggests that H.M.
was unable to hold information in short-term memory or that he was
unable to consolidate the information to form long-term memories.
t
142
Examples in neuropsychology
Mirror drawing This is a test of implicit knowledge and requires that participants
learn how to trace an image over the course of several days. H.M.’s
performance improved despite the fact that he couldn’t remember
ever doing the task before, suggesting that sensorimotor skills were
intact despite a lack of declarative knowledge.
Rotary pursuit This is also a test of implicit memory in which participants must learn
to maintain contact between a stylus and a moving target. H.M.’s
performance improved, despite the fact that he couldn’t remember
ever doing the task before, suggesting that sensorimotor skills were
intact despite a lack of declarative knowledge.
Incomplete picture In this task participants must complete a drawing in which contours
have been removed. Again, H.M.’s performance improved even
though he wasn’t aware that he had seen the task before.
Conditioned fear H.M. was able to learn a conditioned blink response, which is another
response form of implicit knowledge. However, this response took longer to
learn than it does with healthy control subjects.
Clive Wearing
Clive Wearing experienced total amnesia after contracting Herpes Simplex
Encephalitis. Clive suffered both severe anterograde amnesia and severe
retrograde amnesia, which significantly impaired his ability to function (Campbell
& Conway, 1995). While he experienced islands of memories, these tended
to lack detail and were often incomplete, although his procedural knowledge
was relatively intact. As such, he was only able to process information in his
immediate consciousness and kept a minute-to-minute diary. He also suffered
mood swings due to the emotional turmoil and confusion caused by the
amnesia. Several documentaries have been made about Clive Wearing and
these are freely available on websites such as YouTube if you wish to see the
extent of his amnesia.
Patient A.C.
Patient A.C. experienced a series of strokes which resulted in severely impaired
perceptual object knowledge while non-perceptual knowledge remained
relatively intact (Coltheart, 2001; Coltheart et al., 1998). For example, A.C.
was able to describe any attribute of a stimuli except its visual properties. This
demonstrates how selective memory impairment can actually be.
Patient K.F.
Patient K.F. suffered damage to his left parieto-occipital region in a motorcycle
accident. While K.F.’s short-term memory was severely impaired, his long-term
memory remained largely unaffected. More specifically, his impairments were
primarily to his auditory–verbal short-term memory in that he was unable to repeat
sequences of digits read to him by experimenters (Shallice & Warrington, 1970).
143
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Korsakoff’s patients
Patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome demonstrate sensory deficits, motor
problems, confusion, amnesia, personality changes and have an increased risk
of death from liver, gastrointestinal or heart disease due to alcoholism (Carlson,
2004; Pinel, 2003). Post-mortems reveal lesions to the medial diencephalon
(medial thalamus and medial hypothalamus), neocortex, mammalian bodies and
cerebellum. While amnesia is initially anterograde, as the condition progresses
patients also develop retrograde amnesia.
Alzheimer’s disease
Alzheimer’s disease is initially characterised by a mild deterioration of memory
but progresses to dementia where all cognitive functions are impaired and
quality of life is significantly diminished. There are extensive changes in the brain
including the development of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (Haass,
1999). There is also evidence of atrophy in which the brain significantly reduces
in size and mass (Haass, 1999).
Key terms
Alzheimer’s disease: A medical condition which is associated with amyloid
plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the brain. It is initially characterised by a mild
deterioration of memory but progresses to dementia where all cognitive functions are
impaired and quality of life is significantly diminished.
Korsakoff’s syndrome: A medical condition produced through alcoholism in
which patients demonstrate sensory deficits, motor problems, confusion, amnesia
and personality changes. Patients also have an increased risk of death from liver,
gastrointestinal or heart disease.
144
Examples in neuropsychology
Concussion
Following a blunt force trauma to the head, individuals sometimes experience
retrograde amnesia for events just before the incident and anterograde amnesia
for a period afterwards (Carlson, 2004; Pinel, 2003). However, the duration and
extent of these deficits are dependent upon the extent of the injury. While
most cases of retrograde amnesia are resolved with time, people rarely recover
memories for things which occurred just before the trauma.
Electroconvulsive shock
Patients who undergo electroconvulsive shock (ECS) for conditions such as
obsessive compulsive disorder and major depression experience a degree of
retrograde amnesia and a period of confusion following the treatment. Most
interestingly, ECS appears to be a useful technique for investigating the time
required for memory consolidation in that longer gradients of retrograde
amnesia would suggest that memory consolidation is a longitudinal process.
This appears to be the case, suggesting that memories become gradually more
resilient to damage and interference (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Pinel, 2003).
CRItICAL FoCus
145
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Rats which have damage to the hippocampus are unable to learn the relationship between
the sample, new object and food source. However, you should remember that human
brain damage is rarely as isolated as this and generalisation is difficult across species.
Spatial memory
You should already know that lesions to the hippocampus have a detrimental
effect on memory for spatial locations and that there are specialised ‘place cells’
in the hippocampus which are only activated when in certain locations (Carlson,
2004; Wilson & McNaughton, 1993). This suggests that this structure may
mediate spatial memory. Indeed, Maguire, Frith, Burgess, Donnett & O’Keefe
(1998) identified that activation of the right hippocampal region was associated
with knowing where places were and navigating towards them. Furthermore,
Maguire et al. (2000) identified that after 20 years’ service, London taxi drivers
had significantly more posterior hippocampal grey matter than usual. However,
it is important to remember that these changes may give rise to better spatial
memory or result from these differences in memory, making it difficult to draw
conclusive arguments concerning the role of the hippocampus in spatial memory.
The current general consensus is that the rhinal cortex is associated with object
recognition while the hippocampus is associated with memories concerning the
spatial relationship between them (Henke, 2010).
Critically evaluate two or more theories concerning the role of the hippocampus
in memory for spatial locations.
146
Limitations of neuropsychology
8.10 H.M.’s performance may have been intact for which tasks?
8.11 What tests can be used in neuropsychology to assess specific
impairments?
8.12 Medial temporal lobectomy impairs performance on what task?
8.13 What are the limitations of case studies in neuropsychology?
8.14 Which substance is in short supply in the brains of patients with
Alzheimer’s?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Limitations of neuropsychology
147
8 • The biology of learning and memory
MCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
MAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
MGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
148
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
149
8 • The biology of learning and memory
Notes
150
9
Biological basis of
psychological abnormality
Historical
context
• Symptoms and diagnosis
Major • Biological aetiology
(unipolar)
depression • Non-biological aetiology
• Treatment
151
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Introduction
Abnormality is one of the most studied areas in psychology and attracts many
students to the specialism of clinical and counselling psychology. This is partially
due to the fact that everyone encounters some form of psychological distress
during their lifetime, whether this is through personal experience or through
the experiences of someone who is close to us (Carlson, 2004; Carr & McNulty,
2006; Comer, 2007). You should appreciate that abnormal behaviour is not
just studied from a biological perspective but also features prominently in
cognitive, developmental, humanistic and social psychology. Indeed, as you may
already be aware, contemporary approaches tend to endorse the diathesis–
stress model in which biological dispositions to mental illness are triggered by
environmental and cognitive factors (Carlson, 2004). For example, an imbalance
in the neurotransmitters serotonin and dopamine may predispose an individual
to depression, but this condition may not become evident until a major life
event triggers an episode. It is important that you remember this principle
throughout this chapter as any reports or assignments you write in this field
should demonstrate that you can write critically and that you fully appreciate
that multiple influences shape both normal and abnormal behaviour.
It is also vital that you can understand that the distinction between normal and
abnormal behaviour is constantly changing and is not absolute or universal
(Comer, 2007). Indeed, as with all areas of psychology, theoretical accounts of
abnormal behaviour are proposed, investigated, adapted or potentially rejected
based on theoretical shifts, social norms and emerging evidence. This also
applies to issues concerning which treatments are endorsed at any given time
and how the individual is perceived and treated.
Revision checklist
Assessment advice
The following points provide guidance concerning essay writing in abnormal
psychology. These points will help you to organise your responses to essay
questions and to develop an appropriate writing style:
152
Introduction
Sample question
Could you answer this question? Below is a typical essay question that could
arise on this topic.
Evaluate the extent to which the distinctions between normal and abnormal
behaviour are universally accepted rather than culturally specific.
Guidelines on answering this question are included at the end of this chapter,
whilst further guidance on tackling other exam questions can be found on the
companion website at: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
153
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Historical context
It was not that long ago that individuals suffering from psychological illnesses
were persecuted and hidden away in mental asylums where they were often
subjected to treatments which would be seen as inhumane, brutal and
reprehensible today (Comer, 2007; Pinel, 2003). Today, several psychological
organisations across the world have developed codes for ethical conduct in
both research and practice and the public’s interest also safeguards against
such maltreatment (Carr & McNulty, 2006; Comer, 2007). While sufferers often
report still feeling isolated, it is significant that mental illness is now discussed
more openly. Although the representation is occasionally inaccurate, mental
illness often features in popular media. In addition, treatment (such as through
psychopharmacology) is often community-based, especially in the case of
minor disorders (Carr & McNulty, 2006; McWilliams & O’Callaghan, 2006).
This should suggest to you that there have been significant developments in
promoting positive attitudes and aims to provide support for individuals who are
experiencing psychological turmoil. However, while mental illness is significantly
less stigmatised than it used to be, prejudice and discrimination often resurface.
Indeed, the view that homosexuality was dysfunctional and should be treated
as a psychological abnormality has only recently been rejected by mainstream
Western psychology. While discrimination has been reduced, it has not yet been
fully eradicated and other cultures often hold different perspectives (Carlson,
2004; Comer, 2007; Pinel, 2003). It is not within the scope of this chapter or
the job of a psychologist to say any cultural perspective is better than another;
you just need to be aware of these cross-cultural differences. Hence, it is
important that you remember that other generations and cultures often define
mental illness differently from that which is adopted in contemporary Western
psychology and that the findings from psychology can be used to both inform
and misinform public opinion. Indeed, Comer (2007) defined psychological
abnormality by the following aspects:
MP deviance from one’s cultural norms and concept of suitable functioning
MP distress experienced as a result of behaviour, emotions or experiences
MP dysfunction characterised by interference to normal life
MP danger to oneself or others.
While you will need to maintain a contemporary understanding of the discipline,
you are also required to understand how this came about and how it varies from
other perspectives. At the same time, you need to be aware of how your own
experiences, attitudes and beliefs will shape your interpretation of the literature
and how you perceive individuals who have psychological problems. Your
reports will require that you write objectively and as such you need to support
any claims with evidence and where possible avoid value judgements. These
factors may form a significant aspect of your critical evaluation of the literature.
154
Major (unipolar) depression
This chapter will provide an overview of the biological factors associated with
several forms of psychological dysfunction which are experienced in adulthood.
The topics covered are not exhaustive and suggested reading is provided at
the end of each section to guide the expansion of your literature search. For
issues concerning psychological dysfunction in children I recommend Comer
(2007) and Carr (2006). As such, by the end of this chapter you should be able
to understand how biological factors may predispose adults to mental illness or
contribute towards the manifestation or treatment of the disorders. You will also
be reminded of several other influences on human behaviour which may interact
with biological factors.
It is important to remember that 10–25 per cent of women and 5–12 per cent
of men will experience major (unipolar) depression during their lifetime (APA,
2000). It can either be reactive, in which it arises in response to major life events,
or endogenous if there is not a notable preceding trauma. You should already
be aware that major depression can involve either a single incidence or recurrent
episodes and as such can significantly reduce an individual’s quality of life, making
it a prominent area in psychology and psychiatry. As an undergraduate student you
should also understand that psychological disorders often co-occur (a phenomenon
called comorbidity) and it is the practitioner’s responsibility to interpret the
diagnostic criteria appropriately to differentiate these conditions. For example,
patients with major depression often also present with personality disorders,
155
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
substance dependence and eating disorders. The following sections will provide
you with an overview of the symptoms and aetiology of this condition. You should
also consider issues concerning the validity and reliability of the diagnostic manuals.
Key terms
Major (unipolar) depression: A psychological condition characterised by low mood,
negative thoughts, extreme negativism, reduced interest, lack of pleasure, changes
in appetite and sleep patterns and difficulty concentrating. In extreme cases sufferers
can also experience hallucinations and delusions.
Comorbidity: The presence of more than one condition at a given time.
Biological aetiology
There are numerous biological influences which may predispose individuals to
unipolar depression and you should be able to discuss these in detail for any
report or essay in the area. These include genetics, physiology, neurology and
156
Major (unipolar) depression
circadian rhythms. Table 9.1 presents a summary of some of the factors you will
need to understand.
Table 9.1 Biological factors associated with the development of unipolar depression
Factor Description
Physiology: hormones The endocrine system has been found to release abnormal levels of
hormones during major depressive episodes. This is especially the
case for the stress hormone cortisol and the chemical melatonin.
Circadian rhythms and Sleep patterns of depressed individuals tend to be shallow and
zeitgebers fragmented with decreased slow-wave delta sleep and increased
stage 1 sleep. Rapid eye-movement (REM) tends to occur earlier
and more frequently. There are also significant seasonal influences
(for example, seasonal affective disorder).
KEY STUDY
157
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Non-biological aetiology
Some of the non-biological factors which you will need to consider when revising
unipolar depression include traumatic life events, personality traits, worry,
negative thoughts and observational learning (Carlson, 2004; Carr & McNulty,
2006; Comer, 2007; Pinel, 2003). It is important you remember that individuals
do not exist in a vacuum consisting only of biological factors. Including these
factors in your reports can demonstrate that you are able to synthesise evidence
and write critically if you do it correctly. For example, you may like to compare
the role of biological and environmental factors in endogenous and reactive
depression. Also, how are cause and effect established? Do biological factors
predispose individuals to depression or are they in response to environmental
stressors? Just because an environmental influence cannot be identified does
this mean that one did not occur? It is these types of questions which allow you
to establish a well-rounded and critical account of the literature.
Treatment
There are several biomedical treatments used to reduce the symptoms of
unipolar depression. Table 9.2 summarises some of the biological treatments
that you will need to know, although this list is not exhaustive and you
should direct your further reading appropriately. You should also remember
that pharmacological substances are often addictive and are likely result in
withdrawal symptoms if terminated too quickly.
158
Major (unipolar) depression
Treatment Description
Sleep deprivation REM sleep deprivation over several weeks or short-term total sleep
deprivation often reduces the symptoms of unipolar depression. It is still
uncertain how this occurs although there has been speculation that a
substance is produced during sleep which has a depressogenic effect.
You should always remember that there are many possible sides to an academic
debate and avenues for critical thinking when you are constructing your
assignments and revision plan. For example, there have been several well-
publicised incidences of which you may already be aware where SSRIs such as
Seroxat have been linked to higher suicide rates in adolescents (Wooltorton,
2003). However, while you should understand the evidence which supports this
view you must also consider the alternative perspective. Indeed, the validity
of these reports is still to be determined and other prominent studies have
suggested that this is not the case (Simon, Savarino, Operskalski & Wang, 2006).
Indeed, there is a fine line between childhood and adolescence, and many
changes occur during this period which may explain varying degrees of suicidal
impulses. A question you must consider is whether the benefits of treatment
outweigh the potential harm?
159
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
9.1 Which natural chemicals have been associated with major depression?
9.2 What pharmacological treatments can treat major depression?
9.3 How might circadian rhythms influence mood?
9.4 Which non-biological factors are associated with major depression?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
Bipolar depression
It is vital that you can differentiate unipolar and bipolar depression regardless
of whether you are required to answer an essay question, exam question or
analyse a case study (Carr & McNulty, 2006; Comer, 2007). You will also need
to appreciate that bipolar disorder can also take several forms. Indeed bipolar
disorder is characterised by two subtypes. Bipolar I is characterised by manic
episodes, major depressive episodes and often mixed episodes in which
both states occur. In contrast, bipolar II is characterised by hypomania (rapid
160
Bipolar depression
cycle but short-lived mania) and major depressive episodes, but no manic or
mixed episodes. Patients with bipolar disorders often present with personality
disorders, self-harm, substance dependence and anxiety or panic disorders. The
following sections will remind you of the symptoms, diagnoses and treatments
for bipolar disorder.
Key terms
Bipolar disorder: A psychological condition characterised not only with periods of
depression but also by periods of mania.
Bipolar I: Bipolar disorder characterised by manic episodes, major depressive
episodes and often mixed episodes in which both states occur.
Bipolar II: Bipolar disorder characterised by hypomania (rapid cycle but short-lived
mania), major depressive episodes but no manic or mixed episodes.
Hypomania: Rapid cycle but shorted-lived episodes of mania.
Biological aetiology
The biological factors associated with depressive episodes in bipolar disorder
are discussed in Table 9.3. You will need to be able to both differentiate the
symptoms of unipolar and bipolar disorder and also identify their biological
commonalities. However, the same non-biological factors discussed under major
depression can also interact with bipolar condition.
161
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Table 9.3 Biological factors associated with the development of bipolar depression
Factor Description
Treatment
The biological treatments for the depressive episodes are similar to those used
for major depression, although other treatments which reduce the symptoms
of mania are described in Table 9.4. It is important that you remember that the
nature of bipolar disorder makes it far more likely that patients will forget doses
or choose not to take their medication because they miss the elevated state.
Pharmacological substances are also addictive and are likely result in withdrawal
symptoms if terminated too quickly.
Treatment Description
Antidepressants The antidepressants detailed in Table 9.2 can also be used to treat
the depressive symptoms of bipolar disorder.
162
Schizophrenia
Compare and contrast the diagnoses and treatment of unipolar and bipolar
depression.
Schizophrenia
163
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
with schizophrenia it is unlikely that they will ever make a full recovery due to
the cerebral factors associated with the condition, although the symptoms are
significantly less pronounced with the correct treatment and during remission.
However, the diagnostic criteria have been highly criticised for lacking validity
and reliability, meaning that several practitioners view schizophrenia as a
‘failed category’ in which many individuals can be placed without successful
differentiation (Carr & McNulty, 2006; Comer, 2007; Kuipers, Peters &
Bebbington, 2006) and you should be aware of this debate.
Key terms
Alogia: Impoverished speech observed in cases of schizophrenia.
Anhedonia: An inability to feel pleasure.
Avolition: A lack of desire or motivation.
Echolalia: Involuntary repetition of other peoples utterances.
t
164
Schizophrenia
Biological aetiology
Several biological factors appear to predispose individuals to schizophrenia or
manifest as a result of the condition (see Table 9.5). While earlier psychologies
believed that schizophrenia resulted from environmental factors such as cold
parenting, this view has been replaced with the diathesis–stress and biomedical
approaches. However, you need to remember you are studying psychology and
not psychiatry and so you should always consider all possible influences on the
individual from each of the main perspectives.
Factor Description
Heredity Schizophrenia has been associated with all chromosomes except 3, 12, 14,
16, 17, 19, 20, 21, and Y. While an unaffected monozygotic twin is slightly
more likely to subsequently develop schizophrenia than an unaffected
dizygotic twin if their sibling was diagnosed with the condition, this remains
below 50 per cent. These findings suggest that while there may be a
genetic predisposition, other factors also contribute towards the aetiology.
Physiology: The dopamine hypothesis states that schizophrenia may be related to
neurotransmitters imbalances in dopamine (Kuipers et al., 2006). Indeed, it is significant
that dopamine receptor blockers reduce the symptoms of schizophrenia
and the antipsychotic medication Clozapine functions by blocking the
dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Levels of dopamine are
increased through hypofrontality (discussed below).
Cerebral activity Patients with schizophrenia tend to display a phenomenon known as
hypofrontality in which there is significantly less activity in the prefrontal
cortex than in individuals without schizophrenia (Comer, 2007). There
may also be increased dopamine metabolism in the nucleus accumbens.
Cerebral structure CT and MRI scans have identified a general loss of brain grey matter
for patients with schizophrenia, which appears to occur suddenly in
early adulthood and results in enlarged ventricles due to this atrophy
(Wright et al., 2000). However, there also appears to be a loss of volume
in the hair-like branching networks of dendrites and axons in the brain
(Thompson et al., 2001). Patients with schizophrenia also tend to have
a smaller anterior hippocampus and larger lateral and third ventricles
(Carlson, 2004; Comer, 2007). Autopsies reveal that schizophrenic
patients have a greater number of dopamine receptors in the brain.
Other factors Parental age has been associated with schizophrenia as the fathers
of schizophrenic children tend to be older. There are also seasonal
influences with more sufferers being born in late winter or early spring.
Schizophrenia is also more likely if the mother of the sufferer was
RH-negative and the sufferer is RH-positive, possibly due to the mother’s
immune system attacking the foetus. Other factors include vitamin D
deficiency, prenatal malnutrition, population density, viral epidemics and
distance from the equator (Carlson, 2004).
165
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Treatment
Table 9.6 summarises some of the pharmacological treatments available for
use in schizophrenia which you should be familiar with. However, you should
remember that long-term use of these substances can result in involuntary
movements such as tick-like gestures, rapid movement of arms or legs,
grimacing, rapid blinking and tongue protrusion. This condition, known as
tardive dyskinesia, is mostly irreversible. These medications can also result in
the hypersensitivity of dopamine receptors because long-term blocking causes
irreparable neural damage. The substances are also addictive and are likely
result in withdrawal symptoms if terminated.
Key term
Tardive dyskinesia: Involuntary movements such as tick-like gestures, rapid
movement of arms or legs, grimacing, rapid blinking and tongue protrusion produced
through long-term use of anti-psychotic medications.
Treatment Description
KEY STUDY
166
Schizophrenia
that of patients with schizophrenia. This loss of grey matter began in the parietal lobes
and proceeded to the temporal lobes, somatosensory and motor cortex and dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex. It was also significant that the symptoms presented by the patients
with schizophrenia corresponded to both the area in which the atrophy was occurring
and the extent to which grey matter was lost. Hence, hallucinations occurred when
atrophy began in the temporal lobe and the magnitude of this impairment correlated to
the proportion of grey matter which was lost.
Non-biological considerations
You should always remember to demonstrate that you have considered a
range of influences and not just those from a biological perspective. You must
remember that an individual who suffers from schizophrenia does not exist in a
biological vacuum and you can demonstrate critical thinking by expanding your
knowledge to include alternative factors. For example:
MP Cultural norms concerning acceptable and unacceptable behaviour may
determine whether the individual is categorised as psychologically abnormal,
eccentric or normal.
MP The support network available to an individual with schizophrenia will
determine when treatment is sought and also influence their rate of recovery
and chances of relapse. Also remember that lack of understanding and media
coverage which portrays individuals with schizophrenia as dangerous will also
influence how people perceive and treat them. This can exacerbate their
symptoms and make them socially isolated.
MP Socioeconomic status significantly influences the likelihood that they will be
diagnosed and treated appropriately. This is especially relevant if health care
is not freely available or the individual is homeless and not amongst people
who will notice the changes in their behaviour.
MP The patient’s cognitive strategies and coping mechanisms may influence how
they compensate for their symptoms and how well they recover.
167
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Anxiety disorders are multifaceted and you are likely to study several types
during your undergraduate studies (Carr & McNulty, 2006; Comer, 2007). These
are likely to include acute stress disorder, agoraphobia (with or without panic
disorder, generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), obsessive compulsive disorder
(OCD), panic disorder (with or without agoraphobia), phobias and post-traumatic
stress disorder (PTSD). However, it is important to remember that stress and
anxiety are normal emotional states which are related to several physiological
factors and were discussed in earlier chapters. The main debates surrounding
anxiety disorders concern determinism (for example, biologically driven
reflexes) versus free will (for example, intentionally changing thought patterns),
the validity and reliability of the diagnostic manuals to differentiate between
conditions and the value of combining treatments to maximise the benefits to
the patient. You should be able to discuss these debates at length.
168
Anxiety and stress disorders
Disorder Description
Acute stress disorder Arises after the experience of a highly traumatic event which
threatened injury or death and created feelings of fear and
helplessness. Arises within four weeks of the trauma (Brewin,
Andrews, Rose & Kirk, 1999; Bryant & Harvey, 2000). Characterised
by anxiety, irritability, poor concentration, insomnia, restlessness
and dissociative states including detachment, derealisation
or depersonalisation. Patients also re-experience the event in
flashbacks, avoid associated places or people.
Panic disorder A condition which often arises without a discernible cause but may
follow periods of high anxiety or arise due to misinterpretation
of bodily responses (Taylor & Asmundson, 2006). Characterised
by sudden and acute attacks of fear or anxiety resulting in
panic attacks, stress, heart palpitations, rapid breathing or
hyperventilating, blurred vision, dizziness and flights of thought.
Generalised anxiety A general state of anxiety which is applied across life in general
disorder and may have originated due to the exacerbation of other
anxieties or traumatic events (Wells & Carter, 2006). Characterised
by anxiety, worry, heart palpitations, dizziness and recurrent stress
response across a range of situations.
Post-traumatic stress Anxiety disorder arising after extreme trauma which caused feelings
disorder of intense fear and helplessness which can arise at any point
after the trauma including years later (Creamer & Carty, 2006).
Characterised by flashbacks, nightmares, obsessive thoughts,
avoidance of triggers, heightened anxiety and startle response.
169
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
CrITICAl FoCUS
Biological factors
From your reading of the earlier chapters in this text, you should appreciate
that stress and anxiety are intrinsically linked to the functions of the central,
peripheral and endocrine systems. Table 9.8 summarises some of the biological
factors which may contribute towards the development of anxiety and stress
disorders with which you should be familiar.
Table 9.8 Biological factors associated with anxiety and stress disorders
Factor Description
170
Anxiety and stress disorders
Evolutionary factors According to evolutionary theory, stress and anxiety are normal
responses which facilitated the survival of our ancestors and were
therefore transmitted genetically to following generations. Anxiety
and stress disorders would be exaggerations of these normal
behaviours which no longer serve evolutionary functions due to
changing environments (Comer, 2007).
Non-biological factors
Non-biomedical factors which you should research also include irrational or
obsessive thoughts, negative experiences, occupational status (if the occupation
is particularly stressful), sociocultural factors, personality types such as trait
anxiety, observational learning, operant conditioning and classical conditioning.
These topics are covered in other texts in this series and in the suggested
further reading.
171
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Treatment
Table 9.9 summarises some of the biomedical treatments available to reduce
anxiety. However, you should remember that all of these treatments, with the
exception of biofeedback, only offer temporary relief from the symptoms and
will not eradicate the individual’s beliefs about and attitude towards the source
of their anxiety. For this reason you should also consider cognitive behavioural
approaches which could be combined with biomedical treatments to maximise
the benefits of the treatment and reduce the chance of using medication
long-term. For example, medication can be used to reduce the physiological
responses but cognitive-behavioural therapy would challenge and replace
unhealthy beliefs (such as obsessions) and behaviours (such as avoidance or
compulsions) resulting in lasting effects.
Treatment Description
172
Chapter summary – pulling it all together
Panic disorder Taylor, S., & Asmundson, J. G. (2006). Panic disorder and agoraphobia. In A.
Carr and M. McNulty (Eds.), The handbook of adult clinical psychology: An
evidence-based approach (pp. 458–486). Hove: Routledge.
GAD Wells, A., & Carter, K. (2006). Generalized anxiety disorder. In A. Carr and
M. McNulty (Eds.), The handbook of adult clinical psychology: An evidence-
based approach (pp. 423–457). Hove: Routledge.
9.13 What are the differences between acute and post-traumatic stress
disorders?
9.14 How would OCD be treated from a biomedical perspective?
9.15 When would generalised anxiety disorder prevent normal functioning?
9.16 Which methodological approaches can be used to investigate the
aetiology of psychological conditions?
Answers to these questions can be found on the companion website at:
www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
PCan you tick all the points from the revision checklist at the beginning of
this chapter?
PAttempt the sample question from the beginning of this chapter using the
answer guidelines below.
PGo to the companion website at www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
to access more revision support online, including interactive quizzes,
flashcards, You be the marker exercises as well as answer guidance for the
Test your knowledge and Sample questions from this chapter.
Answer guidelines
Evaluate the extent to which the distinctions between normal and abnormal
behaviour are universally accepted rather than culturally specific.
173
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Notes
174
Notes
Notes
175
9 • Biological basis of psychological abnormality
Notes
176
And finally, before the exam . . .
177
And finally, before the exam . . .
lead you to other sources that may be important to the field. One thing that an
examiner hates to see is the same old sources being cited all of the time: be
innovative and, as well as reading the seminal works, find the more obscure and
interesting sources as well – just make sure they’re relevant to your answer!
MM Don’t avoid revision. This is the best tip ever. There is something on the TV,
the pub is having a two-for-one offer, the fridge needs cleaning, your budgie
looks lonely . . . You have all of these activities to do and they need doing
now! Really . . . ? Do some revision!
MM Don’t spend too long at each revision session. Working all day and night
is not the answer to revision. You do need to take breaks, so schedule your
revision so you are not working from dawn until dusk. A break gives time for
the information you have been revising to consolidate.
MM Don’t worry. Worrying will cause you to lose sleep, lose concentration and
lose revision time by leaving it late and then later. When the exam comes,
you will have no revision completed and will be tired and confused.
MM Don’t cram. This is the worst revision technique in the universe! You will not
remember the majority of the information that you try to stuff into your skull,
so why bother?
MM Don’t read over old notes with no plan. Your brain will take nothing in. If you
wrote your lecture notes in September and the exam is in May is there any
point in trying to decipher your scrawly handwriting now?
MM Don’t write model answers and learn by rote. When it comes to the exam
you will simply regurgitate the model answer irrespective of the question –
not a brilliant way to impress the examiner!
178
And finally, before the exam . . .
aren’t expected to know or remember everything in the course, but you should
be able to show your understanding of what you have studied. Remember as
well that examiners are interested in what you know, not what you don’t know.
They try to write exam questions that give you a good chance of passing – not
ones to catch you out or trick you in any way. You may want to consider some of
these top exam tips.
MM Start your revision in plenty of time.
MM Make a revision timetable and stick to it.
MM Practise jotting down answers and making essay plans.
MM Practise writing against the clock using past exam papers.
MM Check that you have really answered the question and have not strayed off
the point.
MM Review a recent past paper and check the marking structure.
MM Carefully select the topics you are going to revise.
MM Use your lecture/study notes and refine them further, if possible, into lists or
diagrams and transfer them on to index cards/Post-it notes. Mind maps are a
good way of making links between topics and ideas.
MM Practise your handwriting – make sure it’s neat and legible.
One to two days before the exam
MM Recheck times, dates and venue.
MM Have a good meal, two to three hours before the start time.
MM Arrive in good time.
MM Spend a few minutes calming and focusing.
In the exam room
MM Keep calm.
MM Take a few minutes to read each question carefully. Don’t jump to conclusions
– think calmly about what each question means and the area it is focused on.
MM Start with the question you feel most confident about. This helps your morale.
MM By the same token, don’t expend all your efforts on that one question – if you
are expected to answer three questions then don’t just answer two.
MM Keep to time and spread your effort evenly on all opportunities to score
marks.
179
And finally, before the exam . . .
MM Once you have chosen a question, jot down any salient facts or key points.
Then take five minutes to plan your answer – a spider diagram or a few notes
may be enough to focus your ideas. Try to think in terms of ‘why and how’ not
just ‘facts’.
MM You might find it useful to create a visual plan or map before writing your
answer to help you remember to cover everything you need to address.
MM Keep reminding yourself of the question and try not to wander off the point.
MM Remember that quality of argument is more important than quantity of facts.
MM Take 30–60-second breaks whenever you find your focus slipping (typically
every 20 minutes).
MM Make sure you reference properly – according to your university requirements.
MM Watch your spelling and grammar – you could lose marks if you make too
many errors.
Test your knowledge by using the material presented in this text or on the
website: www.pearsoned.co.uk/psychologyexpress
180
Glossary
181
Glossary
aromatisation The process by which sex steroids derived from cholesterol are
converted into other sex steroids.
atrophy The decay or wasting of a structure, organ or system.
attention The allocation of cognitive resources to stimuli.
autobiographical memory Memory for events, experiences and personal
information from one’s own life.
autonomic nervous system A division of the peripheral nervous system which is
responsible for governing responses which are largely beyond conscious control.
autotopagnosia The inability to name and identify body parts.
avolition A lack of desire or motivation.
behavioural genetics An approach in psychology which attempts to identify what
proportion of the variance in a trait or behaviour can be attributed to genetics and to
the environment.
behaviourism A school of thought concerned with purely observable and
measureable human and animal behaviour.
beta activity A pattern of activity observed when actively engaging in mental
activity, characterised by 13–30 Hz (cycles per second). Activity is desynchronised in
that areas of the brain vary in their levels and pattern of activity.
bipolar disorder A psychological condition characterised not only with periods of
depression but also by periods of mania.
bipolar I Bipolar disorder characterised by manic episodes, major depressive
episodes and often mixed episodes in which both states occur.
bipolar II Bipolar disorder characterised by hypomania (rapid cycle but short-lived
mania), major depressive episodes but no manic or mixed episodes.
bloodletting An ancient technique of releasing blood from the body in an attempt
to restore balance.
Broca’s aphasia A deficit in language production caused by damage to Broca’s area
in the prefrontal cortex. Characterised by anomia, agrammatism and difficulties in
articulation. Also known as expressive aphasia.
bruxism A condition in which individuals grind their teeth and clench their jaw while
asleep, potentially resulting in a sore jaw, damaged teeth and headaches.
cataplexy The sudden loss of muscle tone which can be anything from slight
paralysis of a body part to complete collapse.
central nervous system A complex system which governs all top-down processes
and consists of the brain and the spinal cord.
circadian clock A hypothetical biological mechanism which is theorised to control
sleep–waking patterns and the other biological prerequisites for sleep (such
as temperature change, the release of growth hormones and the secretion of
neurotransmitters).
circadian rhythm A behavioural or physiological process which changes daily
according to a set pattern, such as the sleep–wake cycle.
clinical trial The procedure by which medications are tested and legalised.
182
Glossary
183
Glossary
endocrine system The network of glands and organs which release and regulate
hormones.
enteric nervous system A division of the autonomic nervous system which is
responsible for maintaining the gastrointestinal system.
enuresis Persistent bedwetting occurring after a child has been potty trained.
episodic memory Memory for specific events including times, places, emotions
experienced during the event.
ethological research A research technique in which animals are studied in their
natural environment with little intervention by the researcher.
eugenics A school of thought in which intelligent people are encouraged to
reproduce in an attempt to improve the species.
evolutionary theories Theories that behaviour and experience have developed
through centuries of genetic mutation, evolution and survival of the fittest.
exocrine glands Glands which secrete substances into ducts, from which they pass
from cell to cell through diffusion.
explicit memory Knowledge which is easily vocalised, including knowledge about
oneself and the world.
exteroceptive sensory systems The structures and processes responsible for the
senses of touch, smell, taste, hearing and vision.
family study A research technique in which the prevalence of a trait or type within a
family is assessed in regards to heredity.
functionalism An approach which is concerned with identifying the functions which
behaviour and experience serve.
generalised anxiety disorder A general state of anxiety which is applied across life
in general and may have originated due to the exacerbation of other anxieties or
traumatic events. Characterised by anxiety, worry, heart palpitations, dizziness and
recurrent stress response across a range of situations.
genetic engineering The manipulation or cloning of the genome within laboratory
settings.
heredity The proportion of variance in a given trait or type which can be accounted
for by genetics and the environment.
homeostasis The naturally balanced state of the body. This is the ideal state
and the parasympathetic nervous system strives to restore this equilibrium when
physiology is imbalanced.
hormones Endogenous substances produced by the glands of the body.
humorism An ancient school of thought concerned with the balance of the body’s
naturally produced substances: bile, blood and phlegm.
hypomania Rapid-cycle but shorted-lived episodes of mania. Common in bipolar II.
iconic memory Short-term memory for information in the visual field.
idealism A perspective which states that the only reality is that created by the mind.
implicit memory Memory which cannot be easily articulated, such as in the case of
procedural knowledge.
184
Glossary
185
Glossary
monist/monism A perspective which states that either the mind or the body exists
independently. There is only one reality and that may be through the mind (idealism)
or the body (materialism).
monozygotic twins Identical twins who share the same DNA.
narcolepsy A condition in which individuals are unable to control their sleep
patterns, suffer from excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS), fall asleep at inappropriate
times or in inappropriate places and enter REM sleep after only ten minutes.
nature–nurture A theoretical debate surrounding the topic of whether an individual’s
behaviour and experience are determined and inevitable due to biological factors or
undetermined and changeable due to environmental factors.
neuroimaging A procedure in which neurological imaging technology is used to
visualise and record the activity of the brain.
neuropsychology An approach in psychology which attempts to identify both normal
and impaired human function, usually through studying the effects of brain damage.
neuroscience An approach in psychology which attempts to identify the neural
correlates of cognition using a combination of physiological, experimental and
computational measures.
neurotransmitter A naturally occurring chemical produced in the body at the
terminal buttons of neurons which facilitates the transmission of action potentials
across synaptic gaps. The activation threshold and compatibility of the post-synaptic
receptor cells will determine their efficiency. Neurotransmitters can have excitatory or
inhibitory effects.
nocturia Frequency waking due to the need to urinate, which can result in sleep
deprivation.
non-REM sleep Constituting stages 1–4 of normal sleep which is not characterised
by rapid eye movements.
normal wakefulness Full consciousness and engagement with the environment.
obsessive compulsive disorder An anxiety disorder characterised by excessive
worry, persistent obsessive thoughts, irrationality and overriding compulsions to
perform actions which reduce the patient’s anxiety. Repetitive actions are intended to
neutralise the source of the anxiety. May prevent normal daily functioning.
orthographic dysgraphia A writing disorder in which individuals are unable to spell
irregularly spelled words while still being able to spell regularly spelled words.
panic disorder A condition which often arises without a discernible cause but may
follow periods of high anxiety or arise due to misinterpretation of bodily responses.
Characterised by sudden and acute attacks of fear or anxiety resulting in panic
attacks, stress, heart palpitations, rapid breathing or hyperventilating, blurred vision,
dizziness and flights of thought.
parasomnias Parasomnias include sleep walking, night terrors and bruxism.
pavor nocturnus Night terrors. A condition which arises during slow-wave sleep and
is characterised by extreme fear, gasping and often screaming.
parasympathetic nervous system A division of the autonomic nervous system which
promotes the conservation of resources.
186
Glossary
187
Glossary
188
Glossary
189
References
190
References
Bernal, B., & Ardila, A. (2009). The role of the arcuate fasciculus in conduction aphasia. Brain,
132(9), 2309–2316. Available: http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/132/9/2309.full
Bi, Y., Wei, T., Wu, C., Han, Z., Jiang, T., & Caramazza, A. (2009). The role of the left anterior
temporal lobe in language processing revisited: Evidence from an individual with ATL
resection. Cortex, 47(5), 575–587.
Blonder, L. X., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). The role of the right hemisphere in
emotional communications. Brain, 114(3), 1115–1127.
Bouwknecht, J. A., Hijzen, T. H., Gugten, J. van der, Maes, R. A. A., Hen, R., & Olivier, B. (2001).
Absence of 5-HT1B receptors is associated with impaired impulse control in male 5-HT1B
knockout mice. Biological Psychiatry, 49(7), 557–568.
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., Rose, S., & Kirk, M. (1999). Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic
stress disorder in victims of violent crime. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(3), 360–366.
Brodal, P. (2010). The central nervous system: Structure and function (4th ed.). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Broerse, P. J. (2001). Perception and attention. In N. W. Bond and K. M. McConkey (Eds.),
Psychological science: An introduction (pp. 42–96). Sydney: McGraw-Hill.
Bryant, R. A., & Harvey, A. G. (2000). Acute stress disorder: A handbook of theory, assessment,
and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Burton, R. (1989/1994). The anatomy of melancholy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI
studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 1–47.
Campbell, R., & Conway, M. (Eds.) (1995). Broken memories: Case studies in memory
impairment. Oxford: Blackwell.
Carlson, N. R. (2004). Physiology of behavior (8th ed.). New York: Pearson Education Inc.
Carr, A. (2006). The handbook of child and adolescent clinical psychology: A contextual
approach. Hove: Routledge.
Carr, A., & McNulty, M. (Eds.) (2006). The handbook of adult clinical psychology: An evidence-
based approach. Hove: Routledge.
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and
implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183–204. Available:
www.socialemotiveneuroscience.org/pubs/carver_hj2009psy_bull.pdf
Castrén, E. (2005). Is mood chemistry? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 241–246.
Chan, S., & Kirby, M. D. (2000). Thyroid hormone and central nervous system development.
Journal of Endocrinology, 165(1), 1–8. Available: http://joe.endocrinology-journals.org/
content/165/1/1.full.pdf
Chan, T., Kyere, K., Davis, B. R., Shemyakin, A., Kabitzke, P. A., Shair, H. N., Barr, G. A., &
Wiedenmayer, C. P. (2011). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in innate fear regulation
in infants, juveniles and adolescents. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(13), 4991–4999.
Chaudhari, N., & Roper, S. D. (2010). The cell biology of taste. Journal of Current Biology,
190(3), 285–296. Available: http://jcb.rupress.org/content/190/3/285.full
Cho, K., Ennaceur, A., Cole, J. C., & Suh, C. K. (2000). Chronic jet lag produces cognitive deficits.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1–5. Available: www.jneurosci.org/content/20/6/RC66.short
Cohen, G., Johnstone, R. A., & Plunkett, K. (Eds.) (2002). Exploring cognition: Damaged brains
and neural networks – Readings in cognitive neuropsychology and connectionist modelling.
Hove: Psychology Press
Coltheart, M,. (2001). Assumptions and methods in cognitive neuropsychology. Hove:
Psychology Press.
Coltheart, M., Ingles, L., Cupples, L., Michie, P., Bates, A., & Budd, B. (1998). A semantic
subsystem specific to the storage of information about visual attributes of animate and non-
animate objects. Neurocase, 4, 353–370.
Comer, R. J. (2007). Abnormal psychology (6th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.
Corkin, S., Sullivan, E. V., Twitchell, T. E., & Grove, E. (1981). The amnesic patient H.M.: Clinical
observations and a test performance 28 years after operation. Society of Neuroscience
Abstracts, 7, 235.
191
References
Correa, A., Sanabria, D., Spence, C., Tudela, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Selective temporal
attention enhances the temporal resolution of visual perception: Evidence from a temporal
order judgement task. Brain Research, 1070(1), 202–205.
Coull, J. T. (1998). Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiology,
functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Progress in Neurobiology, 55(4),
343–361. Available: http://dionysus.psych.wisc.edu/Lit/Articles/CoullJ1998a.pdf
Counsell, C. E., & Ruddell, W. S. (1994). Coeliac disease and autoimmune thyroid disease. Gut,
35(6), 844–846.
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology:
Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences.
Neuropsychology, 40(8), 1196–1208. Available: http://abdn.ac.uk/~psy086/dept/pdfs/
Neuropsychologia_2002_conflims_singlecase.pdf
Creamer, M., & Carty, J. (2006). Post-traumatic stress disorder. In A. Carr and M. McNulty (Eds.),
The handbook of adult clinical psychology: An evidence-based approach (pp. 523–557).
Hove: Routledge.
Crick, F., & Mitchison, G. (1983). The functions of dream sleep. Nature, 304(5922), 111–114.
Critchley, H. D. (2009). Psychophysiology of neural, cognitive and affective integration: fMRI
and autonomic indicants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(2), 88–94. Available:
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2722714/
Damann, N., Voets, T., & Nilius, B. (2008). TRPs in our senses. Current Biology, 18(18), 880–889.
Available: https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/221461/2/TRPs
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). A neural basis
for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 499–505.
Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Grabowski, T., Adolphs, R., & Damasio, A. (2004). Neural systems
behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition, 92(1–2), 179–229.
Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of the species by natural selection, or the preservation of favoured
races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray.
Davidson, R. J. (2003). Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: Towards a synthesis.
Psychophysiology, 40, 655–665. Available: http://dionysus.psych.wisc.edu/lit/articles/
DavidsonR2003b.pdf
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Putnam, K. (2002). Depression: Perspectives from
affective neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 545–574.
Dewsbury, D. A. (1990). Contemporary issues in comparative psychology. Sunderland, MA:
Sinauer Associates Inc.
D’Hondt, F., Lassonde, M., Collignon, O., Dubarry, A., Robert, M., Rigoulot, S., Honoré, J.,
Lepore, F., & Sequeira, H. (2010). Early brain–body impact of emotional arousal. Human
Neuroscience, 4, 1–10. Available: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2859881/
Drevets, W. C. (2001). Neuroimaging and neuropathological studies for the cognitive–emotional
features of mood disorders. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(2), 240–249.
Drevets, W. C., Videen, T. O., Price, J. L., Preskorn, S. H., Carmichael, S. T., & Raichle, M. E.
(1992). A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. The Journal of Neuroscience,
12(9), 3628–3641.
Ehlert, U., Gaab, J., & Heinrichs, M. (2001). Psychoneuroendocrinological contributes to the
etiology of depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and stress related bodily disorders:
The role of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis. Biological Psychology, 57, 141–152.
Available: www.socialbehavior.uzh.ch/static/home/heinrichs/downloads/
BiolPsychol-UE01.pdf
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniels, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286–290. Available: www2.furman.edu/
academics/psychology/facultyandstaff/einstein/documents/2005a.pdf
Escobar, G. M., Obregón, M. J., & Rey, F. E. (2004). Maternal thyroid hormones early in
pregnancy and fetal brain development. Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 18(2),
225–248.
192
References
Espa, F., Ondze, B., Billiard, M., & Besset, A. (2000). Sleep architecture, slow-wave activity,
and sleep spindles in adolescent patients with sleepwalking and sleep terrors. Clinical
Neurophysiology, 111(5), 929–939.
Fitzgerald, M. (2010). The lost domain of pain. Brain, 133(6), 1850–1854.
Freud, S. (1953). The interpretation of dreams (3rd ed.) (A. A. Brill Trans.). Plain Label Books.
(Original work published 1931.) Available: http://koyakin.yolasite.com/resources/
Interpretation%20Of%20Dreams%20326%20Pages.pdf
Furness, J. B. (2006). The enteric nervous system. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Gallo, L. C., & Eastman, C. I. (1993). Circadian rhythms during gradually delaying and advancing
sleep and light schedules. Physiology and Behavior, 53(1), 119–126.
Galton, F. (1865). Hereditary talent and character. Macmillan’s Magazine, 12, 157–166. Available:
http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Galton/talent.htm.
García-Aragón, J., Lobie, P. E., Muscat, G. O., Gobius, K. S., Norstedt, G., & Waters, M. J.
(1992). Prenatal expression of the growth hormone (GH) receptor/binding protein in the rat:
A role for GH in embryonic and fetal development. Development, 114, 869–876.
Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T., & Van Cantfort, T. E. (1989). Teaching sign language to
chimpanzees. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Gaskell, M. G. (Ed.) (2007). The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Gazzaniga, M. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind (3rd edn.). New York: W.
W. Norton, & Co.
Geschwind, N. (1970). The organization of language and the brain. Science, 170(3961), 940–944.
Gibson, A. D., & Garbers, D. L. (2000). Guanylyl cyclases as a family of putative odorant
receptors. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 417–439.
Glaser, R., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (Eds.) (1994). Handbook of human stress and Immunity. San
Diego: Academic Press.
Goldstein, B. E. (2009). Sensation and perception (8th edn.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Goodale, M., & Milner, D. (2006). One brain – two visual systems. The Psychologist, 19(11),
660–663.
Gottesmann, C. (2008). Noradrenaline involvement in basic and higher integrated REM sleep
processes. Progress in Neurobiology, 85(3), 237–272.
Grosbras, M., & Paus, T. (2002). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye
field: effects on visual perception and attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7),
1109–1120.
Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000).
Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
12(5), 711–720.
Gulevich, G., Dement, W. C., & Johnson, L. (1966). Psychiatric and EEG observations on a case
of prolonged (264 hours) wakefulness. Archives in General Psychiatry, 15, 29–35.
Guzick, D. S. (2004). Polycystic ovary syndrome. Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 103(1), 181–193.
Available: www.utilis.net/Morning%20Topics/Gynecology/PCOS.PDF
Haass, C. (1999). Biology of Alzheimer’s disease. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical
Neuroscience, 249(6), 265.
Harris, M., & Grunstein, R. R. (2009). Treatments for somnambulism in adults: Assessing the
evidence. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13(4), 295–297.
Handwerger, S., & Freemark, M. (2000). The roles of placental growth hormone and placental
lactogen in the regulation of human fetal growth and development. Journal of Pediatric
Endocrinology and Metabolism, 13, 343–356.
Harley, R. (2007). The psychology of language: From data to theory (3rd edn.). Hove: Psychology
Press.
Harrington, R. C., Fudge, H., Rutter, M. L., Bredenkamp, D., Groothues, C., & Pridham, J. (1993).
Child and adult depression: A test of continuities with data from a family study. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 162, 627–633.
193
References
Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than
consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 523–532. Available: http://psychology.
stanford.edu/~jlm/pdfs/HenkeNRNpaper.pdf
Hergenhahn, B. R. (2009). An introduction to the history of psychology (6th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Herman, L. M., Kuczaj, S. A., & Holder, M. D. (1993). Responses to anomalous gestural
sequences by a language-trained dolphin: Evidence for processing of semantic relations and
syntactic information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(2), 184–194.
Hertenstein, M. J., Holmes, R., McCullough, M., & Keltner, D. (2009). The communication of
emotion via touch. Emotion, 9(4), 566–573.
Hindmarch, I. (2001). Expanding the horizons of depression: Beyond the monoamine hypothesis.
Human Psychopharmacology, 16(3), 203–218.
Hirschfeld, R. M. A. (2000). History and evolution of the monoamine hypothesis of depression.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61(6), 4–6.
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origins of speech. Scientific American, 203, 89–96.
Holdstock, J. S., Parslow, D. M., Morris, R. G., Fleminger, S., Abrahams, S., Denby, C., Montaldi,
D., & Mayes, A. R. (2008). Two case studies illustrating how relatively selective hippocampal
lesions in humans can have quite different effects on memory. Hippocampus, 18, 679–691.
Available: www.julietholdstock.com/pdfs/Holdstock%20et%20al%202008.pdf
Horne, J. A. A. (1978). A review of the biological effects of total sleep deprivation in man.
Biological Psychology, 7, 55–102.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1977). Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 198, 1–59.
Huber, R., & Kravitz, E. A. (2010). Aggression: Towards an integration of gene, brain and
behaviour. In T. Székely, A. J. Moore, & J. Komdeur (Eds.), Social behaviour: Genes, ecology
and evolution (pp.165–180). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Huberle, E., Driver, J., & Karnath, H. (2010). Retinal versus physical stimulus size as determinants
of visual perception in simultanagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1677–1682.
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Dodge, K. A., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., & Tully, L. A. (2005).
Nature x nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities interact with physical maltreatment to promote
conduct problems. Developmental Psychopathology, 17(1), 67–84. Available: http://www.
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2768347/
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (vol. 1). Available: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/
James/Principles/index.htm
Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: The cortical
mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neuroscience, 18(7), 314–320
Kanbayashi, T., Nakamura, M., Shimizu, T., & Nishino, S. (2010). Symptomatic narcolepsy or
hypersomnia, with or without hypocretin (Orexin) deficiency. Narcolepsy, 2, 135–165.
Keyers, C., Kaas, J. H., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 11(6), 417–428.
Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond extinction: Erasing human fear responses and
preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 12(3), 256–258. Available: www.psypoli.
nl/webdata/files/kindt%20beyond%20extinsion.pdf
Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999).
Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the
hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 154–162. Available:
http://dionysus.psych.wisc.edu/lit/Articles/KirschbaumC1999a.pdf.
Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Cortical regions involved in processing object shape. Journal
of Neuroscience, 20(9) 3310–3318.
Kuipers, E., Peters, E., & Bebbington, P. (2006). Schizophrenia. In A. Carr and M. McNulty (Eds.),
The handbook of adult clinical psychology: An evidence-based approach (pp. 843–896).
Hove: Routledge.
Lam, D., & Jones, S. (2006). Bipolar disorder. In A. Carr & M. McNulty (Eds), The handbook of
adult clinical psychology: An evidence-based approach (pp. 346–382). Hove: Routledge.
194
References
Lashley, K. S. (1930). Basic neural mechanisms in behaviour. Psychological Review, 37(1), 1–24.
Available: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Lashley/neural.htm.
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235.
Legro, R. S., Kunselman, A. R., Dodson, W. C., & Dunaif, A. (1999). Prevalence and predictors of
risk for type 2 diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance in polycystic ovary syndrome:
a prospective, controlled study in 254 affected women. The Journal of Endocrinology and
Metabolism, 84(1), 165–169.
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial expression generates
emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384.
Lewis, M. P. (Ed.) (2009). Ethnologue: Languages of the world (16th ed.). Dallas, TX.: SIL
International. Online version: www.ethnologue.com
Lewis, M. (2011). Inside and outside: The relation between emotional states and expressions.
Emotion Review, 3(2), 189–196. Available: www.depauw.edu/learn/lab/publications/
documents/touch/2009_Touch_The_communication_of_emotion_via_touch.pdf
Liddle, P. F., Laurens, K. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Ngan, E. T. (2006). Abnormal function of the brain
system supporting motivated attention in medicated patients with schizophrenia: An fMRI
study. Psychological Medicine, 38(8), 1097–1108.
Loui, P., Guenther, F. H., Mathys, C., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Action–perception mismatch in tone-
deafness. Current Biology, 18(8), 331–332.
Low Choy, N., Brauer, S. G., & Nitz, J. C. (2007). Age-related changes in strength and
somatosensation during midlife. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1114, 180–193.
Lumpkin, E. A., & Caterina, M. J. (2007). Mechanisms of the sensory transduction in the skin.
Nature, 445(7130), 858–865.
Madsen, P. L., Holm, S., Vorstrup, S., Friberg, L., Lassen, N. A., & Wildschiodz, G. (1991). Human
regional cerebral blood flow during rapid-eye-movement sleep. Journal of Cerebral Blood
Flow and Metabolism, 11, 502–507.
Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Frith, C. D., & O’Keefe, L. (2000).
Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science, 280, 291–924.
Maguire, E. A., Frith, C. D., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., & O’Keefe, L. (1998). Knowing where
things are: Parahippocampal involvement in encoding objects in virtual large-scale space.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 61–76.
Mani, S. K., Allen, J. M. C., Clark, J. H., Blaustein, J. D., & O’Malley, B. W. (1994). Convergent
pathways for steroid hormone- and neurotransmitter-induced rat sexual behavior. Science,
265(5176), 1246–1249. Available: www-unix.oit.umass.edu/~blaustei/man1994s.pdf
Manolagas, S. C. (2000). Birth and death of bone cells: Basic regulatory mechanism and
implications for the pathogenesis and treatment of osteoporosis. Endocrine Reviews, 21(2),
115–137.
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of
congenitally and noncongenitally blind individuals. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 96(1), 1–10.
Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion,
23(2), 209–237. Available: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2756702/
McDolald, J. W., & Sadowsky, C. (2002). Spinal-cord injury. Lancet, 359(9304), 417–425.
Available: www.theneuroinstitute.com/research/medref/052%20LIB%20PAPER.pdf
McWilliams, S., & O’Callaghan, E. (2006). Biomedical approaches and the use of drugs to treat
adult mental health problems. In A. Carr and M. McNulty (Eds.), The handbook of adult
clinical psychology: An evidence-based approach (pp. 220–252). Hove: Routledge.
Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: A new theory. Science, 150(699), 971–979.
Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (2008). The challenges of pain (updated 2nd ed). London: Penguin
Books. (Original work published 1982.)
Meyer, J. S., & Quenzer, L. F. (2004). Psychopharmacology: Drugs, the brain and behavior.
Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates Inc.
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1993).Visual pathways to perception and action. In T. P. Hicks,
S. Molotchnikoff, & T. Ono (Eds.), Progress in brain research, Vol. 95. The visually responsive
neuron: From basic neurophysiology to behavior (pp. 317–337). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
195
References
Moore, R. Y. (1997). Circadian rhythms: Basic neurobiology and clinical applications. Annual
Review of Medicine, 48, 253–266.
Mumby, D., Pinel, P. J., & Wood, E. R. (1989). Non-recurring items delayed nonmatching-to-
sample in rats: A new paradigm for testing nonspatial working memory. Psychobiology, 18,
321–362.
Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the
hippocampal complex. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 7, 217–227.
Naya, Y., Yoshida, M., & Miyashita, Y. (2001). Backward spreading of memory-retrieval signals in
the primate temporal cortex. Science, 291(5504), 661–664.
N’Diaye, K., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Self-relevance processing in the human
amygdala: Gaze direction, facial expression, and emotional intensity. Emotion, 9(6), 798–806.
Available: http://kndiaye.free.fr/lab/publications/ndiaye09.pdf
Nguyen, B. H., Pérusse, D., Paquet, J., Petit, D., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R. E., Montplaisir, J.
(2008). Sleep terrors in children: A prospective study of twins. Paediatrics, 122(6), 1164–1167.
Nicholson, P. J., & D’Auria, D. A. (1999). Shift work, health, the working time regulations and
health assessments. Occupational Medicine, 49(3), 127–137.
Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions (2nd ed.). Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
Ongür, D., Drevets, W. D., & Price, J. L. (1998). Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cortex
in mood disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 95(22), 1329–95.
Orwoll, E. S., & Klein, R. F. (1995). Osteoporosis in men. Endocrine Reviews, 16(1), 87–116.
Owens, M. J., & Nemeroff, C. B. (1994). Role of serotonin in the parthophysiology of depression:
focus on the serotonin transporter. Clinical Chemistry, 40(2), 288–295.
Pace-Schott, E., Solms, M., Blagrove, M., & Harnad, S. (Eds.) (2003). Sleep and dreaming:
Scientific advances and reconsiderations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face
perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 75–92. Available: www.abdn.
ac.uk/~psy423/dept/HomePage/Level_3_Social_Psych_files/Palermo%26Rhodes.pdf
Patterson, F. G. (1978). The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another pongid. Brain
and Language, 5(1), 72–97.
Patterson, F. G. (1981). Ape language. Science, 211(4477), 86–88.
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Pepperberg, I. M. (2006a). Ordinality and inferential abilities of a Grey Parrot (Psittacus
erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 205–216.
Pepperberg, I. M. (2006b). Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) numerical abilities: Addition and
further experiments on a zero-like concept. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 1–11.
Petit, D., Touchette, E., Tremblay R. E., Boivin, M., & Montplaisir, J. (2007). Dyssomnias and
parasomnias in early childhood. Pediatrics, 119(5), 1016–1025.
Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Gatenberg, C., Gore, J.C., Grillon, C., & Davis, M. (2001).
Activation of the left amygdala to cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 4(4),
437–441. Available: http://dionysus.psych.wisc.edu/Lit/Articles/PhelpsE2001a.pdf
Pichon, A., & Chapelot, D. (2010). Homeostatic role of the parasympathetic nervous systems in
human behavior. New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Pilcher, J. J., & Huffcutt, A. I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A meta-
analysis. Sleep, 19(4), 318–326
Pinel, J. P. (2003). Biopsychology (5th ed.). New York: Allyn & Bacon.
Pinel, J. P., & Dehaene, S. (2010). Beyond hemispheric dominance: Brain regions underlying
joint lateralization of language and arithmetic to the left hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 22(1), 48–66. Available: www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/
jocn.2009.21184
Plomin, R. (1988). The nature and nurture of cognitive abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances
in the psychology of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
196
References
Potegal, M., Stemmler, G., & Spielberger, C. (Eds.) (2010). International handbook of anger:
Constituent and concomitant biological, psychological and social processes. New York:
Springer.
Premack, D. (1983). Animal cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 34, 351–362.
Proctor, M. R. (2002). Spinal cord injury. Critical Care Medicine, 30(11), 489–499. Available:
www.sassit.co.za/Journals/Trauma/Paediatrics/spinal%20injury.pdf
Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis
for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360.
QAA (2010). Quality Assurance Agency benchmark for psychology. London: Quality Assurance
Agency.
Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., LeCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2002). Reduced
prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality
disorder. Archives in General Psychiatry, 57(2), 119–127.
Ranabir, S., & Reetu, K. (2011). Stress and hormones. Indian Journal of Endocrinology and
Metabolism, 15(1), 18–22.
Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual
search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506. Available:
http://benschweitzer.org/DROPBOX/litreview/rayner-Eye%20movements%20and%
20attention%20in%20reading.pdf
Rechschaffen, A., & Bergmann, B. M. (1995). Sleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water
method. Behavioural Brain Research, 69(1), 55–63.
Rechschaffen, A., Bergmann, B. M., Everson, C. A., Kushida, C. A., & Gilliland, M. A. (1989).
Sleep deprivation in the rat: Integration and discussion of the findings. Sleep, 12, 68–87.
Rechschaffen, A., Gilliland, M. A., Bergmann, B. M., & Winter, J. B. (1983). Physiological
correlates of prolonged sleep deprivation in rats. Science, 221, 182–184.
Reilly, T., Atkinson, G., & Waterhouse, J. (1997). Travel fatigue and jet-lag. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 15(3), 365–369.
Reiter, R. J. (1991). Pineal gland interface between the photoperiodic environment and the
endocrine system. Trends in Endocrinology and Metabolism, 2(1), 13–19.
Robertson, L. C., Knight, R. T., Rafal, R., & Shimamura, A. P. (1993). Cognitive neuropsychology
is more than single-case studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory
and Cognition, 19(3), 710–717. Available: http://istsocrates.berkeley.edu/~shimlab/1993_
Robertson_JEPLMC-case.pdf
Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on
fear circuitry. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32(1), 289–313. Available: http://dionysus.
psych.wisc.edu/Lit/Articles/RodriguesS2009a.pdf
Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2011). The role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664–1680.
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological
Review, 110(1), 145–172. Available: http://directory.umm.ac.id/articles/psyc-rev2003.pdf
Russell, W. R., & Espir, M. L. E. (1961). Traumatic aphasia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sack, R. L., & Lewy, A. J. (1997). Melatonin as a chronobiotic: treatment of circadian desynchrony
in night workers and the blind. Journal of Biological Rhythms; 12(6), 595–603.
Sacks, O. (1985). The man who mistook his wife for a hat and other clinical tales. New York:
Summit Books.
Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1995). Neurobiology of skill and habit learning. Current Opinions in
Neurobiology, 152(1), 1–6.
Saper, C. B., Scammell, T., & Lu, J. (2005). Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian
rhythms. Nature, 437, 1257–1263.
Sassi, R. B., Nicoletti, M., Brambilla, P., Mallinger, A. G., Frank, E., Kuffer, D. J., Keshavan, M. S.,
& Soares, J. C. (2002). Increased gray matter volume in Lithium-treated bipolar disorder
patients. Neuroscience Letters, 392(2), 243–245.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rupert, E. (1986).
Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan
paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 211–235.
197
References
198
References
199
References
Wright, I. C., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Woodruff, P. W. R., David, A. S., Murray, R. M., & Bullmore,
E. T. (2000). Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 157(1), 16–25.
Wundt, W. (1902). Principles of physiological psychology. Available: http://psychclassics.yorku.
ca/Wundt/Physio/
Yehuda, R. (2001). Biology of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62(17),
41–46.
Zager, A., Anderson, M. L., Ruiz, F. S., Antunes, I. B., & Tufik, S. (2007). Effects of acute and
chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative
Psychology, 293, 504–509.
Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2009). Auditory event files: Integrating auditory perception and
action planning. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 71(2), 352–362. Available: http://
home.planet.nl/~homme247/Auditory%20event%20file.pdf
Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of personality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
200
Index
201
Index
202
Index
203
Index
hallucinations 115, 116, 156, 163, 164, 167 language 33, 34, 44, 63–79, 85
handedness 70, 73 cognitive neuroscience 76, 77–8
‘helping-hand’ phenomenon 71 defining 65–9
heredity 17, 18–19, 165, 170, 179, 186 lateralisation 69–73, 187
see also genetics localisation 72, 73–5, 187
hippocampus 32, 124 units of 67
memory 139, 140, 142, 145–6, 148 Wernicke–Geschwind model of 75–6
REM sleep 104 see also aphasia
schizophrenia 165 lateralisation 69–73, 187
homeostasis 39, 40, 106, 186 learning 111, 136–7, 138, 187
homosexuality 57, 154 Lee–Boot effect 57
hormones 26, 35, 42, 48–9, 51–2, 186 lesions 7, 8, 11, 15, 57, 127, 187
circadian clock 106 emotional expression 130–1
depression 157 hippocampus 145, 146, 148
emotion 128–30 Korsakoff’s syndrome 144
foetal development 54–5 sleep 100
gender differences 53, 60–1 somatosensation 93
growth 102–3, 109 lexicon 67
hypothalamus 33 linguistic relativity 67
imbalances 58, 59 linguistic universals 65–6, 187
sexual behaviour 56–8 lithium 162, 163
humorism 4, 7, 186 localisation 72, 73–5, 187
hypofrontality 165 long-term memory 137, 142, 187
hypomania 160–1, 186 long-term potentiation 139, 140, 187
204
Index
205
Index
206
Index
207
Index
208