WestminsterResearch
http://www.westminster.ac.uk/westminsterresearch
Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis, and Psychometric
Properties, of a Spanish Translation of the Body Appreciation
Scale-2 (BAS-2)
Swami, V., García, A. A. and Barron, D.
NOTICE: this is the authors’ version of a work that was accepted for publication in Body
Image. Changes resulting from the publishing process, such as peer review, editing,
corrections, structural formatting, and other quality control mechanisms may not be
reflected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was
submitted for publication. A definitive version was subsequently published in Body
Image, 22, pp. 13-17, 2017.
The final definitive version in Body Image is available online at:
https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.05.002
© 2017. This manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
The WestminsterResearch online digital archive at the University of Westminster aims to make the
research output of the University available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain
with the authors and/or copyright owners.
Whilst further distribution of specific materials from within this archive is forbidden, you may freely
distribute the URL of WestminsterResearch: ((http://westminsterresearch.wmin.ac.uk/).
In case of abuse or copyright appearing without permission e-mail [email protected]
Running head: BODY APPRECIATION SCALE-2
Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis, and Psychometric Properties, of a Spanish
Translation of the Body Appreciation Scale-2 (BAS-2)
Viren Swami1-2, Antonio Alías García3, & David Barron4
1Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, UK
2Department of Psychology, HELP University College, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
3Department of Education, University of Almeria, Almeria, Spain
4Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK
Address for correspondence: Prof. Viren Swami, Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin
University, East Road, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire CB1 1PT, UK. Email:
[email protected].
Body Appreciation Scale-2 2
Abstract
We examined the psychometric properties of a Spanish translation of the Body Appreciation
Scale-2 (BAS-2) in a community sample of 411 women and 389 men in Almería, Spain.
Participants completed the 10-item BAS-2 along with measures of appearance evaluation, body
areas satisfaction, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and self-reported body mass index (BMI).
Exploratory factor analyses with one split-half subsample revealed that BAS-2 scores had a one-
dimensional factor structure in women and men. Confirmatory factor analysis with a second split-
half subsample showed the one-dimensional factor structure had acceptable fit and was invariant
across sex. There were no significant sex differences in BAS-2 scores. BAS-2 scores were
significantly and positively correlated with appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, self-
esteem, and life satisfaction. Body appreciation was significantly and negatively correlated with
BMI in men, but associations in women were only significant in the second subsample. Results
suggest that the Spanish BAS-2 has adequate psychometric properties.
Keywords: Body appreciation; Positive body image; Spain; Translation; BAS-2
Body Appreciation Scale-2 3
Introduction
Body appreciation is defined as “accepting, holding favorable opinions toward, and
respecting the body, while also rejecting media-promoted appearance ideals as the only form of
human beauty” (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015, p. 53). The construct was initially measured
using the 13-item Body Appreciation Scale (BAS), with the parent study supporting a one-
dimensional factor structure in English-speaking U.S. adults (Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow,
2005). However, some studies conducted with non-English-speaking samples have instead
reported that BAS scores reduce to two distinct dimensions, tapping body appreciation and body
image investment separately (for reviews, see Swami, 2017; Webb, Wood-Barcalow, & Tylka,
2015). In light of this issue, and to keep pace with developments in the conceptualisation of body
appreciation, Tylka and Wood-Barcalow (2015) developed a revision of the scale, known as the
Body Appreciation Scale-2 (BAS-2).
In the parent study with U.S. adults, Tylka and Wood-Barcalow (2015) found through
exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) that the 10-item BAS-2
had a one-dimensional factor structure, with adequate internal consistency. Studies using EFA
have also supported this one-dimensional structure for scores derived from Cantonese (Swami &
Ng, 2015), Persian (Atari, 2016), Serbian (Jovic, Sfroza, Jovanovic, & Jovic, 2016), Icelandic
(Pálmarsdóttir & Karlsdóttir, 2016), and Dutch (Alleva, Martijn, Veldhuis, & Tylka, 2016)
translations of the BAS-2. In addition, one study using CFA indicated that scores from a Standard
Chinese version of the scale had adequate fit (Swami, Ng, & Barron, 2016). Finally, support for a
one-dimensional structure has been provided for French BAS-2 scores using both EFA and CFA
(Kertechian & Swami, 2017). These studies suggest that the BAS-2 retains its one-dimensional
factor structure across distinct linguistic and national groups.
Body Appreciation Scale-2 4
In addition, several studies have indicated that BAS-2 scores are invariant across
participant sex (Kertechian & Swami, 2017; Swami et al., 2016; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow,
2015). Most studies that have examined sex differences in BAS-2 scores have reported that men
have significantly higher body appreciation than women (Atari, 2016; Kertechian & Swami,
2017; Swami & Ng, 2015; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015). However, the effect sizes of the
reported differences have generally been small-to-moderate (ds = 0.15-0.58) and one study found
no significant difference between women and men from mainland China (Swami et al., 2016).
Beyond sex differences, the available evidence also suggests that BAS-2 scores have adequate
internal consistency coefficients across studies, as well as good patterns of convergent validity.
Only the parent study has examined test-retest reliability, finding that BAS-2 scores retained their
reliability up to three weeks (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015).
In the present study, we examined the psychometric properties of a Spanish translation of
the BAS-2 in a community sample of adults. Doing so is important as it not only extends the
available cross-cultural research on the BAS-2, but it may also provide a useful tool for the
assessment of positive body image in Spanish-speaking populations. Indeed, based on the
estimated number of mother-tongue speakers, Spanish is the second-most widely spoken
language in the world (Paulillo & Das, 2006). Specifically, we examined factor structure of
Spanish BAS-2 scores using both EFA and CFA, and expected to find support for a one-
dimensional structure that includes all 10 items. We also examined whether BAS-2 scores were
invariant across sex and hypothesised that men would have significantly higher body appreciation
than women. Finally, we examined the convergent validity of BAS-2 scores by examining
associations with appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, self-esteem, life satisfaction,
and self-reported body mass index (BMI). These variables were selected on the basis of
availability of previously-validated measures in Spanish and based on previous assessments of
Body Appreciation Scale-2 5
the convergent validity of BAS-2 scores. In both women and men, evidence of convergent
validity would be provided to the extent that body appreciation was positively associated with
appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, self-esteem, and life satisfaction, and negatively
associated with BMI.
Method
Participants
Participants of this study were 411 women and 389 men recruited from the community in
Almería, a city in Andalusia, Spain. All participants were citizens of Spain (pueblo español) and
most self-reported as Christians (83.3%). Participants ranged in age from 17 to 86 years (M =
26.77, SD = 11.25) and in BMI from 14.56-43.56 kg/m2 (M = 24.09, SD = 4.12). Most
participants were single (65.1%), while others were married (12.3%), cohabiting but unmarried
(11.0%), or of some other marital status.
Measures
Body appreciation. Participants completed a Spanish translation of BAS-2 (Tylka &
Wood-Barcalow, 2015b). All items are presented in Table 1 and were rated on a 5-point scale,
ranging from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always). The BAS-2 was first translated from English into Spanish
using the parallel back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1986). A bilingual individual unaffiliated
with the study translated the scale from English to Spanish, while a second individual translated
this version back into English. Next, the items obtained were assessed by a committee consisting
of the individuals who participated in the translation process, the second author, and two
psychology professors who settled minor discrepancies in the translation through consensus.
Appearance evaluation. We used the Appearance Evaluation Subscale of the
Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire–Appearance Subscales (MBSRQ-AS;
Cash, 2000; Spanish translation: Roncero, Perpiña, Marco, & Sánchez-Reales, 2015). This is a 7-
Body Appreciation Scale-2 6
item measure that assesses one’s beliefs and feelings of satisfaction or dissatisfaction with
appearance. All items were rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (Definitely disagree) to 5
(Definitely agree) and an overall score was computed as the mean of all items so that higher
scores reflect more positive appearance evaluation. CFA indicated that the Spanish translation of
the Appearance Evaluation Subscale retained all items as its parent version, showed good fit
indices, and had adequate internal consistency coefficients (Roncero et al., 2015). In the present
study, the Appearance Evaluation Subscale had adequate internal consistency (Cronbach α = .76).
Body areas satisfaction. The questionnaire included the Body Areas Satisfaction
Subscale of the MBSRQ-AS (Cash, 2000; Spanish translation: Roncero et al., 2015). This
subscale includes 9 items that measure satisfaction with specific body areas or attributes. All
items were rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (Very dissatisfied) to 5 (Very satisfied). An
overall score was calculated as the mean of all items. Higher scores on this subscale reflect
greater body areas satisfaction. The Spanish version of the Body Areas Satisfaction Subscale
retains all items as its parent version and has been shown to have good fit indices and adequate
internal consistency (Roncero et al., 2015). In the present study, the Body Areas Satisfaction
Subscale had adequate internal consistency (Cronbach α = .79).
Self-esteem. Participants were asked to complete the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
(RSES; Rosenberg, 1965; Spanish translation: Martín-Albo, Núñez, Navarro, & Grijalvo, 2007).
The RSES is a 10-item measure of an individual’s overall sense of self-worth and items were
rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 4 (Strongly agree). An overall
score was computed as the mean of all items, with higher scores reflecting greater self-esteem.
Scores on the Spanish RSES have good (CFA-derived) factorial and convergent validity,
adequate internal consistencies, and good test-rest reliability up to 4 weeks (Martín-Albo et al.,
2007). In the present study, Cronbach α for this scale was .84.
Body Appreciation Scale-2 7
Life satisfaction. Life satisfaction was measured using the 5-item Satisfaction With Life
Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Spanish translation: Vásquez, Duque,
& Hervás, 2013), which assesses satisfaction with the quality of one’s life. Items were rated on a
7-point scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree). An overall score was
computed as the mean of all items, with higher scores reflecting greater life satisfaction. A CFA
of Spanish SWLS scores with a representative sample of Spanish adults showed that the one-
factor solution with all 5 items had good fit with adequate internal consistency (Vásquez et al.,
2013). Here, Cronbach α for the SWLS was .86.
Demographics. Participants were asked to provide their demographic information,
consisting of sex, age, citizenship, religion, and marital status. We also obtained self-reported
height and weight data to computed BMI as kg/m2. Previous work has indicated that, in Spanish
populations, self-reported BMI is strongly associated with objectively-measured BMI in non-
elderly age groups (Alvarez-Torices, Franch-Nadal, Alvarez-Guisasola, Hernandez-Mejia, &
Cueto-Espinar, 1993).
Procedures
Ethics approval for this project was obtained from the Bioethics Committee of Human
Research at the University of Almería. Two research assistants opportunistically recruited
participants from public areas of congregate activities (e.g., shopping streets, parks) in Almería
between November 2016 and January 2017. More specifically, the research assistants directly
approached potential participants and provided them with information about the study (i.e., it was
advertised as research on health and appearance). To be eligible for participation, participants
were required to be citizens of Spain (pueblo español) and above the age of 16. Those who
agreed to participate provided written informed consent and completed an anonymous paper-and-
pencil version of the questionnaire. This was done in a quiet area that we set up in areas of
Body Appreciation Scale-2 8
congregate activity for the purposes of this study. The order of presentation of the scales listed
above was semi-randomised, with the demographic items always appearing last. All participation
was voluntary and participants were not remunerated in any form. Upon return of completed
questionnaires, participants were provided with written debrief information.
Statistical Analyses
To examine the psychometric properties of the Spanish BAS-2, we used a two-step
analytic strategy. First, the total dataset was split using a computer-generated random seed. The
factor structure of the BAS-2 was assessed in the first split-half (n = 400) using principal-axis
EFA in IBM SPSS Statistics v.20. The sample size in this split-half far exceeded conservative
item-to-participant ratio requirements (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). A quartimax rotation was
used, as we expected a single, orthogonal factor (Pedhazur & Schmelkin, 1990). Factor loadings
were interpreted using Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2013) recommendations, with loadings of .71
and above considered excellent, .63-.70 considered very good, .55-.62 considered good, .33-.45
considered fair, and .32 or lower considered poor.
Data from the second split-half subsample (n = 400) were subjected to CFA using
Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS v.23). Hypothesized modelling was based on the results
of the EFA in the first split-half, as well as the expected one-dimensional factor structure.
Standard goodness-of-fit indices were selected a priori to assess the measurement models (Hu &
Bentler, 1999). The normed model chi-square (χ²normed) is reported with lower values of the
overall model χ² indicating goodness-of-fit. A χ²normed value of < 3.00 indicates good fit. The
Steiger-Lind root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its 90% confidence interval
provide a correction for model complexity. RMSEA values close to .06 indicate a good fit, with
values ranging to .10 representing a mediocre fit. The standardized root mean square residual
(SRMR) assesses the mean absolute correlation residual and is a badness-of-fit index: the smaller
Body Appreciation Scale-2 9
the SRMR, the better the model fit. A cut-off value for SRMR is recommended to be close to or
< .09. The comparative fit index (CFI) measures the proportionate improvement in fit by
comparing a target model with a more restricted, nested baseline model. The CFI reflects a
goodness-of-fit index and is recommended to close to or > .95 for adequate fit.
We also examined whether BAS-2 scores were invariant across sex at the configural (i.e.,
whether similar factors are measured), factor loading (i.e., whether the magnitude of factor
loadings is the same), and intercept (i.e., whether the intercept of the regression relating each item
to its factor is the same) levels (Chen, 2007). In both split-halves separately, we examined sex
differences in body appreciation scores, computed internal consistency coefficients using
Cronbach’s α, and examined correlations between body appreciation, appearance evaluation,
body areas satisfaction, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and BMI.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analyses
Female sample (n = 196). Means and standard deviations for all BAS-2 items are
presented in Table 1. These data were suitable for EFA based on item distribution, average
correlation with other items, and item-total correlations (Clark & Watson, 1995). Both the
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (.92) and the significance of
Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2(45) = 1004.49, p < .001, indicated that these data had adequate
factorability. The results of the principal-axis EFA indicated that only a single factor should be
extracted (λ = 5.40), explaining 54.0% of the total variance. As can be seen in Table 1, all items
had good-to-excellent factor loadings.
Male sample (n = 204). Descriptive statistics for all 10 items for men are reported Table
1. These data passed standard criteria for EFA (Clark & Watson, 1995). The KMO measure of
sampling adequacy was .93 and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, χ2(45) = 1082.79, p <
Body Appreciation Scale-2 10
.001, indicative of adequate factorability. As with the female sample, the results of a principal-
axis EFA suggested that a single factor should be extracted. This factor had an eigenvalue of 5.55
and explained 55.5% of the total variance. As reported in Table 1, all items had good-to-excellent
loadings.
Further analyses. For both women and men, we computed total BAS-2 by taking the
mean of all items. Internal consistency coefficients were adequate for women (Cronbach α = .90,
95% CI = .88-.92) and men (Cronbach α = .91, 95% CI = .89-.93). An independent-samples t-test
indicated that there was no significant sex difference on BAS-2 scores between women (M =
3.77, SD = 0.75) and men (M = 3.84, SD = 0.72), t(398) = 0.92, p = .359, d = 0.10. In women,
body appreciation was significantly and positively correlated with appearance evaluation, body
areas satisfaction, self-esteem, and life satisfaction, but not with BMI. In men, body appreciation
was significantly correlated with appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, life satisfaction,
BMI, and self-esteem (see Table 2).
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Hypothesized model fit (n = 400). The results of the EFAs with the first split-half
pointed to a one-dimensional factor structure for Spanish BAS-2 scores, with all 10 items loading
onto this dimension. We, therefore, examined the fit of this hypothesised one-factor solution
using CFA with the second split-half sample. The standardised estimates of factor loadings for
the best-fitting model were all very good-to-excellent (see Figure 1). Fit indices values were
found to be: χ² M(35, N = 400) = 188.741, χ²normed = 5.393, CFI = .944, RMSEA = .105 with 90%
CI = .091-.120, SRMR = .036. Since the fit indices values of analysis were not found to be at
acceptable intervals, suggested modification indices were taken into account to improve the
model. Modification indices were consulted to free error covariances (items #1 and #2, and items
#2 and #8). This one-dimensional structure provided an acceptable fit to the data: χ² M(33, N =
Body Appreciation Scale-2 11
400) = 129.784, χ² normed = 3.933, CFI = .965, RMSEA = .086 (low = .071, high = .102), SRMR =
.031.
Sex invariance. The unconstrained model had adequate fit in the female (n = 215) and
male (n = 185) sub-samples individually (see Table 3 for sub-sample metrics), suggesting
configural invariance between the sexes. Differences between the unconstrained and fully
constrained model were not significant, indicating that the structure of the model achieved factor
loading invariance across sex, Δχ²(9) = 4.341, p = .888. Finally, intercept invariance was
evaluated, where all item-factor intercepts were constrained equally across participant sex and
evaluated against the factor loading invariance model. According to the Δχ², intercept invariance
was evident, Δχ²(10) = 2.94, p = .983. Therefore, from these data, we find acceptable evidence
for the one-dimensional structure for the BAS-2 across sex.
Further analyses. Internal consistency coefficients for overall BAS-2 scores were
adequate in women (Cronbach α = .94, 95% CI = .93-.95) and men (Cronbach α = .93, 95% CI =
.91-.95). In this sub-sample, there was no significant difference in body appreciation between
women (M = 3.81, SD = 0.81) and men (M = 3.78, SD = 0.79), t(398) = 0.42, p = .675, d = 0.04.
In both women and men, body appreciation was significant correlated with appearance
evaluation, body areas satisfaction, life satisfaction, self-esteem, and BMI (see Table 4).
Discussion
Previous studies have reported that BAS-2 scores have a one-dimensional factor structure
in distinct linguistic and national groups (Alleva et al., 2016; Atari, 2016; Jovic et al., 2016;
Kertechian & Swami, 2017; Pálmarsdóttir & Karlsdóttir, 2016; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015;
Swami & Ng, 2015; Swami et al., 2016). Our results are consistent with this literature: using
EFA, we found that all 10 items of the Spanish BAS-2 loaded onto the same latent dimension in a
first split-half subsample. Furthermore, using CFA, we were able to confirm that this one-
Body Appreciation Scale-2 12
dimensional model had adequate fit in a second split-half subsample following modifications.
Taken together, the available evidence suggests that BAS-2 scores retain a one-dimensional
factor across different cultural groups. This is important as it will facilitate effective cross-
cultural comparisons of body appreciation in future work.
Our results also indicated that Spanish BAS-2 scores were invariant across participant
sex, which is consistent with previous findings (Kertechian & Swami, 2017; Swami et al., 2016;
Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015). However, contrary to our hypothesis, we found that there were
no significant sex differences in body appreciation in both split-half subsamples. The available
literature on sex differences in body appreciation is mixed, with one study reporting no
significant difference (Swami et al., 2016) and others reporting significant differences with small-
to-moderate effects (Atari, 2016; Kertechian & Swami, 2017; Swami & Ng, 2015; Tylka &
Wood-Barcalow, 2015). Furthermore, a previous study of Spanish adolescents who completed
the BAS also reported a significant sex difference, though again the effect size was small
(Jáuregui Lobera & Bolaños Ríos, 2011). Tiggemmann (2015) has cautioned against drawing
strong conclusions about (the lack of) sex differences in body appreciation in the absence of other
axes of social identity, such as ethnicity or class, as it is likely that the intersection of these axes
determines the experience of body appreciation across social identities.
Our results also indicated that body appreciation scores had adequate internal consistency
coefficients in women and men. Further, we found that Spanish BAS-2 scores had good patterns
of convergent validity: body appreciation was significantly and positively associated with
appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, self-esteem, and life satisfaction in both women
and men. Evidence of convergent validity in terms of BMI, however, was mixed: while we found
the expected negative association in men in both split-half samples, the association in women was
weak in one split-half and did not reach significance in the other. The mixed findings with
Body Appreciation Scale-2 13
women stand in contrast to previous studies that have consistently reported significant negative
correlations (Alleva et al., 2016; Atari, 2016; Kertechian & Swami, 2017; Swami & Ng, 2015;
Swami et al., 2016; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015), although it should be acknowledged that
reported associations have tended to be weak. This aspect of our results could be investigated
further, as it may be suggestive of problems with using self-reported BMI as an index of
convergent validity for BAS-2 scores.
The present study benefitted from using a community sample of Spanish adults, but a
number of sampling limitations should be highlighted. First, our recruitment strategy means that
our sample is unlikely to be representative of the wider Spanish population. Likewise, although
we limited participation to citizens of Spain (pueblo español), we did not request information
about ethnicity (itself a problematic concept in the Spanish context). Future research could extend
the present findings by examining the psychometric properties of the Spanish BAS-2 in other
national groups that speak Spanish (e.g. in Central and South America), as well as in other sub-
national regions of Spain. Future work could also examine associations between body
appreciation and a wider set of factors to establish the divergent and incremental validity of
Spanish BAS-2 scores. Nevertheless, the present study presents preliminary evidence that the
Spanish BAS-2 may be useful tool for the assessment of positive body image in Spanish-
speaking populations.
Body Appreciation Scale-2 14
References
Alleva, J. M., Martijn, C., Veldhuis, J., & Tylka, T. L. (2016). A Dutch translation and validation
of the Body Appreciation Scale-2: An investigation with female university students in the
Netherlands. Body Image, 19, 44-48. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.08.008
Alvarez-Torices, J. C., Franch-Nadal, J., Alvarez-Guisasola, F., Hernandez-Mejia, R., & Cueto-
Espinar, A. (1993). Self-reported height and weight and prevalence of obesity: Study in a
Spanish population. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 17,
663-667.
Atari, M. (2016). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Body Appreciation Scale-2
in Iran. Body Image, 18, 1-4. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04.006
Avalos, L. C., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale:
Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2, 285-297.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.06.002
Brislin, R.W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In W. Lonner & J.
Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research (pp. 137-164). Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
Cash, T. F. (2000). The MBSRQ users’ manual (3rd ed.). Available from the author at www.body-
image.com.
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indices to lack of measurement invariance.
Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464-504. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1203_7
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75. doi:10.1207/ s15327752jpa4901_13
Body Appreciation Scale-2 15
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.
doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Jovic, M., Sforza, M., Jovanovic, M., & Jovic, M. (2016). The Acceptance of Cosmetic Surgery
Scale: Confirmatory factor analyses and validation among Serbian adults. Current
Psychology, in press. doi:10.1007/s12144-016-9458-7
Kertechian, S., & Swami, V. (2017). Psychometric evaluation of a French translation of the Body
Appreciation Scale-2 in university students. Body Image, in press.
Jáuregui Lobera, I., & Bolaños Ríos, P. (2011). Spanish version of the Body Appreciation Scale
(BAS) for adolescents. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 411-420.
doi:10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.b1.37
Martín-Albo-Núñez, J. L., Navarro, J. G., & Grijalvo, F. (2007). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem
Scale: Translation and validation in university students. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10,
458-467. doi:10.1017/S1138741600006727
Pálmarsdóttir, Þ. G., & Karlsdóttir, A. E. (2016). Psychometric properties of the Icelandic
translation of the Body Appreciation Scale-2. Dissertation, University of Iceland. Available
at: http://skemman.is/en/item/view/1946/24746
Paulillo, J. C., & Das, A. (2006). Evaluating language statistics: The ethnologue and beyond.
Paris: UNESCO Institute of Statistics.
Pedhazur, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. P. (1991). Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated
approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Roncero, M., Perpiña, C,. Marco, J. H., & Sánchez-Reales, S. (2015). Confirmatory factor
analysis and psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Multidimensional Body-
Body Appreciation Scale-2 16
Self Relations Questionnaire-Appearance Scales. Body Image, 14, 47-53.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.005
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Swami, V. (2017, forthcoming). Considering positive body image through the lens of culture and
minority social identities. In C. Markey, E. Daniels, & M. Gillen (Eds.), The body positive:
Understanding and improving body image in science and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Swami, V., & Ng, S.-K. (2015). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Body
Appreciation Scale-2 among adults in Hong Kong. Body Image, 15, 68-71.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.009
Swami, V., Ng, S.-K., & Barron, D. (2016). Translation and psychometric evaluation of a
Standard Chinese version of the Body Appreciation Scale-2. Body Image, 18, 23-26.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04.005
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). New York, NY:
Pearson.
Tiggemann, M. (2015). Considerations of positive body image across various social identities and
special populations. Body Image, 14, 168-176. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.002
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015). The Body Appreciation Scale-2: Item refinement
and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12, 53-67. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006
Vásquez, C., Duque, A., & Hervás, G. (2013). Satisfaction With Life Scale in a representative
sample of Spanish adults: Validation and normative data. Spanish Journal of Psychology,
16, 1-15. doi:10.1017/sjp.2013.82
Body Appreciation Scale-2 17
Webb, J. B., Wood-Barcalow, N. L., & Tylka, T. L. (2015). Assessing positive body image:
Contemporary approaches and future directions. Body Image, 14, 130-145.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.010
Table 1. Items of the Body Appreciation Scales-2, Descriptive Statistics for Women and Men, and Item-Factor Loadings in the First
Split-Half Sample (n = 400)
Item Female sample (n = Male sample (n = 204)
196)
M (SD) Loading M (SD) Loading
1. I respect my body/Respecto mi cuerpo 3.89 (1.03) .65 4.04 (0.92) .69
2. I feel good about my body/Me siento bien con mi cuerpo 3.85 (0.99) .82 3.84 (0.94) .82
3. I feel that my body has at least some good qualities/Siento que mi cuerpo 3.73 (0.97) .74 3.89 (0.91) .77
tiene buenas cualidades
4. I take a positive attitude towards my body/Tengo una actitud positiva hacia 3.87 (1.01) .84 3.87 (0.96) .80
mi cuerpo
5. I am attentive to my body’s needs/Estoy atento a las necesidades de mi 3.77 (0.99) .63 3.81 (0.92) .55
cuerpo
6. I feel love for my body/Siento amor por mi cuerpo 3.54 (1.12) .77 3.69 (1.02) .79
7. I appreciate the different and unique characteristics of my body/Aprecio las 3.65 (1.06) .79 3.71 (1.00) .74
características únicas y diferentes de mi cuerpo
Body Appreciation Scale-2 19
8. My behaviour reveals my positive attitude toward my body; for example, I 3.80 (1.03) .65 3.81 (0.98) .76
hold my head high and smile/Mi comportamiento revela mi actitud positive
sobre mi cuerpo; por ejemplo, yo levanto mi cabeza y sonrío
9. I am confortable in my body/Estoy cómodo/a con mi cuerpo 3.77 (1.03) .83 3.83 (1.00) .83
10. I feel like I am beautiful even if I am diffeent from media images of 3.86 (1.01) .58 3.89 (1.04) .65
attractive people (e.g., models, actresses/actors)/Me siento bien, aunque soy
diferente a los prototipos de belleza (EJ: modelos, actores)
Table 2. Correlation Matrix for Women (n = 196; Top Diagonal) and Men (n = 204; Bottom
Diagonal) in the First Split-Half Sample
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
(1) Body appreciation .66* .62* .40* .35* -.09
(2) Appearance evaluation .69* .54* .39* .27* -.25*
(3) Body areas satisfaction .64* .66* .36* .30* -.09
(4) Self-esteem .36* .32* .23* .34* -.04
(5) Life satisfaction .41* .37* .40* .33* -.07
(6) Body mass index -.38* -.36* -.36* -.08 -.13
Note. *p < .001.
Body Appreciation Scale-2 21
Table 3. Model Fit Indices and Tests of Measurement Invariance for the One-Factor Body
Appreciation Scale-2 Model Across Participant Sex
χ² M df M χ² normed RMSEA SRMR CFI
(90% CI)
Men (n = 185) 98.666 33 2.990 .104 (.081, .128) .042 .946
Women (n = 215) 85.448 33 2.589 .086 (.064, .109) .034 .966
Configural Invariance 184.124 66 2.790 .067 (.056, .079) .042 .958
Factor Loading Invariance 188.465 75 2.513 .062 (.051, .073) .043 .959
Intercept Invariance 191.405 85 2.252 .056 (.046, .067) .043 .962
Body Appreciation Scale-2 22
Table 4. Correlation Matrix for Women (n = 215; Top Diagonal) and Men (n = 185; Bottom
Diagonal) in the Second Split-Half Sample
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
(1) Body appreciation .67** .67** .34** .36** -.15*
(2) Appearance evaluation .67** .72** .45** .39** -.24**
(3) Body areas satisfaction .63** .56** .42** .39** -.14*
(4) Self-esteem .31** .34** .23** .34** -.05
(5) Life satisfaction .39** .29** .40** .36** -.15*
(6) Body mass index -.32** -.35** -.19* .02 -.12
Note. *p < .001.
Body Appreciation Scale-2 23
Figure 1. Path diagram and estimates for the one-dimensional model of the Body Appreciation
Scale-2. The large oval is the latent construct, with the rectangles representing measured
variables, and the small circles with numbers representing the residual variables (variances). The
path factor loadings are standardised with significance levels were determined by critical ratios
(all p < .001).