James, A. (2008) - Play in Childhood, An Anthropological Perspective. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 3, 104-109.
James, A. (2008) - Play in Childhood, An Anthropological Perspective. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 3, 104-109.
Play in Childhood : An
Anthropological Perspective
Allison James
often take place alongside those of work and do not perspective immediately casts doubts, then, on the utility
constitute a separate domain of action (Reynolds, 1989). of universal definitions of childhood ; it therefore also
Additionally, as noted, in many societies such as those questions any attempt to develop a universal formulation
described by Rossie (1993) for Africa, ‘ play ’ is not of the role or purposes of children’s play. If childhood
reserved for children, while as Briggs (1986) observes, takes on particular local forms for children, then what
among the Inuit, the games adults play with children are play ‘ means ’ in any culture and for any group of children
highly dangerous and frightening for children and rep- must be explored in that local social context.
resent serious social lessons.
Central to this discussion is the status of children’s culture
An insistence on cultural specificity has encouraged social vis-a' -vis the adult world and the extent to which the
anthropologists, therefore, to abandon the task of seeking ‘ culture of childhood ’ can be said to represent a separate
universal definitions of play, of endeavouring to define its and autonomous children’s world (James, Jenks, & Prout,
purpose, or of trying to separate out play and work as 1998). Clearly, in many cultures, sets of cultural practices
necessarily distinct kinds of activities that children do. (Bourdieu, 1977) specific to children can be identified,
Rather, attention has turned to documenting the activities such as those documented by the Opies (Opie & Opie,
locally embraced as play and to the qualities and exper- 1969, 1977) for Britain. In their seminal work on the lore,
iences attributed to those activities. Thus, in the 1970s, language, and games of childhood the Opies were among
Schwartzman (1978) took the lead in interpreting the first to show that the rhymes and games that children
children’s play cross-culturally, seeing it as an important teach one another are peculiar to children and different
socialising medium through which children learn about from the nursery rhymes and games that adults teach their
the affective and cognitive systems of their particular offspring. They established that many of these children’s
culture. In this sense play was, for her, to be regarded as rhymes and games have a considerable antiquity and that
transformative of children. For Sutton-Smith (1977), on their mode of transmission—between generations of chil-
the other hand, play was best to be regarded as that which dren who pass into and out of childhood—has helped
potentiates rather than simply socialises children. In his perpetuate a distinctive and thriving oral children’s
view, play offers an arena of activity through which culture. At the same time, however, the Opies indicated
children explore and come to understand the social world, that these cultural practices were not simply a branch of
and through this activity children generate social meanings folklore but, instead, very useful practices used by children
that may, but often do not, reflect those of the adult world. in their daily lives. Teasing rhymes and ritual games are
Thus play may be as much about nonsense as about sense. easily adapted by children in their social encounters in the
The ethnographer must be sensitive to this when seeing playground. They are used by children as specific means
play as taking on a ‘ socialising ’ function : play may be as for achieving specific ends. Thus, according to the Opies
much about transformation as imitation and as much (Opie & Opie, 1977, p. 22), the twin themes of con-
about social disorder as about order. It is best regarded as servation and innovation permeate ‘ a thriving unself-
a ‘ cognitive activity which liberates thought ’, something conscious culture ’ belonging exclusively to children.
that permits the exploration of innovative as well as
routine, socialising roles (Sutton-Smith, 1977, p. 236). However, within the new social studies of childhood the
postulation of a separate culture of childhood is no longer
In this article I build on some of these approaches by regarded as theoretically sound (James et al., 1998).
considering what an anthropological approach to the Though a seductive proposition, in that it provides
study of ‘ childhood ’ can contribute to the understanding conceptual space for the voice of the child that is central to
of children’s play through the use of the ethnographic an anthropological approach to childhood, it is clear that
method of participant observation. In brief, I argue that it the activities of children are always and necessarily
facilitates a view of childhood play as a contextualised, contextualised by the adult world. Thus, children’s play
rather than simply naturalised, feature of children’s lives and games, although certainly a distinctive feature of child
and is a view of play that makes no a priori assumptions social life in Western industrialised societies, cannot be
about its socialising function. conceptually separated off from the adult world into an
insular children’s culture. However, in rejecting such a
view it is not necessary to retreat to the older argument
The culture of childhood that saw children’s play and games as simply characterised
This anthropological approach to children’s play thus by a passive mimicry. Rather, taking up Geertz’s (1976)
departs from more common understandings of play as point that any culture ‘ consists of socially established
purposeful and unproblematically imitative and, in doing structures of meaning ’, what a contemporary anthro-
so, is able to acknowledge fully the socially constructed pological approach to children’s play suggests is that in
character of childhood (James & Prout, 1990). By this is those very games and activities, which are said to be
meant that whilst all children experience biological child- expressive of the ‘ culture of childhood ’, can be found
hood, in the sense that all children grow from infancy to some of the structures of social meaning that are mean-
adulthood, the precise form in which that ‘ growing up ’ ingful to children. That is to say, in children’s play can be
takes place will differ across and between societies. Such a discovered some of the signs and symbols through which
children come to understand the potential and possibilities socialisation process. Play and games may be one arena
of the social world. It is interpreting those meanings that within which this orientation can be seen to take place.
becomes, then, the analytic task.
Thorne, 1993), may not occur outside that context. This Patsy’s version was quickly accepted by Jess who, shy and
was revealed to me by 6-year-old Alice. In answer to the less articulate, saw herself as a novice in these affairs.
question ‘ Do you play with boys ? ’ Alice replied that she ‘ Patsy ’, Jess told me in hushed tones, ‘ knows them all ’.
played with Bobby at home and on the way to school. At Thus, although undoubtedly such changes in the telling
the school gate, however, this friendship was, each day, and retelling from one child to another accounts for the
temporarily suspended. Only on their return journey home variations and transformations in linguistic lore docu-
was it resumed. Similarly, ways of behaving and ways of mented by the Opies (Opie & Opie, 1977), within any
talking may abruptly change as children leave their home particular group of children there does exist an authorised
environment and join their contemporaries in the play- version that each child must get to know. Eight-year-old
ground. Cassie, also a little shy and reticent, described how this
arises. She told me that ‘ you just hear other people doing
Thus the school, in providing an important location for it ’ and then copy their version. Lorna, more confident,
the ‘ culture of childhood ’, can be said to furnish children disparagingly told me that ‘ Mary Smith kept making
with, in Sutton-Smith’s (1977) terms, potential ways of them up and kept spreading them ’. It was as if some
‘ being ’ or models of identity that they can try out and disease had run rife through the company of children.
refashion, adopt or abandon, augment or modify. Nina was the most explicit about how rhymes became
Although these may often turn out to represent future known and particular renderings accepted : ‘ Jackie’s sister
adult roles—as classical socialisation theory suggests—the taught Jackie, Jackie taught Polly, Polly taught me and I
way in which this process occurs is not through simple taught Susie and Susie taught Kim ’. All this takes time.
imitation. Children are not mirrors in which we can see Moreover, it is a mode of oral and visual transmission that
our younger selves. Instead, children’s progressive under- requires an active participation, necessitating endless
standing of their future as men and women in the social repetition and practice, careful listening, and observation
world is a complex, inventive, and innovative process. (see also Sluckin, 1981).
Children actively engaged with their future adult roles
through the intricacies of their day-to-day encounters.
They are not simply copied or taken on. Those children who are not allowed by others to play or
from whom knowledge about how to play is withheld are
But in order to play—whether it be informal games of thus effectively already taking on identities as losers in the
‘ Mummies and Daddies ’ or the rule-bound games of Tig social world. In this respect the ownership of games can be
and skipping games—a child must first learn how to play as critical. Some games gather their participants demo-
a child among other children at school. This means getting cratically. Two children put their arms round each other’s
to know and learning to use the implicit rules of behaviour shoulders and march through the playground calling out :
and thought embedded in the cultural practices of ‘ Who wants to play ? ’, followed by the name of the game.
childhood which, as I show below, may facilitate the Anyone may join such a line and, on some occasions, this
enactment of particular power relations. This is not to say activity transforms into a game itself : as the line gets
that all play relations are necessarily also power relations. longer so the chanting becomes louder and the faster it
Thus it is only among the 6- and 7-year-olds—those who wheels around the playground. At other times, however,
have been a year or so at school—that the traditional one child may decide on a game and initiate play ; for that
games of childhood play noted by the Opies (Opie & Opie, playtime ownership of the game befalls that child and she
1969, 1977) begin to flourish, for knowing how to play or he is able to choose the players. Being ‘ allowed ’ to play
takes both time and practice. In addition, it requires a signals identity as an insider through bestowing temporary
degree of social skill for, in effect, you can’t play until you membership of a particular social group upon an in-
know how to play and you only get to know how to play dividual child. Not being allowed to play, conversely,
by joining in. Surmounting these barriers is also difficult to confers outsider status as either a fleeting or more
achieve, as 7-year-old Jess and Patsy’s conversation permanent identity. A child may therefore use this power
illustrates. The two girls were telling me about the ‘ dips ’ to augment his or her own identity ; to curry favour
used by children to initiate a game and Jess had begun to with a more popular child or, indeed, to humiliate or
relate one of the rhymes they use : ostracise.
‘ You’re out of my house ’ she says to no one in particular as ways to initiate a game, to later refuse the ‘ rules of the
she brings plates and cups to a table. ‘ I haven’t no peas in my game ’ is not a skill to be admired. Jess complained bitterly
house ’. (To a boy standing watching) : ‘ Will you look after to me of what she regarded as one girl’s unfairness. This
my food ? … You’re daddy right ? Come on, hurry. You can girl always refused to be ‘ it ’, to be the chaser in games of
have milk shake and I’ve got some peas. I know where they chase. Jess interpreted her refusal not as a skill developed
are … lost them … in the pink jug. Where’s the milk jug over time, but as the explicit rejection of the very rules of
because I need it ? No. We don’t need it there. I gave it to dad play itself and as a sign of her potential outsiderness. ‘ It’s
and he was losing it. I’m going home ’. (To the boy again :) not fair ’, a phrase that commonly echoes round the
‘ You come to my house, dad, there’s your hat.’ (She gives playground, may not therefore constitute an appeal for
him a straw hat). ‘ Go away.’ (She pushes away another boy parity and equality—jostling for position in the hierarchy
who attempts to join in). On another day, hanging around of esteem is central to much of what children do. It may,
outside the Wendy House in the reception class and refused instead, be an objection to another’s flagrant disregard of
access by the girls for the third time, 5-year-old Saul the implicit rules of play itself.
reluctantly announced : ‘ I’ll go off to work again ’.
Huizinga, J. (1949). Homo ludens. London : Routledge & Kegan Rossie, J. (1993). Children’s play, generations and gender with
Paul. special reference to the Ghrib. Ethnographica, 9, 193–203.
James, A. (1993). Childhood identities : Self and social relationships Sack, A. L. (1977). Sport, play and work. In P. Stevens (Ed.),
in the experience of the child. Edinburgh : Edinburgh University Studies in the anthropology of play and culture. New York :
Press. Leisure Press.
James, A., Jenks, C., & Prout, A. (1998). Theorising childhood. Schwartzman, H. B. (1978). Transformations : The anthropology
Cambridge : Polity Press. of children’s play. New York : Plenum.
James, A., & Prout, A. (Eds.) (1990). Constructing and recon- Sluckin, A. (1981). Growing up in the playground. London :
structing childhood. Basingstoke : Falmer Press. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. Social Stainton-Rogers, R., & Stainton-Rogers, W. (1992). Stories of
Problems, 23, 478–487. childhood : Shifting agendas of child concern. London : Har-
Opie, I., & Opie, P. (1969). Children’s games in street and vester Wheatsheaf.
playground. Oxford : Oxford University Press. Sutton-Smith, B. (1977). Play as adaptive potentiation. In P.
Opie, I., & Opie, P. (1977). The lore and language of school Stevens (Ed.), Studies in the anthropology of play. New York :
children (First published 1959). Oxford : Oxford University Leisure Press.
Press. Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play : Girls and boys in school. New
Reynolds, P. (1989). Children in crossroads : Cognition and society Brunswick, NJ : Rutgers University Press.
in South Africa. Claremont, South Africa : David Phillip Wallman, S. (Ed.) (1979). The social anthropology of work.
Publisher. London : Academic Press.