Fanship and Wellbeing PSY499H - Jennifer - AlHarouny - Marianne - ElKhoury - Spring2022
Fanship and Wellbeing PSY499H - Jennifer - AlHarouny - Marianne - ElKhoury - Spring2022
Abstract
This study aimed to examine the relationship between loneliness, self-esteem, fanship, celebrity
worship and well-being. A survey was conducted on a sample of which the majority were Lebanese
youths. Statistical analyses yielded varying results: well-being did not correlate with fanship or
entertainment-social celebrity worship. On the other hand, self-esteem was found to negatively
Loneliness and self-esteem did not predict fanship but did predict celebrity worship. Moreover,
women reported higher levels of celebrity worship than men. Furthermore, fans of celebrities
reported higher levels of celebrity worship and fanship in comparison with other kinds of fans.
These results aim to paint a clearer picture of fanship free of bias while simultaneously
Fan culture has been prominent for several years and studies have uncovered many of its
aspects. Fans of sports clubs, celebrities, and fictional stories such as TV shows and books have
often been rendered “obsessed” and “delusional” by others (Gerrard, 2021). Thus, they have been
primarily regarded in a negative sense. Research has found different results and does not have a
definitive answer to whether being a fan of an interest is “bad” or “good” (Brooks, 2018). The
nature of fanship remains nuanced and dependent on many different factors such as self-esteem
and loneliness. The purpose of this study is to explore these aforementioned factors and their
relationship to each other as well as their role in predicting the likelihood of fanship. Furthermore,
this study attempts to uncover the relationship between fanship and well-being. The following
literature review presents the previous literature on the different factors used in this study.
Fanship usually involves a one-sided connection to the admired celebrity (Brooks, 2018);
however, many researchers identify a specific form of such a connection, also known as celebrity
worship (Sansone & Sansone, 2014). This term was developed in order to better understand
fanship and the different extents to which it can be classified. As such, McCutcheon et al. (2002)
developed the Celebrity Attitude Scale to better operationalize such dimensions. The scale consists
of three subscales, representing the three levels of celebrity worship: the first is manifested in the
form of the fan’s appreciation towards the entertainment the celebrity provides, the second is
related to the fan’s stronger preoccupation with the celebrity, and the third is characterized by a
delusional interconnection between the fan and the celebrity (McCutcheon et al., 2017). A fourth
component, “deleterious imitation”, refers to a fan’s thoughtless and immoral imitation of a certain
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 4
celebrity and has been suggested by several researchers for use (Brooks, 2018). However, current
studies are still focused on the original three factors. Given such developments, it is apparent that
celebrity worship influences different aspects of fans’ lives, some of which will be revisited in this
study.
Research has had contradicting results in relation to the effect of fanship on self-esteem. In
a systematic literature review, Brooks (2018) cited studies that found a positive correlation
between self-esteem and an intermediate level of celebrity worship. One of these studies
discovered this relation to persist with borderline-pathological celebrity worship as well. However,
other studies found the opposite, and some found no relation between self-esteem and fanship, but
they were a minority. O'Donovan (2016) found that participating in fandom activities tended to
lead to the development of the individual’s skills and the boosting of their confidence,
subsequently leading to a higher self-esteem. Additionally, in a study done on K-pop fans, high
levels of fanship were predictors of positive psychosocial outcomes, including self-esteem (Laffan,
2020). Notably, Zsila, Orosz, McCutcheon, and Demetrovics (2021) reported gender differences
in the relationship between fanship and self-esteem; women scored lower on self-esteem when
engaging in fanship, while men did not. Considering the links between self-esteem and anxiety
and depression (Henriksen, Ranøyen, Indredavik, & Stenseng, 2017), it is important to study this
relationship further.
Previous literature has also highlighted the found connection between levels of loneliness
and one’s fanship. This has been studied specifically in the context of interactions within a fan
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 5
domain, as seen in the following examples. Researchers such as Reysen et al. (2022) have found
that lonely individuals were more likely to seek out fandoms and identify with being a fan in order
to possibly establish some form of social connection. Additionally, however, studies have revealed
that social group involvement, including that of fandoms, has helped individuals feel less lonely
due to their involvement with others who are like-minded (Haslam et al., 2019). Some studies have
focused on a sample of sports fans, and results revealed that those who identified with the fandom
experienced lower levels of loneliness as well as higher levels of social satisfaction (Wann et al.,
2015). Other research, such as that executed by Reysen et al. (2017), confirms such findings for
both sports fans as well as non-sports fans, claiming that in-person interaction with other members
of the fandom increases one’s sense of belonging, thereby decreasing feelings of loneliness. While
such findings are significant, they mostly resulted from specific fandoms, thus warranting further
In their study “Better Together”, Reysen et al. (2022) claimed that there exist tentative
findings regarding the effects of fanship on well-being; research is still undecided on whether well-
being is influenced by one’s identity as a fan or by the new friendships that have been created due
to said identity. Past literature has, however, emphasized the mediating role that the social
component of fanship has on individuals’ general well-being. For instance, Ray et al. (2018)
administered a survey to anime fans both at an in-person convention and online in order to examine
the impact of social fan interaction. Consistent with other studies’ findings, Ray et al. found that
respondents who took part in the live convention reported more interactions with other fans and,
subsequently, higher levels of well-being than those who completed the online survey. Laffan
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 6
(2020) has also conducted a study on the effects of Korean Pop fanship and found that it predicts
significantly higher levels of happiness, self-esteem, and life satisfaction for fans. Generally,
fanship has proven to have positive effects on well-being, a revalidation of which will be conducted
in this study.
Many differences have been found in how men and women experience fanship, whether it
be a difference in the nature of what they are fans of, or the intensity of their feelings towards it.
A study on sport fans in Qatar found men to report higher levels of involvement in sport teams
than women, a finding they shared with many other previous studies (Theodorakis, Al-Emadi,
Wann, Lianopoulos, & Foudouki, 2017). They also found women to join fandoms as a way of
connecting with someone over the shared interest. On the other hand, women were more likely to
engage in parasocial activities, which would usually be involving a celebrity (Gleason, Theran, &
Newberg, 2017). Furthermore, Brooks (2018) stated that while some studies do not report a
difference in celebrity worship between genders, others have found women to exhibit it more.
women, in addition to being more likely to be accepting of stalking. On the other hand, women’s
relationship existed for men (Zsila, Orosz, McCutcheon, & Demetrovics, 2021).
There are different kinds of fandoms that exist in the world: sport fans, celebrity fans, TV
show fans, book fans… Previous studies have tended to focus on one type of fandom in their
samples, and they have rarely been compared. However, while their object of admiration is
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 7
different, they tend to have similar experiences. Reysen, Plante, and Chadborn (2017) agreed with
the latter statement, reporting similar psychological processes between different types of fans. A
study on sport fans in Qatar reported high levels of team identification in sport fans, leading to
feelings of belongingness to a group (Theodorakis, et al., 2017). Similarly, women fans of fiction
and celebrities reported making friends and a willingness to share and connect due to their fandom,
which has led to a feeling of belonging (O'Donovan, 2016). On the other hand, some phenomena
are particular to certain types of fans. Celebrity worship is a connection between fan and celebrity
that manifests only in fans of people, in contrast to interests such as books or sports. As previously
detailed, celebrity worship has shown to affect self-esteem, fantasy proneness, obsessive
The reviewed literature has many limitations that the present study aims to address. To
begin with, previous research tended to be correlational, failing to study possible predictors of
fanship. Additionally, fanship was operationalized differently across different studies. Another
drawback is the fact that none of these studies took place in Lebanon, and only one takes place in
the Middle East. With the rise of the economic crisis and the need for an escape, Lebanese citizens
are prone to turn to fan behavior and fandoms. As such, it is important to learn the implications of
This study aims to identify the psychological predictors of fanship and intends to explore
how this experience can possibly contribute to one’s well-being. Additionally, it aims to compare
these predictors, self-esteem and loneliness, between genders and different kinds of fans.
H1: Well-being will positively correlate with fanship and entertainment-social celebrity worship.
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 8
H2: Celebrity fans will score higher on celebrity worship than sports fans, music fans, and fiction
fans.
H3: Women will be more likely to score higher on levels of fanship (H3a) and celebrity worship
H4: Loneliness will predict fanship (H4a) and borderline-pathological celebrity worship (H4b).
H5: Self-esteem will predict fanship (H5a) and celebrity worship (H5b).
Methods
Participants
The sample consisted of 166 participants (109 women and 54 men) who were sent a link
to an online survey through social media, email, and text messages. The participants were both
Lebanese (82.5%) and non-Lebanese (17.5%) individuals between the ages of 18 and 61 years old
(M = 22.76, SD = 6.93), and most of them were college students. The sample consisted of 31 sport
fans, 38 celebrity fans, 47 fiction fans, 5 music fans, 2 game fans and 2 fans of political figures.
Instruments
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1979) is a 10-item self-report scale that was
development, the scale has been qualified for use with adult subjects. Each item was scored on a
4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (= strongly agree) to 4 (= strongly disagree). An example of
an item is “I feel that I have a number of good qualities.” Research has found this scale to have
adequate internal consistency and a Cronbach alpha of 0.92 (Rosenberg, 1979). In this study, the
The Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980) is a scale that was
designed to measure individuals’ personal feelings of loneliness and social isolation. It consists of
20 items which are rated according to a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (= Never) to 4 (=
Often). The items include statements such as “My interests and ideas are not shared by those
around me” and “There are people I feel close to”. This scale has proven to have high internal
consistency, with a Cronbach alpha of 0.96 (Russell et al., 1980). In this study, the Cronbach alpha
is 0.90.
The Celebrity Attitude Scale (CAS) (McCutcheon & Maltby, 2002) is a 23-item scale that
aims to evaluate individual inclinations towards celebrity worship. The items are measured
according to a 5-point Likert scale that ranges from 1 (= strongly disagree) to 5 (= strongly agree),
and form three subscales. The first subscale is labeled “Entertainment-social” and reflects the
social aspect of celebrity worship. An example of its items is “I love to talk with others who admire
my favorite celebrity”. The second subscale, “Intense-personal”, measures personal and intense
sentiments towards the celebrity and consists of items such as “I have frequent thoughts about my
celebrity, even when I don’t want to.” The third and final subscale is titled “Borderline-
pathological” as it demonstrates more radical attitudes an individual may have towards the
concerned celebrity. As such, items of this subscale include statements such as “If I were lucky
enough to meet my favorite celebrity, and he/she asked me to do something illegal as a favor I
would probably do it”. The reliability of this scale was also found to be sufficient, having a
Cronbach alpha that ranged between 0.84 and 0.96 across different studies (Aruguete et al., 2019;
Browne et al., 2019; McCutcheon et al., 2004). In this study, the Cronbach alpha of the scale is
The Fanship Scale (Reysen, & Branscombe, 2010) is a 9-point Likert scale designed to
measure the degree of individual identification with being a fan. A higher score would hence
indicate higher levels of fanship. The scale’s 11 items include statements such as “I am emotionally
connected to my interest” and “My interest is a part of me”. This instrument has also proven to be
reliable, holding a Cronbach alpha of 0.84 (Reysen, & Branscombe, 2010). In this study, the
The Display of Fanship scale (Chadborn et al., 2017) is a simple scale that complements
the Fanship Scale and measures the extent to which one displays themself as a fan. It consists of
only two items: “I purchase and/or wear/display items associated or identified with the fandom
[DVD, toys, clothing, etc.]” and “I share my experiences and/or fan interest with nonmembers of
the fandom [nonfans]”. The items are rated according to a 5-point Likert scale that ranges from 1
(= not at all) to 5 (= very frequently). In this study, the Cronbach alpha is 0.67.
The WHO-5 Well-being Index (Bech, 2004) is a short 5-item scale designed to measure
one’s well-being and quality of life. The items are measured according to a 6-point Likert scale
that ranges from 5 ( = All of the time) to 0 ( = At no time) and include statements such as “I have
felt cheerful in good spirits” and “My daily life has been filled with things that interest me”. The
scale has proven to be reliable, maintaining a Cronbach alpha of 0.85 (Mortazavi et al., 2015). In
Procedure
The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Lebanese
American University (LAU). Participants were sent an online link inviting them to participate
through email, social media outlets, and text messages. They chose to partake in the study
voluntarily, and no incentives were offered. First, the survey required participants to fill out
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 11
demographic information, and the scales were then administered in the following order: Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale, Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale. Participants were asked whether they were
fans, and if they were, what is their interest. Fans were then given the Celebrity Attitude Scale,
Fanship Scale, Display of Fanship, and WHO-5 Well-being Index. The survey took around 10 to
15 minutes to be completed.
Results
A bivariate correlation was run in order to study the intercorrelations among our variables,
which are presented in Table 1. First, self-esteem was found to negatively correlate with celebrity
worship (r = -0.244, p < 0.01), specifically with its borderline-pathological subscale (r = -0.285, p
< 0.01) and its entertainment-social subscale (r = -0.250, p < 0.01). Self-esteem was also found to
negatively correlate with loneliness (r = -0.562, p < 0.01). Moreover, results revealed a positive
correlation between loneliness and celebrity worship (r = 0.211, p < 0.1), as well as with two of
its subscales: entertainment-social (r = 0.209, p < 0.1) and borderline-pathological (r = 0.246, p <
0.01).
Fanship was also found to correlate with celebrity worship (r = 0.303, p < 0.01), whereby
significant positive correlations with its subscales were also revealed: entertainment-social (r =
0.248, p < 0.01), intense-personal (r = 0.329, p < 0.01), and borderline pathological (r = 0.298, p
< 0.01). Display of fanship, on the other hand, yielded stronger correlations with the same factors.
It was found to positively correlate with celebrity worship (r = 0.516, p < 0.01), showing further
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 12
positive correlations to the three subscales: entertainment-social (r = 0.500, p < 0.01), intense-
personal (r = 0.495, p < 0.01), and borderline pathological (r = 0.532, p < 0.01).
Finally, results revealed a significant negative correlation between well-being and self-
esteem (r = -0.431, p < 0.01), as well as a positive correlation between well-being and loneliness
In order to test gender differences in fanship, an independent samples t-test was performed,
and men and women did not significantly differ in their levels of self-esteem and loneliness.
However, women reported higher levels of celebrity worship (M = 2.6, SD = 1.01) than men (M
= 1.9, SD = 0.77), t = 3.78, p < 0.001. Consequently, women were also found to have higher scores
= 0.93), t = 3.77, p < 0.001. The intense-personal subscale also revealed a gender difference
whereby women (M = 2.27, SD = 1.06) scored higher than men (M = 1.67, SD = 0.79), t = 3.26, p
= 0.001. The final subscale, borderline-pathological, also revealed a higher score value for women
(M = 2.22, SD = 0.96) than men (M = 1.63, SD = 0.74), t = 3.58, p < 0.001. Another significant
difference was found in the well-being factor, whereby women (M = 3.61, SD = 1.02) reported
higher levels of general well-being in comparison to men (M = 3.22, SD = 1.26), t = 2.02, p < 0.5.
A regression analysis was carried out with fanship as the criterion (see Table 2). The model
was insignificant with loneliness and self-esteem as the predictors. Neither was a significant
predictor.
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 13
Another regression analysis was carried out with celebrity worship as the criterion (see
Table 3). With loneliness and self-esteem as the predictors, the model was significant (F = 4.60, p
< 0.05, Adj R2 = 0.05); however, neither was found to be a significant predictor.
In order to further study celebrity worship, three regression analyses were carried out with
its subscales (see Table 4, Table 5, and Table 6 respectively). The models of entertainment-social
with loneliness and self-esteem as the predictors (F = 4.76, p < 0.05, Adj R2 = 0.53) and that of
borderline-pathological (F = 6.46, p < 0.05, Adj R2 = 0.79) were found significant while that of
ANOVA analyses were conducted in order to test the differences across individuals with
different kinds of fan interest. The ANOVA indicated no significant differences in self-esteem,
loneliness, fanship or well-being. However, significant differences were found in celebrity worship
(F (6,121) = 10.49, p = .000). Follow-up Bonferroni post-hoc tests showed significant differences
in celebrity worship between celebrity fans and other kinds of fans, whereby they showed more
celebrity worship than sports fans (p = .000, 95% C.I. = [.76, 2.00]), fiction fans (p = .000, 95%
C.I. = [.43, 1.54]), fans of games (p < .05, 95% C.I. = [.11, 3.78]), and music fans (p = .001, 95%
= .000). Follow-up Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between celebrity
fans and sports fans (p = .000, 95% C.I. = [1.03, 2.36]), fiction fans (p = .000, 95% C.I. = [.60,
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 14
1.79]), fans of games (p < .01, 95% C.I. = [.35, 4.30]), and music fans (p = .000, 95% C.I. = [.76,
3.35]).
.000). Follow-up Bonferroni post-hoc tests found celebrity fans to score higher on intense-personal
celebrity worship than sports fans (p = .000, 95% C.I. = [.50, 1.86]), fiction fans (p = .001, 95%
C.I. = [.22, 1.45]), and music fans (p = .05, 95% C.I. = [.01, 2.67]).
types of fans (F (6,121) = 5.67, p = .000). Follow-up Bonferroni post-hoc tests found celebrity
fans to have higher borderline pathological celebrity worship than sports fans (p = .000, 95% C.I.
= [.41, 1.69]) and fiction fans (p = .001, 95% C.I. = [.23, 1.38]).
Lastly, significant differences were found in display of fanship (F (6,121) = 3.26, p < .01).
Follow-up Bonferroni post-hoc tests showed significant differences in display of fanship between
sports fans and celebrity fans, whereby celebrity fans exhibited more display of fanship (p = .01,
Discussion
This study explored possible predictors for fanship through self-esteem, loneliness and
celebrity worship, as well as the potential impact that fanship may have on a fan’s well-being.
While multiple significant correlations were yielded, not all of them were predicted and accounted
for within the hypotheses. As such, H1 was disproved as there was no significant correlation found
between well-being and fanship as well as between well-being and entertainment-social celebrity
worship. This is also inconsistent with the findings that Reysen et al. (2022) and Laffan (2020)
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 15
proposed in their studies, whereby they claim that fanship can accentuate one’s well-being and life
satisfaction.
Nevertheless, other aspects of such studies must be considered. Both Laffan (2020) and
Reysen et al. (2022) emphasize that it may not be the fanship itself, but rather the social
relationships that stem from it, that influence a fan’s well-being positively. Traces of such a
principle may be found within the results that this study’s hypotheses did not account for. For
instance, both self-esteem and loneliness were found to correlate with the entertainment-social
level of celebrity worship. This finding implies that social levels of celebrity worship may enhance
one’s self-esteem and decrease their feelings of loneliness. Another significant result is the found
correlations between well-being and self-esteem as well as between well-being and loneliness.
These correlations imply that higher self-esteem and lower levels of loneliness may contribute to
one’s sense of well-being. Taken together, these findings may prove the claims held by Reysen et
al. (2022) and Laffan (2020), whereby fanship may stimulate one’s social environment which, in
The ANOVA run to compare different types of fans yielded a few expected results. Fans
of celebrities did in fact score higher on celebrity worship than sports fans, music fans and fiction
fans, which is in line with our hypothesis (H2) as well as previous research (Brooks, 2018).
Specifically, celebrity fans exhibited more entertainment-social celebrity worship than sports fans,
fiction fans, fans of games, and music fans. They scored higher on intense-personal celebrity
worship than sport fans, fiction fans, and music fans and displayed more borderline-pathological
celebrity worship than sports fans and fiction fans. These results may be caused by the nature of
having a celebrity as one’s interest as it puts the focus of one’s energy on one person rather than a
concept, phenomenon, or a big group of people. Furthermore, the decreasing number of differences
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 16
between types as the level of celebrity increases is expected as people are more likely to be casual
Additionally, celebrity fans were found more likely to display their belonging to a fandom
than other fan types. This was unexpected but its weaker significance is in line with what is
expected, as both celebrity fans and sport fans experience belongingness to a group and use their
fandom to make friends. This would encourage display of fanship and would be expected to
manifest equally for both kinds of fans (O’Donovan, 2016; Theodorakis, Al-Emadi, Wann,
Lianopoulos, & Foudouki, 2017). This may be explained by considering that most celebrity fans
in this study were women, while most sport fans were men, and that men tend to wear less
accessories. Another notable speculation is that women tend to use fandom to connect with others
and make friends more than men (Theodorakis, et al., 2017) and therefore may have more of a
motivation to display their belonging to a fandom as a way of catching the attention of other fans
Further analysis of independent sample t-tests have yielded significant results in terms of
gender differences in fanship experiences and well-being. Women and men had similar reported
levels of fanship, which therefore disproves part of the study’s hypothesis (H3a). This was implied
in other studies, seeing as to how most gender differences in past literature focused on the type of
fandom each gender belongs to, the purpose behind their fanship, and other specific factors
(Gleason et al., 2017; Theodorakis et al., 2017). Another finding was in line with the current
research’s hypothesis (H3b), which speculated that women would have higher levels of celebrity
worship in comparison to men. A study by Brooks (2018) has also shared similar findings, with
an emphasis on the fact that men may score higher on borderline-pathological subscale. However,
the results of this study had opposite results, whereby women reported higher scores on said scale.
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 17
This may be attributed to the social belief that a woman fan is a crazed fan in love with an attractive
celebrity (Obel, 2012). Essentially, this stereotype may have led to a self-fulfilling prophecy. Such
As opposed to our hypothesis (H4) loneliness was found to predict neither fanship nor
celebrity worship and its subscales. This is not in line with the findings of Reysen et al. (2022)
which have found loneliness to encourage individuals to join fandoms. The present study’s result
may be caused by administering the loneliness survey after participants were already fans and their
loneliness has subsided due to fandom activities as reported by previous studies (Reysen et al.,
2022).
disproving our hypothesis (H5); however, it was a significant predictor of borderline pathological
celebrity worship. This result may be explained by adding onto previous research. It has been
found that fanship contributes to an increase in self-esteem (Brooks, 2018; Laffan, 2020). Perhaps
individuals with a lower self-esteem become overdependent on the object of their interest to give
being unable to raise one’s self-esteem despite the worship may lead to the intensifying of said
The model of loneliness and self-esteem did predict celebrity worship, which is possibly
due to the narrowing of the data. This would imply a certain profile for a fan with high celebrity
The obtained results may serve individuals a greater understanding of the underlying
factors behind fanship as well as the associations that accompany it. The study renders this
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 18
important as it contributes to clear misunderstandings on the nature of fans, disprove the negative
social image attributed to them, and decrease the prejudice that surrounds them in social contexts.
Nevertheless, it is also important to highlight the importance of being aware of the signs of
pathology that some fans may possess, as well as the possible causes behind the pathological
behavior. This study thus hopes to provide sufficient information on such matters.
Although this study yields substantial results, several limitations must still be considered.
The recruited sample consists of roughly over 160 participants and is composed of mostly
Lebanese youth. The study had aimed to explore differences in fanship across different cultures
and nationalities but was unable to do so due to the scarcity of non-Lebanese individuals who
participated in the study. Furthermore, the sample presented an unequal distribution of types of
fans. Such factors, alongside the fact that it has been recruited conveniently rather than randomly,
make the sample less representative and the results less generalizable. Another limitation is the
online administration of the survey, seeing as to how it allows multiple submissions which may
therefore skew results. Additionally, reliability testing for the used scales revealed that the 2-item
Display of Fanship scale (Chadborn et al., 2017) had a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.66, which may
Given such limitations, future studies are advised to conduct their research having recruited
a larger, more random sample. Including individuals of different ages and backgrounds may assist
in varying the obtained results and better generalizing them. Moreover, researchers are
recommended to use scales with higher Cronbach’s Alphas in order to better ensure the validity of
the obtained results. It is also advised to use scales that have been validated in Lebanon. Ideally,
the mode of administration of the survey would steer away from one that is online and would entail
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 19
experimental methods. However, if online administration were to be applied, researchers are urged
to control for the possibility of multiple submissions, thus making only one submission per
individual possible and diminishing the probable skewness of results. Most importantly, it is
advised that future research include mediating variables such as social relationships, more
predictors that would make it possible to describe the profile of a fan, more importantly one with
pathological celebrity worship who may put themselves and others at risk. Finally, to ensure the
best results, longitudinal studies are recommended in order to get the most reliable results possible.
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 20
References
Bech P. (2004). Measuring the dimensions of psychological general well-being by the WHO-5.
Brooks, S. K. (2018). FANatics: Systematic literature review of factors associated with celebrity
worship, and suggested directions for future research. Current Psychology, 40(2), 864–
886. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9978-4
Chadborn, D., Edwards, P., & Reysen, S. (2017). Displaying Fan Identity to Make Friends.
Gerrard, Y. (2021). Groupies, Fangirls and Shippers: The Endurance of a Gender Stereotype.
https://doi.org/10.1177/00027642211042284
Gleason, T. R., Theran, S. A., & Newberg, E. M. (2017). Parasocial Interactions and
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00255
Haslam, C., Cruwys, T., Chang, M. X., Bentley, S. V., Haslam, S. A., Dingle, G. A., & Jetten, J.
(2019). GROUPS 4 HEALTH reduces loneliness and social anxiety in adults with
Henriksen, I. O., Ranøyen, I., Indredavik, M. S., & Stenseng, F. (2017). The role of self-esteem
sample of adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 11(1).
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-017-0207-y
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 21
Hitlan, R. T., McCutcheon, L. E., Volungis, A. M., Joshi, A., Clark, C. B., & Pena, M. (2021).
Social Desirability and the Celebrity Attitude Scale. North American Journal of
Laffan, D. A. (2020). Positive Psychosocial Outcomes and Fanship in K-Pop Fans: A Social
https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294120961524
McCutcheon, L. & Maltby, J. (2002). Personality Attributions About Individuals High and Low
Mortazavi, F., Mousavi, S. A., Chaman, R., & Khosravi, A. (2015). [Validation of the World
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25742037/#:%7E:text=Results%3A%20Cronbach’s%20
alpha%20coefficient%20for,63.1%25%20of%20the%20total%20variance.
Obel, C. (2012). Fantasy, Fun and Identity Construction Among Female Fans of Rugby Union.
In Sport and Its Female Fans (1st ed., pp. 115–134). Routledge.
O'Donovan, R. (2016). ‘To boldly go where no psychologist has gone before”: effects of
Reysen, S. & Branscombe, N. (n. d.). Fanship and Fandom: Comparisons Between Sport and
Reysen, S., Plante, C. N., Roberts, S. E., & Gerbasi, K. C. (2022). Social Activities Mediate the
Reysen, S., Plante, C., & Chadborn, D. (2017). Better Together: Social Connections Mediate the
Relationship Between Fandom and Well-Being. AASCIT Journal of Health, 4(6), 68–73.
Reysen, S., Plante, C., & Chadborn, D. (2022). Perceived Permeability of Group Boundaries as a
Russell, D., Peplau, L.A., & Cutrona, C.E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale:
Sansone, R. A., & Sansone, L. A. (2014). “I’m Your Number One Fan”— A Clinical Look at
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3960781/
Theodorakis, N., Al-Emadi, A., Wann, D., Lianopoulos, Y. & Foudouki, A. (2017). An
Wann, D. L., Brasher, M., Thomas, D. L., & Scheuchner, H. L. (2015). Generalizing the team
Zsila, G., Orosz, G., McCutcheon, L. E., & Demetrovics, Z. (2021). Individual Differences in the
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651067
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 23
Appendix
Table 1
Table 2
Table 3
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 24
Table 4
Regression of entertainment-social celebrity worship on loneliness and self-esteem
Table 5
Regression of intense-personal celebrity worship on loneliness and self-esteem
Table 6
Regression of borderline-pathological celebrity worship on loneliness and self-esteem
FANSHIP AND WELL-BEING 25